The Social Norms Approach:Theory, Research, and Annotated Bibliography
The Social Norms Approach:Theory, Research, and Annotated Bibliography
Berkowitz,
A. D. (2004). The social norms approach: Theory, research and annotated
bibliography. Higher Education
Center for Alcohol and Other Drug Abuse and Violence Prevention. US Department
of Education.
Please reference to source, this
paper for note only
|
from:
http://www.alanberkowitz.com/articles/social_norms.pdf
Preface – The State of Social Norms
Prevention
approaches that provide normative feedback are growing by leaps and bounds in
popularity and evidence is mounting that these programs can be effective when correctly
implemented. As a result there is a need for a comprehensive review of the social
norms literature to provide practitioners with an overview of important
research, to summarize the evaluation literature regarding its effectiveness,
and to address questions and concerns. It is my hope that this review will
serve this purpose. I am grateful to the Higher Education Center for Alcohol
and Other Drug Prevention for allowing me to revise it annually since 2000 to
keep pace with the important changes and developments in this field.
The
growing interest in social norms and evidence for its efficacy is indicated by
a number of trends. Interventions that correct misperceived social norms are
currently being funded by over five Federal agencies and by dozens of State
agencies, non-profit organizations, consortia and, less often, by the alcohol
beverage industry. Well implemented social norms programs have received
numerous awards, including two model program awards by the Center for Substance
Abuse Prevention and many from the U.S. Department of Education. Research about
social norms and its effectiveness frequently appears in scholarly journals and
other professional publications. The evidence for the social norms approach was
summarized in 2002, when, as part of a comprehensive effort to examine college
drinking, the National Institute on Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse (NIAAA)
appointed an expert panel of approximately twenty national prevention
specialists to make recommendations for the field. Communicating accurate social
norms was among the strategies suggested by this panel, which commented in its final
report that:
“Initial
results from programs adopting an intensive social norms approach are promising.
Several institutions that persistently communicated accurate norms have
experienced reductions of up to twenty percent in high-risk drinking over a relatively
short period of time…. Together these findings provide strong support for the
potential impact of the social norms approach. Although any case report in this
literature could be challenged methodologically, the results of each study are
remarkably consistent.” (NIAAA, p. 13, 2002)
Since
the NIAAA report numerous new studies have provided additional evidence for social
norms’ efficacy. In addition to addressing alcohol use with social norms
marketing campaigns other programs have demonstrated the effectiveness of
social norms interventions in reducing or preventing cigarette smoking,
reducing DWI, changing attitudes associated with rape proclivity in men, and
reducing sexual assault. Positive results have been obtained with college and
university students, with high school and middle-school populations, and in
defined populations such as sorority and fraternity members, athletes, and
first-year college students, and with individuals, groups, and communities.
More recently, a number of social norms interventions have been evaluated and
found to be successful without parallel changes in comparison groups. In addition, some evaluation research suggests that when
programs incorporate social norms as part of a comprehensive intervention using
multiple strategies, the social norms component is often one of the important
ingredients associated with program effectiveness.
With the growth of the social norms approach, concerns have been expressed
about its efficacy in general and with specific populations in particular. Some
of these concerns arise from findings that may be attributable to
implementation problems such as inadequate exposure to messages, and strategies
that are not faithful to the model.
Another problem that may mask program success is lack of adequate
evaluation. Other concerns are spurred by debates about important issues in the
field – for example, whether social norms should be part of a larger package of
interventions or whether it can be implemented by itself, and whether campaigns
should be directed at more homogeneous sub-groups rather than larger
communities. Finally, other criticisms may be based on misinformation about the
approach or lack of familiarity with the research evidence. Responses to these
concerns are incorporated into the text of this review.
The primary purpose of this paper is to summarize what we know
about social norms by reviewing the published research literature on the social
norms model rather than to provide advice on how to conduct an
intervention. Advice on implementation strategies and case studies of
successful projects can be found by consulting following resources:
• The website of
the National Social Norms Resource Center (www.socialnorm.org)
• The social
norms section of the Higher Education Center for Alcohol and
Other Drug Prevention’s Website (www.edc.org/hec/),
• The Report on
Social Norms, a monthly publication from PaperClip Communications (www.socialnormslink.com) that provides
research reviews, examples of model programs, and commentaries on current
issues,
• The Social
Norms Approach to Preventing School and College Age Substance
Abuse: A Handbook for Educators, Counselors and Clinicians, the first
book on the social norms approach, containing numerous case studies of
successful interventions, theoretical issues, and implementation strategies
(published in 2003 by Jossey-Bass),
• The National
Conference on the Social Norms Model, an annual conference that meets each
summer to provide an intensive collegial opportunity for researchers,
theorists, and practitioners to explore the state of the art of the social norms
approach (for information go to www.socialnorm.org), and:
• The Social
Norms Resource Book, a comprehensive overview of the social norms
approach covering implementation strategies for individuals, groups, and communities
and topical areas such as alcohol abuse and smoking prevention, sexual assault
prevention, and other innovative applications, available from PaperClip
Communications (for information go to: ww.socialnormslink.com, www.Paper-Clip.com, or call 866
295-0505).
This August 2004 review differs from its predecessors in a number of
ways. The text has been edited for improved readability. Results of over 60 new
studies, articles and/or resources have been incorporated, and the annotated
bibliography has been updated and expanded. New sections have been added
covering types of norms, misperceptions in specific subpopulations (athletics,
Greeks, gender and ethnicity), and issues in the evaluation of social norms
campaigns. Finally, the scope of the review has been expanded from a focus on
college and university social norms interventions to include high school,
middle-school, and community settings.
This review adopts guidelines recommended by the Journal of Studies on
Alcohol and the Inter-Association Task Force that the term “binge drinking” be
avoided and replaced by a more suitable term such as “high-risk” or “dangerous”
drinking. For a review of the arguments against the “binge-drinking” term and
relevant research see Berkowitz (2003a).
Finally, I hope that this review is helpful and welcome your comments,
queries, and suggestions for future revisions.
Alan Berkowitz, PhD
August 1, 2004
Trumansburg, N.Y.
alan@fltg.net
www.alanberkowitz.com
Introduction
The social norms approach provides a theory of human behavior that has
important implications for health promotion and prevention. It states that our
behavior is influenced by incorrect perceptions of how other members of our social
groups think and act. For example, an individual may overestimate the permissiveness
of peer attitudes and/or behaviors with respect to alcohol, smoking or other
drug use, or underestimate the extent to which peers engage in healthy behavior.
The theory predicts that overestimations of problem behavior will increase
these problem behaviors while underestimations of healthy behaviors will
discourage individuals from engaging in them. Thus, correcting misperceptions
of group norms is likely to result in decreased problem behavior or increased
prevalence of healthy behaviors. These assumptions have been validated by extensive
research on teenage and young-adult drinking and cigarette smoking and by interventions
to promote safe drinking, tobacco cessation, and safe driving on college campuses
and in middle and high schools. Other social norms interventions have been developed
to prevent sexual assault, improve academic climate, and reduce prejudicial behavior.
Social norms interventions focus on peer influences, which have a
greater impact on individual behavior than biological, personality, familial,
religious, cultural and other influences (Berkowitz & Perkins, 1986a;
Borsari & Carey, 2001; Kandel, 1985, and Perkins, 2002). An extensive
literature has documented the importance of peer influences and normative
beliefs on health behaviors of youth. Research suggests that these peer
influences are based more on what we think others believe and do (the “perceived
norm”) than on their real beliefs and actions (the “actual norm.”)
This gap between “perceived” and “actual” is referred to as a “misperception”
and its effect on behavior provides the basis for the social norms approach.
Presenting correct information about peer group norms in a believable fashion
is hypothesized to reduce perceived peer pressure and increase the likelihood
that individuals will express preexisting attitudes and beliefs that are health
promoting. Thus, providing normative feedback to correct misperceptions of
norms is the critical ingredient of the social norms approach.
This review summarizes evidence in support of the social norms approach.
It includes
individual studies and one meta-analysis (Borsari & Carey, 2003)
that document the importance of social norms and peers in influencing behavior,
including research documenting the existence of misperceptions, their efficacy
in predicting behavior, successful interventions targeting individuals, groups
and communities, a brief discussion of unsuccessful efforts and evaluation issues,
and examples of applications to other health and social justice issues. It
concludes with an annotated bibliography of important resources and an
extensive bibliography.
History
The social norms approach was first suggested by H. Wesley Perkins and myself
(Perkins and Berkowitz, 1986) in an analysis of student alcohol use patterns.
In this study we determined that college students regularly overestimated the
extent to which their peers were supportive of permissive drinking behaviors,
and we found that this overestimation predicted how much individuals drank. Our
recommendation that prevention efforts focus on providing students with
accurate information on peer drinking attitudes and behavior (Perkins &
Berkowitz, 1986; Berkowitz & Perkins, 1987a) represented a radical departure
from traditional intervention strategies that provided information on abuse and
negative consequences and concentrated primarily on the identification,
intervention, and treatment of problem users. When drug prevention emphasizes
problem behavior without acknowledging the actual healthy norm, it may foster
the erroneous belief that drinking problems are worse than is actually the case
and inadvertently contribute to the problem it is trying to solve. In contrast,
interventions based on social norms theory focus on the healthy attitudes and
behavior of the majority and try to increase it, while also using information
about healthy norms to guide interventions with abusers. The theory underlying
the social norms approach has been elaborated by Berkowitz (1997, 2004) and Perkins
(1997, 2003a). In many cases, social norms interventions have been successfully
combined with other drug prevention approaches strategies such as policy change
and other environmental strategies.
During the period of time that we were developing the social norms
approach, Hansen (1993) pointed out that correcting normative beliefs was the
critical ingredient in effective multi-component secondary school-based alcohol
prevention programs.
Michael Haines at Northern Illinois University was the first prevention
specialist to apply the theory to college students. He conducted a longitudinal
intervention in which reductions in misperceptions were associated with
increases in safe drinking and abstaining (Haines, 1996; Haines & Barker,
2003; Haines & Spear, 1996). His work was followed by similar efforts at
the University of Arizona (Glider, et. al, 2001; Johannessen & Glider,
2003; Johannessen et. al, 1999), Western Washington University (Fabiano, 2003),
Hobart and William Smith Colleges (Perkins & Craig, 2002; 2003a), the University
of North Carolina Chapel Hill (Foss et al, 2003; 2004) and other campuses, which
achieved significant reductions in high-risk drinking following the promotion
of accurate social norms about drinking behavior.
Following initial successes in reducing alcohol use and abuse, social
norms interventions were developed at colleges and universities to reduce tobacco
use and/or delay its onset (Haines, Barker & Rice, 2003; Hancock et al,
2002; Hancock & Henry, 2003; Linkenbach & Perkins, 2003a). More
recently, interventions developed for middle and high school students have
succeeded in reducing alcohol and cigarette use and/or delaying the onset of
these behaviors (Christensen & Haines, 2004; Haines, Barker & Rice,
2003; and Rice, 2003). Other applications have successfully increased seat-belt
usage (Perkins & Linkenbach, 2004), and reduced drinking while driving
(Hellstrom, 2004). In addition, social norms interventions have been developed
to prevent sexual assault (Bruce, 2002; Hillenbrand-Gunn et al, 2004;
Rodriguez, Kulley & Barrow, 2003; White, Williams and Cho, 2003).
Since its inception, the social norms approach has been described
variously as the proactive prevention model (Berkowitz, 1997, 1998), social
norming (Hunter, 1998), the perceived norms model (Thombs, 2000), norms correction,
and the norms challenging model (Farr & Miller, 2003; Peeler, et al 2000).
Currently there is an emerging consensus in favor of the term “social norms
theory” to describe the underlying theory and “the social norms approach” to
describe interventions based on the theory.
Types of Misperceptions
The
term “misperception” is used to describe the gap between actual attitudes or behavior,
and what people think is true about others’ attitudes or behaviors. Thus, a misperception
occurs when there is an overestimation or underestimation of the prevalence of
attitudes and/or behaviors in a group or population. Individuals may misperceive
their social groups or larger social environments in a number of ways that influence
their behavior. For example, the majority who engage in healthy behavior may incorrectly
believe they are in the minority (pluralistic ignorance.) In contrast,
the minority of people with unhealthy attitudes and/or behaviors may
incorrectly think that they are in the majority (false consensus).
Finally, an individual may enjoy thinking that her or his behavior is more
unique than it really is (false uniqueness). Each of these misperceptions
operates in a different way and may affect behavior differently. They are each
described in more detail below along with evidence that social norms
interventions can be effective in correcting them.
Pluralistic Ignorance. This is the most common
misperception. It occurs when a majority of individuals falsely assume that
most of their peers behave or think differently from them when in fact their
attitudes and/or behavior are similar (Miller & McFarland, 1987, 1991;
Prentice & Miller, 1996; Toch & Klofas, 1984). For example, most
college students drink moderately or not at all but incorrectly assume that
other college students drink more than themselves and also more than they do in
reality. Pluralistic ignorance encourages individuals to suppress healthy
attitudes and behaviors that are falsely thought to be non-conforming and to
provide encouragement to engage in the unhealthy behaviors that are seen
incorrectly as normative. Prentice and Miller (1996), two of the most
influential theorists on pluralistic ignorance noted that:
The norm
simply must be powerful enough to induce people to act in ways that do not
correspond to their private thoughts and feelings…Individuals recognize that their
own norm-congruent behavior is at variance with their true sentiments, but then
do not assume a similar discrepancy in others. Instead, their social perception
is guided by what they observe: They infer that the actions of others reflect
accurately what they are thinking and feeling. (p.162) Social norms
interventions correct pluralistic ignorance by informing the majority that their
behavior is actually more normative and healthy than they think. This normative
feedback provides permission to act on values of moderation or non-use by
bringing behavior more closely in line with personal attitudes and removes the
fear of embarrassment associated with acting in ways that are thought to be
different.
False Consensus is the incorrect belief that others are like one-self when in fact they
are not (Ross, Greene & House, 1977). For example, heavy drinkers may
incorrectly think that most other students are heavy drinkers, or prejudiced
individuals may incorrectly believe that they speak for their group. The false
consensus misperception functions to maintain an individual’s denial that his
or her attitudes or behavior are problematic or unusual. Thus, heavy drinkers
have a personal motivation for believing in exaggerated drinking norms because
this misperception allows them to justify their abusive drinking and deny that
there is a problem. For this reason, the false consensus misperception is
described as a “self-serving bias.” Studies have found that misperceptions have
more influence on the drinking of alcohol abusers than on other drinkers (Kypri
& Langley, 2003; Page, Scanlan & Gilbert, 1999; Perkins & Wechsler,
1996) and that abusers misperceive more than others in their group (Agostinelli
& Miller, 1994; Pollard, et al, 2000). In one review of the literature,
Borsari and Carey (2001) noted that “the more the student perceives others as
drinking heavily, or approving of heavy use, the higher personal consumption
will be” (p. 402).
Research
has documented similar patterns for smoking, with smokers overestimating smoking
prevalence more than non-smokers (Sherman et al, 1983; Sussman et al, 1988), and
gamblers overestimating gambling and favorable attitudes towards gambling
morethan non-gamblers (Larimer & Neighbors, 2003). Social norms
interventions correctfalse consensus misperceptions and have been successful in
reducing heavy drinking in a number of studies (see later citations).
Toch
& Klofas (1984) noted that the strongest and most vocally expressed views
in a community are often held by those who engage in false consensus. For
example, heavydrinking individuals have a greater stake in believing in their
misperceptions and view themselves as “subculture custodians” or guardians of
the truth about their reference group. In this imagined role, they speak out
actively against enforcement of policy and interventions to combat abuse. The
combination of false consensus and pluralistic ignorance allows these heavy
drinking “subculture custodians” to have an influence that is greatly
disproportionate to their numbers by strengthening their voice and suppressing the
voice of the “silent majority” who may favor policy initiatives and
interventions to curb abuse. This creates a “spiral of silence” in which
individuals who “perceive their position to be unsupported (even if they
constitute the majority) will fall silent, thereby creating the appearance of
even less support for the position” (Prentice & Miller, 1996, p.202).
This
“spiral of silence” may occur in relation to student reactions to campus policy
when the minority in opposition are outspoken because they believe themselves
to represent the majority (false consensus) while the majority in support are
silent because they believe themselves to be in the minority (pluralistic
ignorance). Thus, the application of social norms to policy development can be
useful in presenting a community with the true norm that exists in support of
various policies and consequences for abuse so that a consensus for action can
occur in spite of the denial and objections of those who abuse (DeJong, 2003a;
Dunnagan et al, 2003). For example, Prentice and Miller (1996) documented student
support for a keg ban at a time when most students perceived their peers as
being unsupportive of this policy, and Suls and Green (2003) found that
students underestimated other students’ concern about irresponsible alcohol
use.
False Uniqueness. The phenomenon of false
uniqueness occurs when individuals who are in the minority assume that the
difference between themselves and others is greater than is actually the case
(Suls & Wan, 1987). False uniqueness may occur among abstainers, who
underestimate the prevalence of abstinence and falsely assume that they are
more unique than they really are. When this occurs abstainers may withdraw from
participation in the larger community because they see it as more
alcohol-oriented than it really is. Social norms interventions that provide
information about the actual number of abstainers and responsible drinkers
majority can provide a sense of community for nondrinkers and make it safe to
participate more fully in campus life.
In
summary, social norms interventions have been found to be effective in changing
the behavior of the moderate or occasional-drinking majority (pluralistic
ignorance) as well as confronting and changing the behavior of the heavy
drinking minority (false consensus) while maintaining or increasing the
number of those who abstain or use infrequently (false uniqueness.)
Studies Documenting Misperceptions
False
consensus and pluralistic ignorance misperceptions have been documented in over
fifty-five published studies. Alcohol use misperceptions have been found in
studies with small samples of college students from an individual campus, in
larger surveys of individual campus populations, and in multiple campus studies
(see Table One for a listing of these studies) and among high school and middle
school students.. Some of these studies are discussed in recent reviews by
Berkowitz (2004), Perkins (2002, 2003a) and by Borsari and Carey (2001, 2003).
(Insert
Table One Here)
Borsari
and Carey (2003) describe the evidence for misperceptions using data from 23 studies
in a meta-analysis of the influence of misperceptions on behavior. They conducted
102 tests for misperceptions using data from the 23 studies and found that 91%
revealed a positive “self-other discrepancy.” They concluded that “there
appears to be substantial evidence supporting the existence of self-other norm
discrepancy in perceptions of alcohol use among college students.”
Studies
find that misperceptions of alcohol use are held by all members of campus communities
including undergraduate and graduate students, faculty and staff, students and
student leaders (Berkowitz, 1997, Berkowitz & Perkins, 1986b; University of
Michigan, 1993). They have been documented in a statewide sample of young
adults both in college and not in college (Linkenbach & Perkins, 2003b),
and among middle and high-school students (Beck & Trieman, 1996; Botvin et
al, 2001; D’Amico et al, 2001; Haines, Barker & Rice, 2003; Perkins &
Craig, 2003b; Rice, 2003; Thombs, et al, 1997).
In
addition, Thombs et. al. (1997) reported misperceptions about DWI (driving
while intoxicated) and RWID (riding with someone who is intoxicated).
Other
studies have reported misperceptions about cigarette smoking (Haines, Barker
& Rice, 2003; Hancock & Henry, 2003; Linkenbach & Perkins, 2003a;
Perkins & Craig, 2003b) and about marijuana and other illegal drug use
(Hansen & Graham, 1991; Perkins, 1985; Perkins & Craig, 2003B; Perkins
et al, 1999; Pollard et al, 2000; Wolfson, 2000). In addition to alcohol,
tobacco and other drugs, misperceptions have been documented about gambling
(Larimer & Neighbors, 2003) and bullying behavior (Bigsby, 2002).
Misperceptions of homophobia, attitudes about sexual assault, and eating
behaviors are reviewed by Berkowitz (2003b) and cited later in this paper.
There are also over fifteen studies of pluralistic ignorance documenting
misperceptions for topics such as: White’s attitudes towards desegregation,
participation in gang behavior, and the extent of student radicalism (see
Miller and McFarland, 1991 and Toch & Klofas, 1984 for reviews of this
literature). Finally, Kypri and Langley (2003) documented patterns of
misperceptions for alcohol use in a sample of New Zealand student that replicates
results of social norms research conducted in the United States.
Misperceptions
are formed when minorities of individuals are observed engaging in highly
visible problem behavior (such as public drunkenness or smoking) and when this extreme
behavior is remembered more than responsible behavior that is more common but
less visible (Perkins, 1997). These misperceptions are assumed to be normative
and may be spread further in “public conversation” by community members who act
as “carriers of the misperception,” including those who don’t engage in the
behavior (Perkins, 1997).
Research
suggests that the meaning and extent of these misperceptions may vary among individuals
with different drinking styles (Pollard, et. al, 2000). While Werch and his colleagues
(2000) suggested that correcting misperceptions may have different effects on individuals
at different stages of change, Steinman (2003) found that the misperceptions reported
by individuals at different stages of change were mostly similar.
There
is only one published study that calls into question the existence of misperceptions.
Wechsler
& Kuo (2000) claimed that students accurately perceive campus norms for drinking.
A number of authors have questioned their conclusion, including Borsari and Carey
(2003) and DeJong (2000), who point out a number of problems with methodology and
definitions.
Thus,
with only one exception that has serious methodological flaws; misperceptions have
been consistently documented for a variety of behaviors and social contexts and
in a variety of student and adult populations and sub-populations in both
individual studies and in meta-analyses.
Misperceptions in Specific Sub-populations
Misperceptions
have been found to exist in a variety of campus groups including fraternity and
sorority members (Baer, 1994; Baer, Stacy & Larimer, 1991; Bonday & Bruce,
2003; Carter & Kahnweiler, 2000; Far & Miller, 2003; Johannessen, 2004;
Larimer et al, 2001; Sher et al, 2001; Trockell et al, 2003), athletes (Perkins
& Craig, 2004; Thombs, 2000), student leaders (Berkowitz, 1997; Berkowitz
& Perkins, 1986b), first-year students (Berkley-Patton et al, 2003) and
among members of different ethnic groups (Larimer & Neighbors, 2003; Laird
& Venable, 2002) and among students of different religious backgrounds
(Perkins 1985, 1987). The research on gender differences in misperception and
for fraternities and sororities is summarized below.
Gender
differences in misperceptions. Some studies have noted a pattern of gender differences
in misperceptions (Agnostinelli & Miller, 1994; Berkowitz & Perkins,
1987b; Borsari & Carey, 2003; Campo et al, 2003; Korcuska & Thombs,
2003; Larimer & Neighbors, 2003; Marks, Graham & Hansen, 1992; Lewis
& Neighbors, forthcoming; Prentice & Miller, 1993, 1996; Schroeder
& Prentice, 1998), with women in these studies misperceiving more than men.
This may be due to the fact that women may be more influenced by environmental
influences than men (Berkowitz & Perkins, 1986a; Berkowitz & Perkins,
1987B; Crandall et al, 2002) or that women may be less involved in the culture
of alcohol use and misperceive it more. This explanation is consistent with research
suggesting that groups that are less involved in a culture of use will
misperceive it more, and may explain why some social norms campaigns have
reported a greater impact on women in their earlier phases (Odahowski &
Miller, 2000; Usdan, 2003).
Recently
a number of researchers have examined the relative influence of misperceptions for
same-sex and opposite-sex norms on the drinking of men and women. Korsusha and Thombs
(2003) examined the influence of same-sex peer drinking norms and found that they
were better predictors of drinking than other variables such as role conflict.
In addition same-sex norms for close friends were a stronger predictor of
drinking than same-sex norms for “typical students.” Lewis and Neighbors
(forthcoming) also conducted a study of the influence of same-sex norms and
compared their influence with both opposite-sex norms and combined norms. They
found that same-sex norms predicted drinking better than combined norms for both
men and women, and that opposite-sex norms were not predictive of drinking
behavior. They suggested that feedback with combined norms would be most
appropriate to use in all campus social norms campaigns (a recommendation also
made by Rice, 2001) but that same-sex norms could be used in more targeted
campaigns, especially for women.
In
another study of gender differences in misperceptions, Suls and Green (2003) documented
patterns of misperceptions within each gender, with both men and women reporting
that they were less likely to drink and were more concerned about drinking on campus
than their same-gender peers. However, some gender differences were found, including
that individual men see themselves as having more concern about problem alcohol
use than their best male friends and men in general, but as having equal
concern to women. Women, on the other hand, saw themselves as more concerned
than all reference groups. In addition, alcohol related norms were stronger for
men, resulting in a greater reluctance among men (compared with women) to
express concern about alcohol use due to fears of embarrassment from violating
gender-specific alcohol norms.
Misperceptions
among fraternity and sorority members. Bartholow and his colleagues (2003)
found that peer drinking norms were more influential in predicting Greek
drinking than Greek membership. Misperceptions of fraternity drinking by
fraternity members may not be universal, however. Both Larimer et al (1997) and
Carter & Kahnweiler (2000) documented misperceptions in general campus
populations but reported exceptions for some heavy-drinking Greeks who
misperceived their housemates very little or who accurately perceived the heavy
drinking of their housemates. Similarly, Borsari and Carey (2003), in a meta-analysis
of social norms studies found that misperceptions were smaller in fraternity
than in non-fraternity groups. In one multicampus study, fraternity members
misperceived the injunctive norms of other fraternity members but slightly
underestimated their actual alcohol use (Trockel et al, 2003).
Carter
& Kahnweiler (2000) suggested that social norms interventions may not be appropriate
in such cases. However, on other campuses where misperceptions have been documented
for Greeks and corrected, social norms interventions have been successful (Bonday
& Bruce, 2003; Far & Miller, 2003; Johannessen et al, 2003; Larimer et
al, 1997).
Social
norms interventions that have targeted the alcohol use of specific
sub-populations include sororities (Johannessen, 2004), fraternities ( Bonday
and Baurele, 2003; Larimer et al, 2001), first-year students (Berkley-Patton et
al, 2003; Cimini, Page & Trujillo, 2002), and athletes (Perkins &
Craig, 2004). Berkley-Patton and her colleagues (2003) documented
misperceptions among first year students and found that these misperceptions
were more extreme upon entering college than they were after residing on campus
for most of the first year. Others have used social norms interventions to
address sexual assault, including Hillenbrand-Gunn and her colleagues (2004),
who designed a successful high-school group social norms intervention for men,
White, William and Cho (2003) implemented a media campaign that successfully
reduced sexual assault among deaf and hard-of-hearing students, and Bruce
(2002) who developed a sexual assault prevention media campaign directed at
men..
Types of Norms
There
are different types of norms. One kind of norm refers to attitudes or what
people feel is right based on morals or beliefs (injunctive norms). A second
type of norm is concerned with behavior, i.e. what people actually do (descriptive
norms). Borsari and Carey’s 2003 meta-analysis of 23 studies of norms
misperceptions (described as “selfother differences”) found that misperceptions
for injunctive norms were greater than misperceptions for behavioral norms.
They also found that injunctive norms were more likely than descriptive norms
to predict drinking behavior and negative consequences of drinking. Trockel,
Williams and Reis (2003) reached a similar conclusion in an investigation of
injunctive and descriptive drinking norms in fraternities, as did Larimer and
Neighbors (2003) in a study of misperceptions of gambling norms. While both injunctive
and descriptive norms are widely surveyed in social norms efforts, most successful
interventions have used descriptive norms. Thus, it is not clear from existing research
if one of these norm types would be more likely to change behavior than the other
and should therefore be preferred in social norms interventions. Perkins
(2003b) has pointed out that even when behavioral norms are permissive,
feedback about injunctive norms can be effectively used in social norms
interventions because they are usually more conservative than actual behavior.
Which Norms Are Salient?
Individuals
have friends, are members of groups, live in residence halls on college campuses,
and are parts of a larger community. Each of these overlapping groups have norms
that may be similar or different, and some or all of these norms may exert an influence
on an individual’s behavior. Borsari and Carey (2001) reviewed the literature on
peer influences and reported that eighteen different targets have been used in
social norms research ranging from “your best friend” to “an average student.”
Thus, one critical issue is to evaluate the saliency of these different norms
when designing a social norms intervention. For example, on most campuses
students have a general idea of the “average” student and are influenced by
this campus norm (Perkins, 2003b) even when the norms of friends and more
immediate groups are more influential. In other cases, group identity may
supplant campus or community identity, especially if the community is very
heterogeneous or diffuse (for example, on a commuter campus).
Among
these different targets for norms, it is well-documented that misperceptions increase
as social distance increases. Individuals perceive that friends drink more than
they do and that students in general drink more than their friends (Baer, 1994;
Baer, Stacy & Larimer, 1991; Beck & Treiman, 1996; Borsari & Carey,
2003; Carter & Kahnweiler, 2000; Kypri & Langley, 2003; Thombs, et.al.,
1997; Thombs, 2000).
Among
college students, others in a living unit are thought to drink more than
friends but less than students in general, and students who live together tend
to develop similar patterns of misperceptions over time (Bourgeois & Bowen,
2001). Similarly, misperceptions are greater in larger high schools (Perkins
& Craig, 2003b).
Misperceptions
thus tend to increase as social distance from the misperceiver increases, but
social groups that are “closer” are more influential in shaping behavior. This
was the conclusion of Borsari and Carey (2003) in a meta-analysis of social
norms studies, in which they found that misperceptions were greater as social
distance increased while the influence on behavior of closer or more salient
social groups was stronger. A similar conclusion was reached in a study by
Korcuska and Thombs (2003) and Lewis and Neighbors (forthcoming) in which
perceived norms for close friends were a stronger predictor of alcohol use
and/or consequences than drinking norms for “typical students.”
One
exception to this phenomenon appears to be fraternity members, who often think
that the drinking of other Greeks is greater than it really is, but who
correctly believe that Greeks drink more than non-Greeks (Baer, 1994; Baer,
Stacy & Larimer, 1991; Carter & Kahnweiler, 2000; Larimer, et al,
1997).
Both
“global” and “local” norms may predict behavior and exert simultaneous
influence (Prentice & Miller, 1993) even when they may be of unequal
strength. Which norms are “salient” may vary by group and setting. Thus, Thombs
(2000) found that athletes on one campus were more influenced by non-athlete
norms than athlete norms. In contrast, on campuses with very strong athletic
cultures, athlete norms may be more salient than nonathlete norms. In another
study on a large heterogeneous campus, Campo et al (2003) found that friends’
norms predicted behavior while general student norms did not. This finding is
consistent with the observation of Borsari & Carey (2001) that campus norms
may be weaker predictors of behavior on very large campuses.
The
question of whether closer “local” norms of a group or more distant “global” community
norms should be addressed in a particular norms correction initiative is a complex
one, and must take into account the culture of the group in question and the context
and social ecology of the community. Ideally both can be addressed together through
a combination of primary and secondary prevention strategies such as small group
norms interventions and community-wide social norms media campaigns. In most cases
either general campus-wide campaigns or more local group norms challenging interventions
can result in behavior change, although there may be some groups who are resistant
to campus-wide interventions. Selecting the most relevant and salient norms for
a particular intervention and the appropriate strategy for changing those norms
is an important part of the planning process of a social norms intervention.
(For a more extensive discussion of this issue see Borsari and Carey, 2003).
Do Misperceptions Predict Behavior?
There
are at over twenty published studies in which misperceptions are positively correlated
with drinking behavior or predict how individuals drink (Beck & Trieman, 1996;
Botvin, et al, 2001; Clapp & McDonnell, 2000; D’Amico et al, 2001; Korcuska
& Thombs, 2003; Kypri & Langley, 2003; Lewis & Neighbors,
forthcoming; Marks et al, 1992; Mattern & Neighbors, 2004; Page et al,
1999; Perkins, 1985, 1987; Perkins & Wechsler, 1996; Prentice & Miller,
1993; Scher et al, 2001; Steffian, 1999; Thombs, 1999; Thombs et al, 1997;
Trockel et al, 2003).
In
one literature review, the authors concluded that “perceived normative support
of others for drinking consistently predicts personal alcohol use… and to a
lesser extent, alcohol-related problems” (Borsari & Carey, 2001). In a
number of studies, perceptions of drinking norms were positively associated
with drinking behavior. For example,
• Perkins and Wechsler
(1996), in a multi-campus study, found that perceptions of campus drinking
climate explained more of the variance in drinking behavior than any other
variable;
• Clapp and McDonnell (2000)
found that perceptions of campus norms predicted drinking behavior and
indirectly influenced drinking-related problems;
• In two different studies
conducted on the same campus, Perkins (1985, 1987) found that misperceptions
predicted alcohol use (Perkins, 1985) and problem use (Perkins, 1987) for
students from different religious backgrounds;
• Thombs, Wolcott and Farkash
(1997) and Beck and Trieman (1996) found that the best predictors of alcohol
use were misperceptions of alcohol use and social climate/context, which both
predicted heavy drinking and negative consequences; • Korcuska and Thombs (2003) found that alcohol use intensity and
drinkingconsequences were positively correlated with perceived norms for both
“close friends” and “typical students”; and
• Page, Scanlan and Gilbert
(1999) found that overestimations of high-risk drinking were directly
correlated with rates of high-risk drinking. Thus, higher rates of high-risk
drinking were found among college men who had greater overestimations of its
prevalence.
In
longitudinal studies examining drinking behavior, perceptions of drinking norms
at time one predicted drinking behavior at time two. For example:
• In a longitudinal study of
fraternity drinking patterns, Sher et al (2001) found that “perceptions of
heavy drinking in the Greek system are largely responsible for the prevalence
of heavy drinking among fraternity and sorority members.” (p. 50).
• Prentice and Miller (1993)
conducted a study of college freshmen and found that men adjusted their
drinking over time to fit the misperceived norm, and
•
Steffian
(1999), in a test of a small group norms intervention, found that misperceptions
helped to discriminate between college students who decreased their drinking
and those who didn’t.
Similarly,
in studies of high school and middle school populations, perceptions of norms have
accurately predicted behavior change at a later point in time. Thus,
• In a longitudinal study of
over 1500 high school students, only perceived intensity of student alcohol use
predicted behavior change so that “higher peer perceptions of alcohol use were
associated with subsequent escalations of personal drinking” (D’Amico et al,
2001);
• Two years after a
multi-component controlled middle school-based intervention to reduce high-risk
drinking, accurate perception of peer norms was the only outcome variable
associated with continued reductions in high-risk drinking (Botvin, et al,
2001);
• In a prevention program for
middle school students, participants’ estimates of the prevalence of alcohol
use predicted their level of use one year later (Marks, Graham & Hansen
1992); and
• In a literature review of
effective school-based prevention programs, Hansen (1985) concluded that “after
one year alcohol use was significantly reduced among students who received any
of the programs that included a normative beliefs component” while “students
who did not receive a normative beliefs component… increased their alcohol use
as if no intervention had occurred” (p. 59).
Finally,
• Thombs (1999) tested four
different models of driving while intoxicated (DWI) or riding with someone who
was intoxicated (RWID), and found that misperceptions for DWI and RWID had the
greatest predictive value in explaining both DWI and RWID.
In
summary, a substantial body of research suggests that misperceptions exist,
that misperceptions are associated with increased drinking and negative
consequences from drinking, and that drinking behavior is often best predicted
by misperceptions of drinking attitudes/or and behaviors. This includes
correlational studies, longitudinal studies, and outcome studies with
experimental and control groups. Similar findings have been reported for other
problem behaviors, such as gambling (Larimer & Neighbors, 2003).
What Is the Effect of Correcting Misperceptions?
Successful Interventions Utilizing the Social Norms Approach
Social
norms theory can be used to develop interventions that focus on the three
levels of prevention specified as universal, selective, and indicated
(Berkowitz, 1997). Thesecategories, developed by the Institute of Medicine,
replace what was formerly called primary, secondary, and tertiary prevention
(Kumpfer, 1997). Universal prevention is directed at all members of a
population without identifying those at risk of abuse.
Selective prevention is directed at members of a group that is at risk
for a behavior.
Indicated prevention is directed at particular individuals who already
display signs of the problem. Interventions at all three levels of prevention
can be combined and intersected to create a comprehensive program that is
theoretically based and has mutually reinforcing program elements.
Berkowitz
(2003b) suggested that there are certain questions that must be answered in order
for the social norms model to be applied effectively:
• What misperceptions exist
with respect to the attitude or behavior in question?
• Are there over or
under-estimations of attitudes and/or behavior?
• What is the meaning and
function of misperceptions for individuals and groups?
• Do the majority of
individuals in a group or community hold these misperceptions?
• Does the target group
function as a group with respect to the behavior in question? That is, are the
group norms “salient,” and are the individuals in the group an influence on
each others’ behavior?
• What is the hypothesized
effect of these misperceptions?
• What changes are predicted
if protective behaviors that already exist in the population are supported and
increased?
A
variety of successful social norms interventions have been developed that
provide answers to these questions and address universal, selective, and
indicated prevention.
Interventions
in each of these categories are reviewed below.
Universal Prevention – Social Norms Marketing Campaigns. A number of college campuses
and high schools have successfully reduced drinking by developing community-wide
electronic and/or print media campaigns that promote accurate, healthy norms
for drinking and non-use. This includes Western Washington University (Fabiano,
2003), the University of Arizona (Glider et al, 2001, Johannessen & Glider,
2003; Johannessen, et al, 1999), Northern Illinois University (Haines, 1996;
Haines & Barker, 2003; Haines & Spear, 1996), Hobart and William Smith
Colleges (Perkins & Craig, 2002, 2003a), Rowan University (Jeffrey et al,
2003), and the University of North Carolina Chapel Hill (Foss et al, 2003,
2004). These campaigns use social marketing techniques to deliver messages
about social norms and can be described as “social norms marketing campaigns.”
At these schools, a reduction of 20% or more in high-risk drinking rates
occurred within two years of initiating a social norms marketing campaign, and
in one case resulted in reductions of over 40% after four years. Haines, Barker
and Rice (2003) reported similar results for both tobacco and alcohol in social
norms marketing campaigns conducted in two Mid-western high schools. These
findings were recently replicated in a second high school (Christensen &
Haines, 2004; Rice, 2003).
Among
the most thorough and comprehensive evaluations of social norms campaigns are those
by Perkins and Craig (2002) and Foss and his colleagues (2003, 2004). Perkins
and Craig (2002) described an intervention that combined a standard poster
campaign with electronic media, an interactive web site, class projects that
developed parts of the campaign, and teacher training for curriculum infusion.
The intervention began in 1996 at a college with higher than average alcohol
use. A number of evaluations were conducted to determine the effectiveness of
the campaign. Results included: 1) increases in drinking that normally occur
during the freshman year were reduced by 21%; 2) a campus-wide decrease in
high-risk drinking during the previous week from 56% to 46%; and 3) successive
decreases in alcohol-related arrests over a four-year time period.
Corresponding
reductions were also found in misperceptions of use, heavy drinking at a party,
and negative consequences associated with alcohol use. Surveys conducted at three
time periods over a five-year period indicate successive linear decreases in
all of these measures over time.
At
the University of North Carolina Chapel Hill Foss and his colleagues (2003,
2004) conducted a social norms marketing campaign for first-year students with
the theme:
“Whether
it’s Thursday, Friday, or Saturday night, 2 out of 3 UNC students return home with
a .00 blood alcohol concentration.” A unique feature of the study was that the
BAC data was collected using breath samples of students coming home to their
residence halls.
The
program was thoroughly evaluated and at the end of five years, the mean number
of drinks on the night of the interview decreased from 5.1 – 4.3, the
proportion of drinkers with a BAC above .05% on the night of the interview
decreased from 60% to 52%, and the percentage of respondents who could be
classified as heavy drinkers on the night of the interview decreased from 14%
to 10% (representing an overall decrease of 29%.) By using actual BAC measures,
this study addresses concerns raised about social norms campaigns that rely on
survey data to document effectiveness because it demonstrates that the
reductions in use are not due to potential response bias or the possibility
that students are taught by social norms campaigns to answer surveys
differently.
Social
norms marketing campaigns have also been successful in reducing smoking prevalence
and delaying smoking onset. For example, in a seven county campaign directed at
12-17 year olds in Montana, only 10 percent of non-smokers initiated smoking following
the campaign, while 17 percent in the control counties began smoking. This represents
a 41% difference in the proportion of teens initiating smoking in the intervention
counties as compared with those in the rest of the state (Linkenbach & Perkins,
2003A). At the University of Wisconsin at Oskosh, a 29% decrease in smoking rates
was achieved from a multi-component intervention including a social norms media
campaign, while rates at a control campus did not change significantly (Hancock,
et al, 2002). Finally, at Virginia Commonwealth University, use remained stable
and perceptions became more accurate while the number
of cigarettes smoked per month at a control campus increased (Hancock et al,
2002; Hancock & Henry, 2003). In addition to providing strong support for
the effectiveness of social norms campaigns for smoking reduction, these
studies all used comparison groups, thus strengthening the scientific literature
in support of the model. In an overview of some of these campaigns, Hancock et
al (2002) discussed the differences between smoking and alcohol use behaviors
that need to be considered when designing a social norms marketing campaign for
smoking.
Finally, Hellstrom (2004) recently reported on a three-year, seven
campus study in which DWI was reduced overall by 13% (with one campus reporting
a decline of 40%) along with reductions in high-risk use from 36% to 29%.
The website of the Social Norms Center (www.socialnorm.org) presents data
from these and other schools documenting substantial reductions in health risk
behaviors within one or two years of initiating a social norms marketing
campaign. Monographs developed by Haines (1996), Johannesen et al (1999), and
Perkins and Craig (2002), chapters by Fabiano (2003) and Linkenbach (2003), and the Social Norms
Resource Book (Berkowitz, 2003c) outline the stages of developing a social
norms marketing campaign, provide guidelines for creating effective media, and
present evaluation data in support of the effectiveness of social norms
marketing campaigns.
These results provide strong evidence that the social norms approach can
be effectively applied as a universal prevention strategy to reduce high-risk
drinking and promote moderate alcohol use, and for smoking to reduce smoking
prevalence and delay its onset.
Selective Prevention – Targeted Social Norms Interventions. Targeted
interventions focus on members of a particular group, such as first-year
students, fraternity and sorority members, athletes, or members of an academic
class. Misperceptions of close friends’ behavior are highly correlated with
personal use, a finding that has led to the development of selective social
norms interventions on a number of campuses. In most of these efforts,
information about the actual group norms are provided in small interactive
group discussions, workshops, or academic classes. Due to their smaller size and
more manageable format many of these interventions have been evaluated using randomized
assignment to experimental and control groups.
The following successful targeted small group norms interventions have
been reported:
• Schroeder &
Prentice (1998) designed an intervention for first-year students that randomly
assigned participants to one of two discussion groups during their first term.
In the norm-focused condition, students were given data showing systematic
misperceptions of drinking norms on campus and participated in a facilitated
discussion about actual norms and the social dynamics of drinking. In the individual-focused
condition, students participated in a discussion of how to make responsible
drinking decisions. In a follow-up questionnaire six months later, students in
the norm-focused condition consumed significantly fewer drinks each week than
students in the individual-focused condition.
• Washington State University
pioneered the development of selective interventions with groups such as
athletes, fraternities, sororities, and first-year students (Barnett, et. al,
1996; Far & Miller, 2003; Peeler et al, 2000). WSU’s “Small Group Norms
Challenging Model” provides group members with feedback about their group’s
actual and perceived drinking patterns in a 45-minute workshop.
Discussion
focuses on the nature and causes of misperceptions in a talk show format using
slides with data on actual and perceived norms for that group. This approach
has produced reductions in drinking among first-year students, Greeks, and
athletes who received the intervention and was sustained enough to create campus-wide
reductions in drinking over a number of years (Far & Miller, 2003).
For
example, over a nine-year period, students who drink 5 or more drinks on an occasion
decreased from 59% to 30%, and abstainers increased from 12% to 20%.
• Peeler et al (2000)
designed an intervention as part of a course at Washington State University in
which the experimental group received a class module on drinking norms. At the
end of the term, the male students in the experimental group reported
significant changes in their perceptions of campus drinking and also reported
consuming less alcohol per occasion than students in the control group.
• Steffian (1999) assigned
college men to either a normative education group (experimental group) or a
traditional alcohol education program (control group).
The
experimental group participated in a group consensus exercise in which the group
made predictions about campus drinking norms and then were presented with
actual data, evidence of their own misperceptions, and a discussion of social norms
theory. The control group watched a film on the physiological effects of alcohol.
The author reported that “Participants in the normative education groups demonstrated
more accurate perceptions of campus drinking norms and a significant reduction
in the consequences of alcohol use while those in the control group did not.
Changes in normative perception were among the strongest contributors to a
function discriminating between those who decreased their drinking and those
who did not.”
• Bonday & Bruce (2003)
adapted the small group norms challenging model to develop a normative feedback
intervention for fraternities at the University of Virginia. They reported a
decrease in negative consequences of fraternity member drinking after the
intervention, although actual drinking rates did not change.
• Hillenbrand-Gunn and her
colleagues at the University of Missouri-Columbia (2004) developed a
three-session intervention on acquaintance rape for high school boys that
incorporated local social norms. The workshop resulted in more accurate
perceptions and decreases in rape supportive attitudes that were maintained at
a one-month follow-up.
Another
way of delivering social norms messages to groups is through the use of interactive
peer theater. Scripts for such performances are available from BACCHUS (2002)
or can be developed locally. For example:
• At the University of
Albany, social norms data were integrated into interactive scenarios presented
to students in a freshman seminar class (Cimini, Page & Trujillo, 2002).
Students in the control condition were enrolled in the same class but received
an academic lecture on substance abuse instead. Students in the theater
intervention reported a significant decrease in frequency of use, DWI, and
regretted behavior, and an increase in the incidence of protective behaviors in
comparison with the control group. The intervention group also reported a reduction
in high-risk drinking rates while these rates increased in the control group.
Other
selective interventions have utilized focused media campaigns directed at a particular
group of students in combination with other strategies:
• The University of Virginia
designed a targeted social norms marketing campaign for first-year students by
placing posters in bathroom stalls in first-year residence halls. In its first
year (1999-2000) the campaign was successful in reducing the drinking of women
and non-fraternity men but not fraternity men (Ohahowski & Miller, 2000).
In the next two years of the campaign, which also incorporated environmental
management strategies, these improvements continued and the drinking of
fraternity men also improved (Bauerle, Burwell & Turner, 2002). Thus, over
a period of three years, the number of drinks per week for first-years went
down from 3 drinks a week to 1, the median number of drinks per week for
fraternity first-year men went down from 15 to 7, and the percentage of
abstainers went up from 35% to 49%. In a subsequent presentation of this data,
Bauerle (2003) reported that the campaign was expanded to the entire campus and
that negative consequences for first-year students continued to trend
downwards. These results serve as an important reminder that social norms
campaigns may not affect all groups equally (especially at first) and that
sustained effort is required over a period of years to normalize improvements
and extend them to all students.
• At Rochester Institute of
Technology, a social norms marketing campaign was developed for deaf and
hard-of-hearing students to reduce the incidence of sexual assault (White,
Williams, & Cho, 2003). In this intervention, a campus-wide social norms
marketing campaign to prevent sexual assault that had been offered to all
students (including deaf and hard-of-hearing) was re-designed to tailor it to the
culture and communication styles of deaf and hard-of-hearing students. While the
all-campus campaign did not have an effect on deaf and hard-of-hearing students,
the tailored campaign was successful in changing attitudes and perceptions, and
resulted in fewer sexual assaults.
• At the University of
Arizona (Johannessen, 2004) a targeted social norms campaign was developed for
sorority members focusing on the ethic of caring among women and providing
feedback about actual drinking norms and attitudes.
As
a result of the campaign, significant decreases in high-risk drinking were
reported on a number of measures while sorority drinking remained the same on a
control campus.
• Mattern and Neighbors
(2004) randomly assigned students in a residence hall to an experimental
condition in which participants were given normative feedback through a variety
of channels, and a control condition. They found that corrected perceptions
were associated with decreases in the quantity and frequency of drinking among
students in the experimental group. In addition, a smaller group of students
whose misperceptions increased during the campaign reported higher use,
providing strong support for the assumptions of the social norms approach
• In a project sponsored by
the Kansas Health Foundation (Berkley-Patton et al, 2003), first-year students
received a social norms intervention that resulted in significantly decreased
drinking rates for first year students when rates of use for the summer before
college were compared with spring term drinking.
These
examples provide strong support for the effectiveness of selective social norms
interventions directed at particular groups of at-risk individuals when used
alone or in combination with other strategies. Targeted social norms
interventions such as these may be more effective when the normative data are
tailored to the group in question and when they are presented in more extended,
interactive formats. As noted earlier, it is extremely important to determine
the most salient and relevant influences on the target group before designing
an intervention to make sure that the norms being corrected are influential.
These
influences may differ by gender and membership in groups such as athletics and fraternities
or sororities and may vary by campus. When offered in the context of a campus-wide
media campaign, the two interventions should be designed so that they are compatible
with each other.
Indicated Prevention – Individualized Social Norms Interventions. Normative data about
drinking can be presented to high-risk drinkers and abusers as part of
individual counseling interventions. These interventions are theoretically
sound because abusers tend to adhere strongly to misperceptions that serve to
rationalize their abuse (i.e. see the section on “false consensus.”) Sharing
normative data in a motivational interviewing format is a non-judgmental way to
create cognitive dissonance in heavy drinkers and catalyze change.
The
most well-known and scientifically supported individualized intervention that includes
a norms correction component has been developed by Alan Marlatt and his colleagues
at the University of Washington (Dimeff, et. al. 1999). The Alcohol Skills Training
Program (ASTP) uses an eight-session motivational interviewing approach based
on stages of change theory to provide heavy drinkers with non-judgmental feedback
about their drinking. Data collected prior to the interview are used to provide
comparisons between the individual’s drinking and actual rates of peers’
drinking on campus. This information presents heavy drinkers with the fact that
their drinking is much more extreme than that of peers on a variety of
measures. ASTP has been condensed into both a one-hour intervention (BASICS)
and a correspondence course in which subjects use a manual. All three
interventions have been successful in reducing drinking
at follow-ups as long as 1-2 years (Dimeff, et. al. 1999; Larimer & Cronce,
2002), including with high-risk drinkers (Murphy et al 2001).
Agostinelli, Brown & Miller (1995) were able to produce similar
reductions in drinking by mailing participants personalized graphic feedback
following their completion of a mailed survey. Similar results were found in a
larger population study, in which a normative feedback pamphlet was mailed to
over 6,000 households. In a follow-up general population survey a month later,
respondents from households receiving the normative feedback reported
significantly lower alcohol use than controls (Cunningham et al. 2001). Mailed
feedback was also successful in correcting perceptions and reducing drinking in
a study of high-risk college drinkers (Collins et al, 2002). These finding have
been replicated in other samples, including one conducted in a workplace where reductions
in consumption were documented for heavy drinkers following normative feedback
without any increase in drinking on the part of non-drinkers (Walters & Woodall,
2003). Neighbors, Larimer & Lewis (2004) found similar results using computerized
normative feedback with alcohol consumption remaining lower at three and six
month follow-up assessments. Finally, Agostinelli and her colleagues
(forthcoming) found that heavy drinkers were more likely to acknowledge that
they had a drinking problem when they learned as a result of the intervention
that their own drinking quantity was above the norm.
“Check-Up to Go” or CHUG is another widely used tool for providing
personalized individual feedback about drinking. In it’s original
paper-and-pencil version, it has produced drinking reductions in three
controlled clinical trails (Walters, 2000) and is now available on the web at www.e-chug.com.
These results are extremely promising because they are efficient and
cost-effective, produce measurable results, and can be combined with other
social norms interventions.
For example, both Western Washington University (Fabiano, 2003) and the
University of Washington (Larimer, et al, 2001) have successfully combined
universal interventions with indicated interventions providing specific
information about campus drinking norms to individual high-risk drinkers.
In addition to individual personalized feedback, high-risk drinkers and
smokers also may be influenced by campus-wide media campaigns. For example,
Perkins and Craig (2002) reported four-fold reductions in the typical increase
in high-risk drinking among firstyear students and a 21% reduction in weekly
heavy drinking among students in general at a small private college in the
Northeast. Pryor (2001) reported a decrease from 20% to 13% from 1999-2000 in
the number of students drinking ten or more drinks at a sitting at a different
small Northeastern college. Similarly, a social norms marketing campaign at University
of Wisconsin at Oshkosh directed at smokers with the theme “96% of smokers want
to quit before graduating” resulted in a 29% decrease in smoking rates in one
year.
As noted earlier, social norms interventions at Washington State
University (Far & Miller, 2003), The University of North Carolina Chapel
Hill (Foss et al 2003, 2004) and the University of Virginia (Bauerle, 2003)
have also been successful in reducing highrisk drinking.
In
summary, norms corrections interventions with heavy drinkers are theoretically
sound and can be effective both in individual contexts as part of a
motivational interviewing strategy, through computerized or mailed feedback, or
as part of community-wide media campaigns.
Social Norms Interventions with Multiple Levels of Prevention
The
social norms approach can be used to provide a guiding framework for
interventions that are universal, targeted, and indicated to create synergy
between these levels of prevention. For example, the University of Arizona
combined a universal social norms marketing intervention and other
environmental management interventions with a moderation skills program for
high-risk drinkers and a targeted campaign directed at sorority members (Glider
et al, 2001; Johannessen, 2004; Johannessen & Glider, 2003; Johannessen et
al, 1999). Efforts were made to educate stakeholders who were likely to be
“carriers of the misperception” about the goals and purposes of the
intervention by providing specialized training and developing literature
specifically designed for faculty and staff. Interventions utilizing normative
feedback were also integrated into environmental management strategies that
were successful in reducing problems at the University of Arizona’s annual
homecoming event (Johannesssen et al, 2001) In a well-designed intervention at
the University of Washington, Larimer and colleagues (2001) combined selective
and indicated prevention by providing normative feedback about drinking to
individual fraternity members and their whole houses. Participants were
assessed during their pledge year and one year later. The intervention and
resulted in significant reductions in alcohol use and peak BAC when compared
with fraternity members in the control condition.
These
examples suggest that it is possible to combine social norms interventions at
all levels of prevention to create a comprehensive change environment with
mutuallyreinforcing, synergistic messages delivered through a variety of
channels to a variety of audiences.
Such
programs are comprehensive, relevant, intensive, and promote positive messages,
characteristics that are components of effective prevention programs
(Berkowitz, 1997).
This
integrated approach is more likely to succeed than the common practice of developing
multiple individual interventions that are not compatible or programmatically linked
and that are often inconsistent with each other.
Norms Correction as Part of A Multi-Component Intervention
A
number of community-wide and school-based comprehensive interventions have incorporated
norms correction into classroom or workshop activities that fall within one of
the levels of prevention specified above. Because the social norms component is
only one of multiple interventions used, it is not always possible to evaluate
its impact specifically. In these cases, revealing accurate norms to
participants may have served as a catalyst to increase their receptivity to
other program components, such as skilltraining, information, and strengthening
resiliency.
For
example, a comprehensive middle-school intervention with a norms correction component
that had previously been effective among white students was offered in a minority
inner-city school. The experimental group demonstrated high-risk drinking rates
over 50% lower than the control group in follow-up assessments. Two years after
the intervention, corrected perceptions remained correlated with reductions in
high-risk drinking (Botvin et al, 2001.) In a similar example for smoking,
norms correction strategies were incorporated into a multi-component
intervention to reduce smoking among high-school students that resulted in a
rate of 14% of students smoking weekly in comparison with 24% in the control
group (Perry et al, 1992).
Similar
findings have been reported in more comprehensive literature reviews. For example,
Hingson & Howland (2002) reviewed comprehensive community interventions to
address alcohol, cigarettes, other drugs, and cardiovascular health and
suggested that norms correction strategies may be particularly appropriate for
adolescents and young adults. And in separate analyses of successful alcohol
prevention programs in middle and high schools, both Hanson (1993) and Clemens
and Thombs (2004) concluded that normative feedback was the critical ingredient
accounting for the success of these programs.
Finally,
Dunnagan and colleagues (2003) proposed a theoretical model for reducing underage
drinking that combined environmental management, decision-balance, and norms
correction to demonstrate the efficacy of using multiple models and evaluation techniques
synergistically to formulate public policy.
These
studies suggest that social norms activities can effectively be included in comprehensive
interventions that are multi-faceted and incorporate a variety of compatible
strategies.
When Social Norms Interventions Are Unsuccessful
As
interventions based on the social norms model become more prevalent, there are
more examples of unsuccessful interventions. This is natural as the field
evolves and grows and is especially likely in light of the many ways in which
the implementation and evaluation process can be flawed.
Berkowitz
(2003c), Haines (1996), Johannesesen (1999), and Linkenbach (2003) provide a
detailed overview of the phases of implementing a social norms media campaign,
which Fabiano (1999) has condensed into six stages:
• assessment (collection of
data),
• selection of the normative
message
• testing the message with
the target group
• selecting the normative
delivery strategy
• dosage of the message
• evaluation of the
effectiveness of the message.
Mistakes
can occur at any of these stages. For example, participants are likely to question
initially the validity of survey data because of misperceptions they hold, but
will rethink their assumptions if the data are reliable and presented in an
open manner. In contrast, unreliable or confusing survey data may be rejected
and in the end undermine the campaign and reinforce misperceptions. In
addition, media that are confusing or unappealing, presented by unreliable
sources, or not presented in sufficient doses will not have an impact. Key
stakeholders can also undermine campaigns through negative comments and
criticisms or by sharing their own misperceptions. The following examples
illustrate some of these reasons for campaign failure.
Werch
et al (2000) outlined an unsuccessful campaign in which social norms messages were
sent through the mail to a small sample of freshmen. Three “greeting cards”
with normative data were sent in the fall term and a follow-up phone call was
conducted in the spring term. This campaign may have failed because the
campaign was only conducted over a one-month period, which may not have been
long enough, the messages were not focus grouped with students in advance and
they may not have been persuasive, and the target subjects were exposed to
campus-wide misperceptions that may have undermined the campaign’s messages.
Clapp,
Russell and DeJong (2001) reported on a failed social norms media campaign in which
students did not understand the message, the message and image were
incongruent, and the image overpowered the message. In this campaign the image
(a student throwing up) was inconsistent with the normative data provided, and
students were more likely to remember the image than the data.
Granfield
(2002) provided a case study of a well-designed social norms media campaign that
did not achieve expected outcomes because the message source was not believable
to students. The campaign took place on a campus with a strong fraternity
presence at a time when fraternities felt that they were under attack by the
administration. Due to this feeling students rejected the social norms messages
because the campaign was felt to be part of an administration-led effort to
undermine fraternities.
These
findings suggest that when social norms campaigns are unsuccessful it is
important to assess what went wrong and why, rather than to assume that the
approach itself is flawed.
Unsuccessful
interventions and philosophical/theoretical disagreements have led some to question
the overall validity and effectiveness of social norms. Thus, critics have
raised concerns about unsuccessful interventions, provided theoretical
disagreements about the assumptions of the social norms approach, questioned
the compatibility of norms correction campaigns with the underlying mission of
higher education, and debated definitions of high-risk drinking. Berkowitz
(2002) has provided an extensive response to each of these concerns, suggesting
that some may be based on misunderstandings and overgeneralizations about the
implications of failed interventions, while others reflect important
theoretical and methodological issues that need to be addressed as part of the
evolution of the model. In the same article, Rice (2002) reviewed common
questions and concerns based on methodological issues.
Issues in the Evaluation of Social Norms Interventions
Prevention
approaches that are evidence-based need to be carefully evaluated to determine
their effectiveness. In the case of social norms interventions, it is
hypothesized that corrections of misperceptions translate into behavior change.
Kilmer and Cronce (2003) discussed issues in the evaluation of social norms campaigns
and noted the importance of designing surveys that capture anticipated changes,
the need to evaluate message impact in addition to message exposure, and the
value of assessing differential campaign impact on population sub-groups in
addition to global change. Finally they noted that categorical measures of
change may not reveal other important effects of a campaign. For instance, an
exclusive focus on the total percentage of students drinking “0-4” can mask
beneficial changes that occur within the 0-4 group after a campaign.
Perkins
(2004) has also outlined evaluation challenges including the problem of overlooking
campaign successes through insufficient data analysis.
Evaluations
and the conclusions based on them can be compromised when the premises of the
evaluation are not theoretically sound. For example, assumptions can be made about
the underlying theory of social norms that are incorrect, inappropriate
measures may be used to evaluate change, or evaluators may neglect to assess
the fidelity of the intervention to the model. In each of these cases, an
evaluator may conclude that a particular intervention or the model itself is
not effective when in fact the evaluation itself has been compromised by these
factors. A number of recent evaluations of social norms campaigns share one or
more of these problems.
In
once recent study, for example, Campo and her colleagues (2003) found that
drinking behavior was related to perceptions of friends drinking but not to
campus norms. They concluded that because campus norms were not salient to the
students in their sample that social norms theory was based on inaccurate
premises. Yet their finding of saliency for perceptions of friends’ norms in
fact supports the theory and serves as a reminder that different norms may be
salient for different groups or on different campuses. In addition, their
sample was primarily comprised of students living off-campus, who may have differed
from on-campus students in terms of the saliency of campus norms.
In
another study, Licciardone (2004) analyzed data from 57 campuses and
constructed a measure to assess the degree of misperception of alcohol use on
each campus. Using this measure he found that campuses with more accurate
perceptions had more drinking than campuses with less accurate perceptions,
leading him to conclude that the results contradicted social norms theory.
However this conclusion is not accurate for a number of reasons. First, the
misperception measure used was flawed because it was derived by creating
averages of drinking behavior and misperception for each campus. This would not
create a reliable measure because the degree of misperception can vary among students
and for particular campus sub-groups which will result misleading averages.
Second,
the measure assumed that it is possible to eliminate misperceptions altogether,
something that Borsari and Carey (2001) have suggested is not theoretically
possible. In fact, if a social norms campaign was effective the misperceptions
might still exist. Third, the study evaluated data from only one particular
point in time without assessing if social norms interventions were utilized.
For these reasons it is not appropriate to assume that “the misperceptions
ration may be taken as a surrogate measure of the potential effectiveness of
overall social norms programming on each campus” (p. 242).
Clapp
and his colleagues (2003) conducted a social norms marketing campaign in a residence
hall while another residence hall served as a control group. At the end of
thesix week intervention, misperceptions were reduced but there were no
significant effects or counterintuitive effects on drinking, leading the
authors to conclude that the campaign had “failed.” Yet an analysis of data
provided in the article shows that while drinking increased in both groups
during the course of the campaign, the increase was much less in the
experimental group, which also reported drinking less per occasion. Thus, it
may in fact be that the campaign was having a positive effect but was not
conducted for a long enough period of time to show significant results. In
addition, it is not clear if the normative feedback provided in this study was
strong enough to counteract participant exposure to misperceptions elsewhere on
campus that the campaign did not try to correct.
For
example, Mattern and Neighbors (2004) successfully reduced drinking rates using
a similar research design but with stronger and more frequent normative
feedback.
Finally,
Wechsler and his colleagues (2003) conducted a much-publicized national study which
he described as evaluating the effectiveness of social norms campaigns. He concluded
that it did “not provide evidence for the effectiveness of social norms campaigns.”
The study has been widely criticized for methodological problems that include
poor sample sizes, no assessment of the quality of the campaigns conducted, and
a weak definition of the experimental group (see for example, Berkowitz, 2003d,
DeJong, 2003b, Haines, 2003, and Perkins and Linkenbach, 2003).
In
summary, the studies reviewed point to the importance of ensuring that
evaluations are based on valid theoretical premises and determining whether
measures of effectivenessand sample selection are appropriate.
Social Norms Interventions for Other Health Issues and Social Justice
Issues
Many
of the normative influences that affect alcohol and tobacco use are also
operative for a wide variety of other health and social justice issues,
including sexual assault and violence, disordered eating and body image
disturbance, academic climate, and prejudicial behaviors. An article by
Berkowitz (2003b) suggested that social normsefforts be used to address these
problems, reviewed research documenting misperceptionsfor different health and
social justice issues, and provided examples of innovative programs. These
interventions include a social norms interventions designed to prevent sexual
assault (Bruce, 2002’ Hillenbrand-Gunn et al, 2004; White, Williams, & Cho,
2003), and a homophobia prevention workshop that incorporates a small group norms
challenging activity (Smolinsky, 2002). Heterosexual individuals were found to overestimate
the homophobia of their peers in two studies (Bowen & Bourgeois, 2001; Dubuque
et al 2002). In two other studies, the primary factor influencing men’s willingness
to intervene to prevent sexual assault was men’s perception of other men’s willingness
to intervene (Fabiano et al, 2003; Stein & Barnett, 2004). These findings
are consistent with other research suggesting that perceived social norms can
influence whether or not individuals’ express prejudicial beliefs to others
(Crandall et al, 2002).
Berkowitz
(2003b) suggested that these misperceptions might discourage individuals who are
uncomfortable with prejudicial remarks from speaking out against these
comments.
In
preliminary studies, predictions based on social norms theory have been
confirmed for beliefs about masculinity and gender appropriate behavior, body
ideal, how often people pray, the prevalence of bullying behavior, and honesty
in paying taxes. For example, Gottfried (2002) found that men misperceived
other men’s beliefs about how men should behave, with men overestimating the
extent to which other men hold stereotypical beliefs about masculinity. Greater
disparities between men’s perceptions of themselves and of other men were
correlated with lower self-esteem. The results of this study parallel findings
of research conducted with young boys and girls, who both expressed interest in
playing with stereotypically “boy” and “girl” toys but perceived other members
of their gender to be only interested in same-gender appropriate toys (Prentice
& Miller, 1996).
And,
in a study of misperception of bullying behavior in an elementary school
setting, Bigsby (2002) found that both students and their parents overestimated
the amount of bullying behavior that occurred.
In
a study of body image women significantly overestimated the degree of thinness
that male and female peers considered as ideal. These overestimations were
positively correlated with measures of body dissatisfaction, disturbed eating,
and concern with appearance (Kusch, 2002). Hancock (2003) documented
misperceptions of prayer. She reported that individuals who pray underestimated
the prevalence of praying among their peers, and suggested that this misperception
may cause individuals to reduce or hide prayer behavior in academic
environments. Finally, Wenzel (2001) documented taxpayer misperceptions of
willingness to be honest on income tax forms (i.e., most people thought that
others were less honest than themselves) and found that correcting these misperceptions
increased honesty in the reporting of some deductions.
With
respect to academic success, a pilot project at Ball State University
documented misperceptions indicating that students perceived their peers to be
less academically motivated than themselves on a variety of variables. The
“Academic Success Norming Campaign” corrected these misperceptions with the
goal of encouraging behaviors that are associated with academic success and
retention (Abhold, Hall & Serini, 1999).
Finally,
Linkenbach, Perkins, and DeJong (2003) documented misperceptions among parents
regarding parenting attitudes and behaviors such as how often parents talk with
children about alcohol use and family rules, and discussed how correction of
these misperceptions can be utilized to strengthen effective parenting.
Although
these projects are preliminary and have not yet produced strong outcome data, they
suggest the applicability of the social norms approach to a broad range of behaviors
related to health, social justice, and fostering community.
Conclusions and Recommendations
The
effectiveness of prevention approaches addressing misperceived social norms
have been validated in numerous research studies and in campus and school
interventions since they were first proposed by H. Wesley Perkins and myself in
1986. Programs designed to reduce alcohol and tobacco use have been implemented
successfully at all levels of prevention using a variety of media and
presentation techniques. Despite these successes there are a number of
challenges facing the prevention field at present as we continue to develop new
and improved social norms interventions. These include the following:
• how to meaningfully
integrate universal, selective, and indicated social norms interventions in a
synergistic, mutually reinforcing manner;
• how to effectively combine
social norms interventions at all three levels of prevention with other
strategies such as policy enforcement and other environmental strategies;
• to determine whether
tailored social norms interventions based on gender, ethnicity and other group
identities are appropriate and effective;
• to evaluate the relative
salience of different normative targets for different populations, such as
attitudinal and behavioral norms;
• to utilize our knowledge
about successful social norms interventions to address other problems such as
sexual assault, social justice issues, eating behaviors, academic climate,
prejudicial behavior, and issues of spirituality, and adapt the model
accordingly;
• to develop standardized
evaluation criteria to ensure that social norms interventions are evaluated
appropriately and thoroughly;
• to learn from unsuccessful
interventions to develop an inventory of common mistakes, problems, and
implementation failures; and
• to develop a set of
conditions and criteria for successful implementation.
The
social norms approach provides an excellent example of how theory- and resear chdriven
interventions can be designed, implemented and evaluated to successfully
address health problems. The model incorporates recent understandings about the
important role of the environment in prevention, the nature and impact of peer
influence, the need for interventions that are tailored to their audience, and
the design of comprehensive environments that can foster change. We look
forward to new successes and innovative applications of this approach in the
years to come.
Note:
Please contact the author if you have information about a social norms
intervention or study that you would like to see included in a future revision
of this paper.
Alan David Berkowitz is an independent consultant who helps colleges,
universities, and
communities design programs that address health and social justice
issues. He is well
known for his scholarship and innovative programming and frequently
serves as an
expert advisor to organizations and federal agencies. He is the editor
and founder of
The Report on Social Norms and is the
recipient of five national awards for his work in
sexual assault prevention, drug prevention, gender, and diversity
issues. Alan has served
as a consultant and technical advisor to social norms campaigns at
Washington State
University, the University of Virginia, Rochester Institute of
Technology, the University
of Georgia, Gonzaga University, the Ithaca City School District, and
seven communitybased
campaigns in the state of Virginia. He can be reached via e-mail at: alan@fltg.net
or by calling (607) 387-3789. Other articles of his can be found at:
www.alanberkowitz.com.
Komentar
Posting Komentar