The Effects of Recognition and Group Need on Volunteerism
Jurnal:
The Effects of Recognition and
Group Need
on Volunteerism- A
Social Norm Perspective
ROBERT J. FISHER
DAVID ACKERMAN
Fisher, R. J., & Ackerman, D. (1998). The effects of
recognition and group need on volunteerism: A social norm perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(3), 262-275.
Ini hanya sebuah catatan untuk pribadi,
silahkan merujuk sumeber aslinya.
Note only, please reference
to source
The significance of volunteering for both individuals and society has lead
to numerous
studies on this behavior across the social sciences. However, virtually
no prior research has evaluated how and to what extent organizations
can effectively encourage individuals to contribute time to a worthy cause. The
present research uses a social norm perspective to examine the conditions under
which promotional
appeals based on group need and promises of recognition affect
volunteerism. The perspective suggests that norm
compliance can be expected only when the prescribed behavior is both important to
the group’s welfare and subject to group-mediated rewards. Consequently, we hypothesize that promotional appeals based
on group need and promised recognition are effective only
when they are used in combination. Results of a
laboratory and a field experiment are consistent with this hypothesis and provide insights
into the process by which the appeals affect individuals’ decisions to help. The
results also have implications for understanding and promoting other socially desirable
behaviors such as recycling, energy conservation, litter reduction, and the purchase of ‘‘green’’ products.
===
The decision to
volunteer is an important, life-altering commitment for many individuals.
Of the estimated 94 million American adults engaged in some form of volunteerism, 25 million donate five or more hours per week (Independent
Sector 1992). Given that the average American adult spends approximately 17
hours per week in leisure (Samuelson 1989),
volunteering often represents a
significant proportion of total nonworking time Moreover, volunteering affects
expenditures of one of the scarcest and therefore most precious commodities: discretionary
time (Hawes 1977; Holbrook and Lehmann 1981). Volunteering means fewer hours on
the golf course, at the movies, or in the pursuit of other
recreational or consumption activities. It also means less time devoted to
wealth creation either in the form of career advancement or part-time sources
of income.
From a more macro
perspective, volunteer work can be viewed as a nonmarket
activity that provides services that otherwise would not be available in a
community (Schram and Dunsing 1981). Volunteers make possible programs for
children such as little league and scouts,
extracurricular activities at public
schools, and act as a substitute for government-support
programs for the disadvantaged. Without volunteer participation the services that
are offered by many profit and nonprofit organizations would be unavailable or
provided at higher cost to consumers and taxpayers. Consider that volunteers
are an inexpensive or free labor source for some 40, 000 organizations in the United States (Kotler 1982) and generate
services worth more than $150 billion annually (Independent Sector 1992).
The importance of volunteerism to both individuals
and society has lead to numerous studies on volunteer motivations across the
social sciences. Research can be found in social psychology (e.g., Harrison 1995; Omoto and Snyder 1995); sociology
(e.g., Babchuk and Gordon 1962); organizational behavior
(e.g., Pearce 1993); leisure research (e.g., Henderson 1981); social issues (e.g., Jenner 1981); voluntary organizations (e.g., Bonjean,
Markham, and Macken 1994; Gallup
Organization 1987); and economic psychology (e.g., Unger 1991).1
Despite the wide
breadth of studies on the topic, prior research sheds little light on how organizations
can encourage volunteerism because studies in the area typically use cross sectional
designs and sample only those who volunteered.
The reliance on
self-report methods also makes the data vulnerable to social desirability bias (Fisher
1993). Given that helping others for personal gain is contrary to social norms, self-reports are likely to overestimate
altruistic reasons for volunteering and underreport instrumental ones such as
personal development, career advancement, and social approval (Smith
1981).
Research in consumer
behavior and marketing provides limited guidance on volunteerism, despite a variety of studies on other types of
giving. Prior research has examined the effects of attitudes on blood and
body-part donations (Bagozzi 1981, 1982; Burnett 1981; Burnkrant and Page 1982;
LaTour and Manrai 1989; Pessemier, Bemmaor,
and Hanssens 1977), the effectiveness of fund raising promotional
strategies (e.g., Bagozzi and Moore 1994; Meyers-Levy and
Maheswaran 1992), and compliance techniques such as
a-foot-in-the-door and even-a-penny-will-help (e.g., Fraser, Hite, and Sauer 1988; Moore, Bearden,
and Teel 1985; Reingen 1978). A
recent literature review by Bendapudi, Singh, and Bendapudi (1996) reveals only one study in
marketing and consumer behavior that examines volunteerism, and this study focuses
primarily on financial contributions (i.e.,
Dawson 1988).
Accordingly, there is limited knowledge of how and to what
extent organizations can promote volunteerism. The present research uses norm
theory to investigate this substantive issue because volunteering is a socially
desirable, and therefore normative, behavior. Norm theory
asserts that norms are most effective in regulating behaviors that are both important to the group’s welfare
and backed by socially mediated rewards or punishments (Schwartz 1977). We
therefore hypothesize that volunteerism is increased only when promotional
appeals emphasize both the group’s need and promise recognition to those who help.
Unlike prior survey-based studies, we use a between-subjects experimental design
that improves our ability to make causal inferences and reduces the potential for
social desirability bias.
We begin the article
with a discussion of the literature that was used as a framework for the
research. Next, we present the designs and results of two
studies. The first study is a laboratory experiment in which students are asked
to volunteer for a school fund-raising team. The second study is a field
experiment based on a sample of parents who are registering their children for
a youth sports league. Finally, we discuss the implications of the research for understanding and promoting
volunteerism and other socially desirable behaviors.
A
NORMATIVE PERSPECTIVE ON VOLUNTEERING
One way that groups regulate
the conduct of their members is through the establishment and reinforcement of norms.
We define norms as perceptions, attitudes,
and behaviors that are approved of
by the group and expected of its members (cf. Baron, Kerr, and Miller 1992). Norms specify
what is socially sanctioned, that is,
what group members should or ought to do in a particular
situation (Cialdini, Kallgren,
and Reno 1991). Norms are not usually
written down or discussed openly, but they have a strong influence on a wide
range of behaviors (Feldman 1984; Leary 1995).
Norms are reinforced
within groups through socially mediated rewards and punishments (DeRidder, Schruijer,
and Tripathi 1992). Individuals who
comply with norms can expect to create a good impression or receive praise for
their actions, whereas those who do not can anticipate negative
verbal or visual expressions of disappointment, or even outright rejection
(Festinger, Schachter,
and Back 1950). Expectations of
rewards (and the avoidance of sanctions) have been found to motivate a variety
of socially desirable behaviors including helping those in need (Bandura 1977;
Baumann et al. 1981; Berkowitz and Daniels 1964; Eisenberg 1982), reciprocity (Gouldner 1960), and pro-environmental actions (Reno, Cialdini,
and Kallgren 1993).
Norms vary greatly in
importance and therefore so does the extent to which behavioral compliance is
required by the group (DeRidder et al. 1992). Whereas weak norms allow for wide
latitude of behaviors, strong norms demand more precise responses. In
general, the most important norms regulate behaviors
that affect the group’s survival and prosperity. As a consequence, the more important a behavior is to the group, the stronger the expectation that group
members will undertake it. We examine the relationship between the importance
of helping and the willingness to volunteer in the next section.
Group Need and the
Importance of Helping Anecdotal evidence abounds of attempts by charitable and
other nonprofit organizations to motivate volunteering and other helping
behaviors by emphasizing the significance of their need. For example, a 1996 letter requesting financial
contributions for a local PBS station refers to cuts in federal funding and
states, ‘‘We just can’t make it without Member
support.’’ The appeal highlights the dire situation facing the organization and
the significance of the member’s help. Also,
the level of need can be represented
by the success of the organization in achieving its goals. The United Way, for example,
establishes specific financial
targets for each community and corporate campaign. The degree to which the
organization’s fund-raising objective is attained is a direct measure of the
campaign’s success and hence the need for further financial and volunteer
support.
Because group members
are expected to assist their group in achieving its collective purpose (Tajfel
1970), groups invoke this norm when they promote the
significance of their need. When group need is high, group members are expected
to volunteer because they have the opportunity to make an important
contribution. Without their support the group would be unable to accomplish its
charitable goals. Volunteering is less critical when the group is
self-sufficient, and so the expectation that group members will
volunteer should be weak.
Nevertheless, norms are unlikely to guide even the most
important behaviors unless social consequences are expected for compliance or
deviance (e.g., Reno et al. 1993). Although group members
should feel an obligation to help when group need is high, a social norm perspective
suggests that they will act only if the behavior is monitored and rewarded (Schwartz
1977). Recognition provides one mechanism for elevating the visibility of desired
behaviors and creating favorable social consequences for the recipient. We
discuss these effects in a volunteering context in the next section.
The
Promise of Recognition
Recognition is a
strategy that is frequently used by charitable and nonprofit organizations. For
example, the American Red Cross, American Legion, YMCA, and United Way use dinners and special events
to recognize the contributions of those who help (American Red Cross 1988;
Brakeley 1980; Leibert and Sheldon 1972; McGee 1988; Unger 1991). Formal
recognition ceremonies and other noneconomic rewards provide an important basis
for volunteer recruitment and motivation because, by definition, volunteers are not compensated monetarily for their
efforts and are free to withdraw their support from the organization at any
time.
We define recognition as a public expression of
appreciation given by a group to individuals who undertake desired behaviors.
Although recognition can include elements that have economic value such as cash
awards or prizes, these aspects are neither necessary nor
sufficient.
Indeed, recognition symbols such as plaques, certificates, and trophies have no commercial
value once they have been personalized to include the recipient’s name, organization, and charity event (unless, perhaps,
the recipient is famous). Who would
want to purchase a plaque that honors someone else’s volunteering contribution
to Meals on Wheels? At the same time, personalizing a recognition object increases
its symbolic value to the recipient because it incorporates meanings that were
absent in the original commodity (e.g., an ‘‘off-the-shelf ’’ trophy or plaque).
Recognition is therefore distinct from commissions, bonuses, or incentives that are effective because of their
monetary value.
Recognition differs
from informal and private forms of appreciation such as a manager
congratulating a volunteer on a ‘‘job well done’’ in a hallway conversation. In
this situation the only two individuals who have direct knowledge of the
interaction are the manager and worker, and so the rewards created by the act are
primarily personal (e.g., feelings of satisfaction and competence).2
In contrast, recognition publicly communicates the group’s
respect for those who support the group, and thereby raises the recipient’s status and
prestige (cf. Belk 1995; Turner 1988). For example, a volunteer who works
behind the scenes in a clerical or secretarial capacity might be relatively
anonymous until recognition makes his or her contribution visible. The
individual’s fellow group members or other referents then have the opportunity
to express their appreciation for and admiration of the volunteer.
Despite the apparent power of recognition to
generate favorable social consequences for the recipient, its impact cannot be
determined independently of the rewarded behavior. When recognition is promised
under low group-need conditions, volunteers anticipate a public display of appreciation
for conduct that is not strongly prescribed by group norms. The social
consequences of volunteering should not be enhanced because the value of
recognition depends on the importance of the rewarded behavior. We therefore
hypothesize that volunteerism is not increased when recognition is promised
under low group-need conditions. In contrast,
we expect that volunteerism is
elevated when the behavior is important and recognition is promised. In this
situation, group members believe that their help is
critical to the group and that others will be made aware of their contribution.
The social consequences are significant because the behavior is both highly
desirable and visible.
…
===
…
EXPERIMENT
2
Experiment 2 was
motivated by the desire to replicate the results of the first experiment in a
field setting to evaluate the commitment of volunteers to follow through on
their pledges. A second objective was to employ a more typical volunteering
context than the student fundraising competition used in experiment 1. Experiment
2 was designed to be a noncompetitive context and one in which the group and
its members were less formally inked. One consequence of this setting was that
group need could be manipulated directly rather than indirectly through group
success. Finally, a second experiment would allow us to include
additional process and confound checks related to the effects of the
recognition manipulation on the perceived visibility of volunteering, the distinctiveness of the recognition offered, and subjects’ personal (i.e., nonsocial) feelings about helping. We discuss
the rationale for each of these extensions in the next section.
Method
Experiment 2 employed a
2 (low/high group need)12 no/promised
recognition) design. Subjects were parents registering their children (four to
17 years of age) for the upcoming season of a youth soccer league. Researchers were
given permission by league officials to run the experiment as part of their
actual registration process with here provisions. First, the information given to
subjects had to be truthful and so any promises of recognition would have to be
fulfilled. Second, the manipulations could not contain
information that was detrimental to the organization’s image. Third, researchers were asked to make the
experimental package as brief as possible to ensure that the registration
process was not disrupted.
As subjects began the
registration process they were given the research instrument and a flyer asking
for volunteers to help at a yearly event called ‘‘Soccer Festival.’’
The manipulations of
group need and recognition were contained in the flyer. Group need was
manipulated with information about the level of volunteer help received at the
prior year’s event. Subjects in the low need condition were given information
that ‘‘we had plenty of volunteers at last year’s Soccer Festival, but we want your help too!’’ The high need
condition flyers indicated that ‘‘we were very short of volunteers at last
year’s Soccer Festival— we desperately need your help!’’
Recognition was
manipulated with information about the extent to which volunteers could expect
a public expression of gratitude from the organization. Subjects in he promised
recognition condition were informed that they would receive an official ‘‘I’m a
Volunteer’’ T-shirt.
The
T-shirt would be worn by volunteers at the Soccer
Festival because ‘‘we
want everyone to know who our volunteers are and to recognize their good
work!’’3
Subjects in the
no-recognition condition did not receive any information about a formal
acknowledgement by the league for their participation. Regardless of
recognition condition, all volunteers who helped at Soccer Festival were
given a free T-shirt by the league (donated by the researchers).
Subjects were randomly
assigned to each of the four conditions (23–25 per cell) as they arrived at the
registration location. Of 150 subjects handed an instrument and asked to ‘‘fill
out a league survey on volunteering, ’’
97 indicated that they had read the flyer and completed all questions for a 65
percent response rate. The response rate did not differ across the four cells
based on a chi square test (p>.20).
Subjects tended to be female (64 percent) with an average age of 34 and with
two or three children living at home.
Measurement
x̄ = X̄
Manipulation
Checks. The manipulation check for group need was the sum
of ‘‘your help is important to this year’s Soccer Festival’’ and ‘‘you are
needed as a volunteer at this year’s Soccer Festival’’ (X̄ = 11.22, r = .73, σ = 2.90). The manipulation check for recognition was the sum of
‘‘the organization properly thanks Soccer Festival Volunteers’’ and ‘‘the
organization shows its gratitude to Soccer Festival Volunteers’’ (X̄ = 10.62, r = .95, σ = 3.10). The measures
were based on seven-point ‘‘strongly disagree’’ to ‘‘strongly agree’’ scales.
Perceived
Social Visibility. We wanted to examine whether the
recognition manipulation lead to heightened perceptions of social visibility
for volunteers. Our measure of social visibility used the question stem, ‘‘the help of volunteers at this year’s Soccer
Festival will be... ’’ and asked subjects to respond on a seven-point semantic differential
scale anchored with ‘‘not noticed’’ and ‘‘noticed’’ (X̄ = 5.94, σ = 1.27).
Estimated
Proportion of Volunteers. Given that recognition is valued
more when it is distinctive, the pattern of results from the first
experiment could be explained by the tendency of subjects in the low
success/promised recognition cell to predict that relatively few people would
volunteer to help. If this were the case,
a confound would exist because it is
unclear whether differences in the number of hours donated resulted from
recognition’s distinctiveness or its capacity to visibly reward an important
behavior. We therefore asked subjects to predict the number of prospective
volunteers who would help at Soccer Festival. The question was, ‘‘Out of 10 people asked to volunteer, approximately how many do you think actually
will?’’ Subjects then circled a number between zero and 10, anchored with ‘‘no one who
is asked will volunteer’’ and ‘‘everyone who is asked will volunteer, ’’ respectively (X̄ = 4.43, σ = 2.15).
Personal
Feelings. A measure of subjects’ personal feelings about
volunteering was included to capture the personal (i.e., nonsocial) effects of the
manipulations. We anticipated that subjects would have more positive feelings
when group need was high because it is in this condition that their
contribution is most important to the group. We also wanted to evaluate the
potential for recognition to have been effective in the high group-need
condition for personal reasons. In particular, recognition might be
valued as much as an expression of the group’s appreciation as for its social
effects. If this were the case, communicating the group’s appreciation for
helping in private might be as effective as a public award. Evidence of the
nonsocial value of recognition would exist if we found that the group need by
promised recognition inter action hypothesized for social approval also occurred
for subjects’ personal feelings about volunteering. We examined this potential
by asking subjects how they would eel about volunteering at the Soccer Festival
on a one o seven, ‘‘very bad’’ to ‘‘very good’’ scale (X̄ = 5.46, σ = 1.49).
Expectations
of Social Approval. We used a two-item measure of social
approval based on the anticipated reaction of other parents at the Soccer
Festival and other volunteers. The question stem was, ‘‘Would people in each of
these groups appreciate it if you volunteered to help on Soccer Festival Day?’’
A seven-point ‘‘very unlikely’’ to ‘‘very likely’’ scale was used (X̄ = 11.23, r = .72, σ = 2.49).
Number
of Hours Donated. The measure of the number of hours
donated was, ‘‘How many hours would you be willing to
volunteer for Soccer Festival, if any?’’ Subjects were asked to circle a
number between zero and 11 or more hours (X̄
= 2.43, σ = 1.78).
Volunteer
Commitment. A potential weakness of any study based on
self-reports is that subjects might not act n a way that is consistent with their
stated intentions. The lack of correspondence between predicted and actual behaviors
might be particularly acute for volunteering because participation is costly in
terms of time and effort. Therefore, we asked subjects to include their name and phone
number at the end of the questionnaire if they had indicated a willingness to
help. Volunteers who included his information on the questionnaire could expect
to be called by the organization to fulfill their pledge. This is he actual
approach used by the soccer league to identify and ultimately contact
volunteers.
Results
Manipulation
Checks. An ANOVA revealed that perceptions of group need
and promised recognition were manipulated as intended. The need manipulation
had a significant effect on perceived need (F(1, 96) = 12.41, p < .01,
X̄ high need
= 12.14>X̄ low need = 10.16,
η2 = .34),
and the recognition manipulation had
a significant effect on perceived recognition (F(1, 96) = 20.41, p < .001, X̄ p recog = 11.88>X̄ no recog = 9.33,
η2 = .42). No other significant main or
interaction effects were found.
Perceived
Social Visibility. As expected, we found a significant
main effect of the recognition manipulation on subjects’ perceptions of the
visibility of volunteering (F(1, 96) = 8.68,
p < .01). Subjects in the promised recognition condition perceived that
volunteering would be more visible than subjects in the no-recognition condition
(X̄
p
recog = 6.28 >X̄ no recog = 5.56,
η2 = .29).
Table 2
INTERACTION EFFECTS OF PROMISED RECOGNITION AND
GROUP NEED ON SOCIAL APPROVAL AND VOLUNTEER PARTICIPATION: EXPERIMENT 2
|
||||
|
High group need
|
Low group need
|
||
|
No recognition
|
Promised
recognition
|
No recognition
|
Promised
recognition
|
Social approval
|
10.58
(2.64)
|
12.48 (1.88)
|
11.00 (2.80)
|
10.76 (2.47)
|
Number of hours donated
|
2.33
(1.93)
|
3.56 (1.48)
|
1.96 (1.85)
|
1.76 (1.36)
|
Proportion
of subjects volunteering at least one hour (%)
|
71
|
96
|
70
|
72
|
n
|
24
|
25
|
|
|
Estimated
Proportion of Volunteers. We found no significant main or
interaction effects of the manipulations on the proportion of people expected
to volunteer (p >.10).
Consequently, the results cannot be explained by variations
in the perceived distinctiveness of the recognition promised by the
organization.
Personal
Feelings. There was a significant main effect of group need
on subjects’ personal feelings about volunteering with more positive feelings
when group need was high (F(1, 96) = 6.40, p < .05, X̄
high
need = 5.80 >X̄ low need = 5.06,
η2 = .22). Promised recognition and group
need did not interact to affect subjects’ personal feelings about volunteering
(p>.20). These results suggest
that the effect of the manipulations on the number of hours donated was not the
result of differences in subjects’ personal feelings about volunteering.
Expectations
of Social Approval. We found a significant interaction of
promised recognition and group need on social approval (F(1, 96) = 4.76, p < .05) with the pattern of means consistent with the
hypothesis. A simple effects test reveals that the mean social approval level was
significantly higher when recognition was promised under high group need (F(1, 48) = 9.32,
p < .01, X̄ p recog = 12.48>X̄ no recog
= 10.58, η2 = .41)
but not under low group need (F(1, 47) < 1,
p>.70, X̄ p recog
= 10.76, X̄ no recog = 11.00,
η2
= .08).
As expected, there was no significant difference between
the high and low need means in the no-recognition condition (F(1, 46)›1, p
>.60; X̄
high need = 10.58,
X̄ low need = 11.00,
η2
= .08).
The effects of group need and recognition on social approval are consistent
with the hypothesis. No main effect of either manipulation was found on
expectations of social approval.
Number
of Hours Donated. The subject population had the
potential to volunteer for several tasks in addition to the Soccer Festival. We
therefore included the number of other tasks the subject volunteered for as a
covariate to the number of hours donated at Soccer Festival. We found a
significant interaction between promised recognition and group need (F(1, 96) = 5.55,
p < .025) with the number of other positions volunteered for by the subject
a significant covariate (F(1, 96) = 11.21,
p < .01). Simple effects tests reveal a significant increase in the
self-reported number of hours donated when recognition is provided in the high
need condition (F(1, 48) = 6.30,
p ›.025; X̄
p recog = 3.56>X̄ no recog = 2.33,
η2 = .34) and no significant difference in
the low need condition (F(1, 47)›1, p>.50; X̄
p recog = 1.76,
X̄ no recog = 1.96,
η2 = .09). As expected, there was no significant
difference between the high and low group need means when recognition was not
offered (F(1, 46) ›1, p >.60;
X̄
high need = 2.33,
X̄ low need = 1.96,
η2 = .08). Consistent with experiment 1, we found that the number of hours volunteered
was significantly different from zero only in the high group-need/promised recognition
cell (p < .05).
We examined the overall
proportion of subjects who volunteered at least one hour of time and found that
77 percent of subjects stated a willingness to participate. A chi-square test
reveals a significant difference in the pro portion of subjects who volunteered
across the four cells (x 2 = 8.58,
df = 3, p < .05) with 96
percent of subjects volunteering in the high group-need/promised recognition
cell. The results are consistent with experiment 1 and illustrate how the
interaction of the manipulations affected the willingness of prospective
volunteers to participate. The results related to the interaction effects are
summarized in Table 2 and Figure 2.
Mediational
Tests. As in experiment 1 we used path analysis to
examine the mediational hypothesis. Using the same series of equations outlined
in the first study we found results that are consistent with a mediational role
of social approval. We found (1) a significant effect of the interaction term
on social approval (β = .38, t
= 2.18, p < .05),
(2) a significant effect of the
interaction term on the number of hours donated (β = .33, t = 1.97,
p < .05), and (3) when both social
approval and the interaction term are used to predict the number of hours donated, the effect of social approval is significant at
p < .10 (β = .15, t = 1.48). 4 Further, we combined across the two experiments to summarize
the results (see Rosenthal and Rubin 1986). We found a highly significant effect
of social approval on the number of hours donated given the effects of
recognition and group need (Z = 4.67, p < .01). The strength and
consistency of the result across experiments provides further evidence of the
mediating role of social approval.
Ceiling
Effect Test. The mean number of hours donated in
the low need condition was below the middle of the range of zero to 11 or more
hours donated and the mean in the high need condition was larger than the mean in the low need condition (X̄
high need = 2.96>X̄ low need = 1.90). Consequently, the lack of a recognition
effect in the low need condition cannot be attributed to a ceiling effect (i.e., that subjects were already donating the
maximum time they are willing or able to contribute in the low need condition).
Volunteer
Commitment. Seventy-three percent of those who volunteer at
least one hour to the Soccer Festival included their name and phone number on
the questionnaire. Commitment was strongest in the high group need/promised
recognition cell with 80 percent including the information needed to contact
them, although there was no significant difference
across cells (p >.30). These
results provide some confidence that the self-report data we collected would
translate into actual differences in volunteering behaviors.
Experiment
2 Discussion
The purpose of
experiment 2 was to replicate the results of the first study in a field setting
and to provide further insights into the social norm hypothesis. In terms of
the replication objective, both experiments found that promised
recognition increased expectations of social approval and the number of hours
donated only when group need was high. Both experiments also found support for
the role of social approval as a mediator between the interaction term and the
number of hours volunteered. Finally, neither experiment found a significant
difference for either social approval or the number of hours donated between (1)
the no- and promised-recognition cells when group need was low, or (2) the high and low group-need cells when
recognition was not promised.
Experiment 2 also
addressed the potential confound that promised recognition affected the number
of hours donated because of variations in its perceived distinctiveness rather
than its greater value when group need is high. The results revealed that the
manipulations had no significant effect on subjects’ predictions about the pro portion
of others who would help. We found no evidence that recognition’s perceived
distinctiveness varied across conditions.
Experiment 2 provided
further insights into how recognition affects the willingness to volunteer. As
expected, we found that the promise of recognition
elevated subjects’ predictions about the visibility of volunteering. We also
found that subjects felt more positive about volunteering when group need was
high, but this did not trans late into a main effect
of group need on the number of hours donated. Finally, there was no interaction
effect of group need and recognition on personal feelings about volunteering.
These results, coupled with the interaction of the
manipulations with respect to both social approval and number of hours donated;
indicate that recognition was more effective under high group-need conditions
for social rather than personal reasons. Moreover, and consistent with
experiment 1, it appears that recognition did not have
intrinsic value to subjects. It was only when subjects thought that their
contribution was important and therefore more socially desirable that
recognition increased the number of hours donated.
Experiment 2 included a
measure of volunteer commitment to examine the effects of the manipulations on
actual behaviors. The vast majority of subjects who volunteered were willing to
provide their name and telephone number so that the organization could contact
them later about their offer to help. No significant difference was found across
cells in the willingness of subjects to provide this information. Written
commitments such as the one used in experiment 2 have been found to be highly
correlated with behaviors across a variety of settings (for a review of this
evidence see Cialdini [1993]).
An examination of cell
means revealed that only the number of hours donated in the high need/promised
recognition cell is significantly different from zero. By implication, a statistically meaningful level of
volunteering occurred only when the behavior was important to the group and a
group-mediated reward was expected for helping. The results do not, however,
suggest that subjects only
volunteered for selfish reasons such as increased social approval. The majority
of subjects in both experiments volunteered to help even when group need was low
and no recognition was promised. In other words, most subjects responded to
a request for help without any inducement whatsoever.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Beyond the robust nature of the results across experimental settings and
samples, several differences between
the two experiments are worthy of discussion. The first
difference is that the volunteering behaviors we studied
in experiment 1 were clerical tasks that were not highly
visible to others in the group (i.e., stuffing envelopes, making
copies, etc.) whereas in experiment 2 the taskswere
much more visible (i.e., working in a snack booth
serving other parents and volunteers). One might expect
that recognition would be less important for the socially
visible tasks in experiment 2 because others would
already notice and have the ability to reinforce the behaviors. However, this was not the case as recognition was
also effective in experiment 2.
The studies also differed in the extent to which prospective volunteers
were associated with the group seeking help. In experiment 1 subjects were asked to volunteer
to a fund-raising group that was a subunit of their
business school. In this instance subjects had an established relationship with the
group seeking help. In contrast, subjects in experiment 2 were in the process of registering their
children for a youth soccer league. The parents’
connection to the soccer league is typically informal and limited.
The differing levels of group connection across studies
did not affect the pattern of results although the
average number of hours volunteered by students was much higher
(X̄ Exp
1 = 4.5
hours, X̄ Exp
2 = 2.4
hours).
Finally, the studies differed with respect to the
nature of
the manipulations. In experiment 1 the volunteer-based
group was competitive,
and so perceptions of group need
were influenced by information about the group’s
performance relative to a ‘‘hated’’ crosstown rival. In contrast, the
group-need manipulation in experiment 2 was based
directly on the organization’s level of volunteer
support.
In terms of the recognition manipulations, subjects in the
promised recognition condition in experiment 1 were told
they would receive a plaque at their graduation ceremony
whereas in experiment 2 subjects were informed they
would receive an official volunteer T-shirt in
appreciation for helping. The differences in the manipulations were
necessary to examine the hypotheses in the varied group
contexts, but they did not affect the pattern of
results.
Directions for Future Research
Future research is needed to understand the effects of
group success and recognition on factors related to
volunteer loyalty, task performance, and task-related
satisfaction. Recognition could have a significant effect on long
term aspects of volunteering related to retaining
existing volunteers and maintaining their participation levels. Examining this
phenomenon would require a research design
very different from that used in the present studies.
Specifically, the research would need to examine attitudes
and behaviors subsequent to both volunteer participation
and the presence or absence of actual recognition.
Also related to the
temporal aspects of the study is the potential for low success or high need
appeals to be less effective with repeated use. One reason for diminishing returns
and perhaps even a negative effect on volunteering is that ‘‘cries of wolf ’’
become less believable the more they are employed. A second reason is that the
target might become more likely to assume that the reason for low success (or
high need) lies within the group rather than outside it (see Folkes 1988).
Consequently, prospective volunteers might be less likely to
contribute time and effort to a group that is seen as responsible for its own lack
of success. Other evidence suggests that the use of extrinsic rewards will
lessen the intrinsic satisfaction of volunteering and weaken task performance (for
a review see Lepper and Greene [1978]).
Future research also
might benefit from applying the social norm concepts examined here to other
socially desirable consumption behaviors such as litter reduction, blood donation, energy conservation, and recycling.
Given that the
likelihood of undertaking such behaviors is elevated when they are perceived to
be both highly desirable and socially visible, organizations can
undertake strategies to encourage the behaviors. For example, recycling might be increased by a combination
of advertising that highlights the importance of the behavior and the use of
highly visible recycling bins and containers. Petroleum companies could promote
the purchase of cleaner gasoline’s by emphasizing the country’s need to reduce
auto emissions and offering bumper stickers that identify the driver as ‘‘part
of the solution.’’ The Red Cross might benefit from developing closer ties
between promotional campaigns that highlight blood supply shortages and the provision
of pins that identify people who have given blood that day. Social norm
concepts might be usefully applied to the promotion of other socially desirable
behaviors as well.
Conclusion
The article reports on
two experiments that found a significant interaction between group need and the
value of recognition. Experiment 1 was a laboratory study using student
subjects in a competitive group setting. Experiment 2 was a field study using
nonstudent subjects in a cooperative setting. Both experiments found that
promised recognition increased volunteer participation only when the group
seeking support was portrayed as needy.
No other effects of group
need or promised recognition on the extent of volunteering were found. The
results suggest that volunteer organizations must be sensitive to the context
under which they establish recognition programs and use promotional appeals
based on the significance of their need.
REFERENCES
American
Red Cross (1988), Volunteer 2000 Study: Findings and
Recommendations, Vol. 1, Washington,
DC: American Red Cross.
Babchuk, Nicholas and C. Wayne Gordon (1962), the Voluntary Association in the Slum, University of Nebraska Studies, New Series,
No. 27, Lincoln: University of
Nebraska Press.
Bagozzi, Richard P. (1981), ‘‘Attitude, Intentions and Behavior: A Test of Some Key Hypotheses, ’’ Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 41 (October), 607–627.
(1982), ‘‘A Field Investigation of Causal Relations among
Cognitions, Affect, Intentions,
and Behavior, Journal of Marketing
Research, 19 (November), 562–583.
And
David J. Moore (1994), ‘‘Public Service Advertisements: Emotions and
Empathy Guide Prosaically Behavior, ’’
Journal of Marketing, 58 (January), 56–70.
Bandura, A. (1977),
Social Learning Theory, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Baron, Reuben M. and David A. Kenny (1986), ‘‘the Moderator-Mediator Variable Distinction
in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual,
Strategic, and Statistical Considerations, ’’ Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 51 (December), 1173–1182.
Baron, Robert S.,
Norbert L. Kerr, and Norman Miller (1992), Group Processes, Group Decision, Group Action, Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Batson, C. Daniel (1990) ‘‘How Social Is the Animal? The
Human Capacity for Caring” American Psychologist, 45 (March), 336–346.
Baumann, Donald J.,
Robert B. Cialdini, and Douglas T. Kendrick (1981), ‘‘Altruism as Hedonism: Helping and
Self-Gratification as Equivalent Responses,
’’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40 (June), 1039–1046.
Belk, Russel W. (1995), ‘‘Awards, Rewards,
Prizes, and Punishments, ’’ in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 22, ed. Frank R. Kardes and
Mita Sujan, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 9–15.
Bendapudi, Neeli, Surendra N. Singh, and Venkat Bendapudi (1996), ‘‘Enhancing Helping Behavior: An Integrative Framework
for Promotion Planning, ’’ Journal of
Marketing, 60 (July),
33–49.
Berkowitz, Leonard and Larry R. Daniels (1964), ‘‘Affecting the Salience of the Social
Responsibility Norm, ’’ Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68 (March),
275–281.
Bonjean, Charles M.,
William T. Markham, and Patrick O. Macken (1994), ‘‘Measuring Self-Expression in Volunteer Organizations:
A Theory-Based Questionnaire, ’’ Journal
of Applied Behavior Science, 30 (December), 487–515.
Brakeley, George A. (1980), Tested Ways to Successful
Fund Raising, New York: AMACOM.
Burnett, John J. (1981), ‘‘Psychographic and
Demographic Characteristics of Blood Donors,
’’ Journal of Consumer Research, 8 (June),
62–67.
Burnkrant, Robert E. and Thomas Page, Jr. (1982), ‘‘An Examination of the Convergent, Discriminant, and Predictive Validity of
Fishbein’s Behavioral Intention Model, ’’
Journal of Marketing Research, 19 (November), 550–561.
Carlo, Gustavo,
Nancy Eisenberg, Debra Troyer, Galen Switzer, and Anna L. Speer (1991), ‘‘The Altruistic Personality: In What Contexts
Is It Apparent, ’’ Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 61
(September), 450–458.
Cialdini, Robert B. (1993), Influence: Science and Practice, 3d ed., New York: HarperCollins.
--------, Carl A. Kallgren, and Raymond R. Reno (1991), ‘‘A
Focus
Theory of Normative Conduct: A Theoretical Refinement and Reevaluation of the
Role of Norms in Human Behavior, ’’
inAdvances in Experimental Social Psychol-ogy, Vol. 24, ed. Leonard Berkowitz, New York: Academic Press, 201–214.
----------, Mark Schaller, Donald Houlihan, Kevin Arps,
Jim Fultz, and Arthur L. Beaman
(1987), ‘‘Empathy-Based Helping: Is It Selflessly of
Selfishly Motivated?’’Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (April), 749–758.
Cohen, Jacob and Patricia Cohen (1983), Applied Multiple
Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dawson, Scott (1988), ‘‘Four Motivations for
Charitable Giving: Implications for Marketing Strategy to Attract Monetary
Donations for Medical Research, ’’
Journal of Health Care Marketing, 8 (June), 31–37.
DeRidder, Richard,
Sandra G. L. Schruijer, and Rama C. Tripathi (1992), ‘‘Norm Violation as a Precipitating Factor in
Negative Intergroup Relations, ’’ in
Norm Violations in Intergroup Relations, ed.
Richard DeRidder and Rama C. Tripathi, Oxford: Clarendon.
Eisenberg, Nancy (1982), The Development of Prosocial Behavior, New York: Academic Press.
Feldman, Daniel C. (1984), ‘‘The Development and
Enforcement of Group Norms, ’’Academy
of Management Review, 9 (January),
47–53.
Festinger, Leon, Stanley Schachter, and Kurt Back (1950), So cial Pressures in Informal Groups, New York: Harper.
Fisher, Robert J. (1993), ‘‘Socially Desirable
Responding and the Validity of Indirect Questioning, ’’ Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (September), 303–315.
Folkes, Valerie S. (1988), ‘‘Recent Attribution
Research in Consumer Behavior: A Review and New Directions, ’’ Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (March), 548–565.
Fraser, Cynthia,
Robert E. Hite, and Paul L. Sauer (1988), ‘‘Increasing Contributions in Solicitation
Campaigns: The Use of Large and Small Anchorpoints, ’’Journal of Consumer Research,
15 (September), 284–287.
Gallup
Organization (1987), The Gallup Study
of Public Awareness and Involvement with Non-Profit Organiza tions, Princeton,
NJ: Gallup.
…
Turner, Bryan S. (1988), Status, Minneapolis:
University of Minnesota Press.
Unger, Lynette S. (1991), ‘‘Altruism as a Motivation
to Volunteer, ’’ Journal of Economic
Psychology, 12 (March), 71–100.
Wilcox, Rand R. (1993), ‘‘Robustness in ANOVA, ’’ in Applied Analysis of Variance in
Behavioral Science, ed. L.
K. Edwards, New York: Marcel Dekker, 345–374.
Komentar
Posting Komentar