CHAPTER 1
CHAPTER 1
Applying Social
Psychology—Typical
Features, Roles, and Problems
Examples of Applied Social Psychology
A Definition of Applied Social Psychology
Divisions of the Field
Typical Features of Applied Social
Psychology
A Problem Orientation
A Value Orientation
Social Utility
A Focus on Social Situations
A Broad Approach
Field Settings
Practical Considerations
Basic Versus Applied Science
Uses of Theory in Applied Work
Roles and Activities
Research
Evaluation
Consultation and Change Agentry
Policy Advice
Management of Organizations
Social Activism
Is Social Psychology Really Applicable?
Has Social Psychology Been Applied?
The Applied Versus Theoretical Conflict
Should Social Psychology Be Experimental?
Can Social Science Influence Public Policy?
Other Responses Concerning Applicability
Problems for Applied Social Psychology
What Is the Evidence?
Is the Evidence Generalizable?
Unintended Consequences
Ethical Issues
Summary
Suggested Readings
All science must be applied science, the goal of which is to
lighten the toil of everyday life.
—Galileo
Social
psychology, briefly defined, is the scientific study of relationships between
people. It develops systematic knowledge about people's beliefs, feelings, and
behavior concerning their social environment, and the effects of their social
environment on them. Despite the goal stated in Galileo's quotation above, much
of psychology (like other sciences) is not applied.
Applied
social psychology, again stated very simply, takes some aspect of the knowledge
base of social psychology and applies it systematically for some social purpose.
The purposes for which social psychological knowledge is used, however, are not
scientifically governed and can be extremely varied—from advancing world peace
to selling iceboxes to Eskimos.
We
will return to a fuller consideration of these definitions shortly. In recent
years applied social psychology has been expanding rapidly on many different
fronts, so let's begin with some examples of its variety.
One
well-known social psychologist, Edwin Hollander, summarized his own
participation in a fascinating variety of projects. Topics of his work have
included attitudes toward the uses of atomic power, opinions about various
political leaders, prediction of long-term job performance, and advice on how
to maintain effective group functioning on lengthy space flights. His atomic
power study was sponsored by UNESCO in order to compare the attitudes of young
people in several different countries, while his report on interpersonal
factors in space flight was of crucial interest to the U.S. authorities in NASA
(Hollander, 1979).
Similarly,
a French applied psychologist, Claude Levy-Leboyer (1988), has described
several of her applied projects. They included helping to design TV
presentations on alcoholism prevention, analyzing reasons for high versus low turnover
of nursing
personnel
in wards of Parisian hospitals, finding and correcting reasons for vandalism to
public phone booths, and determining acceptable schedules for weekend night
work shifts for heavy machine operators in an automobile factory.
Another
social psychologist, Judith Rodin, gave testimony to a U.S. congressional
committee in hearings about the budget of the National Science Foundation.
She
began by stating, "We stand at the threshold of an era where the burdens
of modern society can, in large part, be attributed to problems of
behavior." As examples, she cited problems of energy use, overcrowding of
cities, poverty, and crime, as well as her own research on the causes and
consequences of obesity in humans. She concluded that solutions to these
problems "rely heavily on the scientific study of human behavior"
(Lowman, 1980, p. 160).
Social
psychologists can attack social problems on several different levels. For
instance, on an individual level, the widespread problem of personal shyness
was studied by social psychologist Philip Zimbardo and his colleagues. Their research
culminated in a popular book offering scientifically based advice on how to
overcome shyness (Zimbardo, 1977).
At
the level of group interactions, the social problems of prejudice and racism have
concerned social psychologists for most of this century. Over 40 years ago social
psychological research was cited in the Supreme Court's decision outlawing racial
segregation in schools in the case of Brown v. Board of Education (Cook, 1979).
In subsequent years, many social psychologists have worked toward improving
interracial relations in desegregated schools, for example by introducing
theory-based systems of smallgroup cooperative learning. We will discuss educational
issues of this sort in Chapter 8.
Social
psychologists have also been active at the level of complex organizations.
Notable examples include "team-building" approaches to foster cooperative
work-team efforts toward common goals; innovative organizational procedures such
as "quality circles," participative management, and employee ownership•and
emphasis on humanizing the work environment and improving the "quality of
working life." A critical issue in recent years is how to handle
increasing ethnic, cultural, and gender diversity among organizational members (cf.
Chemers, Oskamp, & Costanzo, 1995). We will discuss some of these applications
of social psychology in organizational settings in both Chapters 9 and 10.
Finally,
in the area of government policy and programs, there has been a wide range of
social science research. There are notable examples concerning programs to fight
poverty, such as the famous negative income tax experiment sponsored by the
Office of Economic Opportunity, programs to improve health care, and educational
innovations such as Head Start (cf. Zigler & Muenchow, 1992). Some of these
are described in more detail in Chapter 17.
A DEFINITION OF APPLIED
SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
After
these examples, let us consider definitional issues more carefully. Defining any
intellectual discipline or field of knowledge is a nebulous endeavor because discipline
boundaries often shift as trends and emphases in the field change. For this
reason the most complete, though not elegant, definitions are ones stated in
terms of what topics are being worked on—for example, defining
"psychology" as "what psychologists do"—because such
definitions are the only ones likely to capture the full range of a field.
For
a more systematic approach, however, we suggest defining applied social
psychology as applications of social psychological methods, theories, principles,
or research findings to understanding or solution of social problems. In adopting
this definition, we should realize that applied work in turn can contribute
fruitfully to fundamental psychological theories, principles, and methods (cf.
Leventhal, 1980). Thus the direction of influence is reciprocal—from basic
psychology to applied work, and from applied psychology back to basic
knowledge.
The
above definition of applied social psychology aims at being inclusive but also focused.
Note that it specifies a problem orientation: Applied work begins with a group
or societal problem, not just with scientific curiosity about some phenomenon.
The definition also implies heavy concentration on field settings—that is, natural
settings where social problems are manifested—though it does not prevent the use
of laboratory experiments when they can help to solve problems. Furthermore, it
suggests serious attention to encouraging social change through the solution of
social problems—a much more activist stance than social psychologists have traditionally
taken. The phrase "solution of social problems" should be interpreted
broadly to include helping groups of people and organizations, as well as
attempting to influence public policy. Finally, the definition includes the use
of social psychological methods, not just theories or research findings—an important
additional aspect of applied work that has often been overlooked or excluded by
more restrictive definitions.
Some
authors would not define applied social psychology so broadly, particularly in
reference to its use
of
methods. It is clear that many of the methods used by social psychologists are
not unique to them but are also used by sociologists, political scientists, economists,
or members of other social science disciplines (for instance, this is true of
survey research, of evaluation research, and of many statistical techniques
such as multiple regression analysis). In fact, social psychology is a field shared
and contributed to by sociologists as well as psychologists. Thus, some of the
methods and the findings discussed in this book can be referred to more broadly
as "applied social science," and some of the work cited here has been
done by researchers from other social science disciplines, not just by people who
would call themselves "psyshologists."
Nevertheless,
where social psychologists have worked productively in a research area, and
where they have used social psychological concepts, principles, and theories to
explain their findings, it seems legitimate to refer to the whole area as
"applied social psychology" and to include the work of other social
scientists in the discussion of findings. Certainly there are enough applied
problems for all of us to work on, so we should not need to fight over the
turf; and it is important to realize that each social science discipline has
some unique contributions to offer to an interdisciplinary problem-solving
approach.
Much
of what is said in this book would apply equally well to applications in other
social science fields, such as applied sociology (see Lazarsfeld & Reitz,
1975). However, psychologists can be differentiated from other disciplines by
typical aspects of their approach, which include rigorous experimental research
methods and emphasis on psychological
processes
and concepts as explanations of obtained findings. A little later in this
chapter we will discuss some of the other typical features of the field.
Divisions of the Field
One
way to divide a discipline is by content areas or topics. Thus applied social
psychology might be divided into content areas such as the environment, mass
media, health and health care, consumer issues, crime and the law, and so on.
Though such divisions may overlook newly emerging areas of the field, they are
usually quite satisfactory as organizational headings, and the third section of
this book has been organized according to such major content areas. An
alternative way to partition a field is by its major types of research methods.
Because use of social psychological research methods is such an important
portion of the field's applications, we have organized the second section of
this book according to that scheme, emphasizing the strengths and weaknesses of
each of five main research approaches.
Another
common way to divide an applied discipline is a three-part one, using the
categories of theory,
research,
and practice. For instance, Ronald J. Fisher (1982) organized his description
of applied social psychology in that way. Moreover, he held that every applied
social psychologist needed to possess skills in all three categories in order
to work effectively on any given type of problem, and that applied
psychologists should not be solely researchers nor solely practitioners. In
keeping with this view, major theories and principles used in applied social
psychology are summarized in Chapter 2 and at many places throughout this
volume. Similarly, examples of practice in creating social change are presented
in many chapters and particularly in the fourth section of the book.
A
different model of applied social psychology was offered by Clara Mayo and Marianne
La France (1980), who stated the goal of the field as improving the quality of
life. This stress on improving the quality of life as the goal of applied
efforts is incorporated in several chapters of this volume.
TYPICAL FEATURES OF APPLIED
SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
How
does applied social psychology differ from other fields of psychology? Starting
from the definition of applied social psychology given above, let's look at
some of its typical features.
A Problem Orientation
Our
definition stated the aim of the field as "understanding or solution of
social problems." This highlights one of the key features of the field: the
fact that it typically begins by focusing on some kind of problem in society. For
example, an applied social psychologist might start with a concern about
violence in our society. From there, he or she might: (a) design a study to
learn more about the phenomenon, or (b) analyze the already available research
knowledge and use it to plan an intervention or social program that would try
to reduce some aspect of violence. In either case, the focus would be on the
problem—violence.
In
contrast, the approach of traditional "basic" science would choose a
topic for study because of its relevance to some theory in the field, and would
focus on finding evidence to support or refute the theory in question. A
traditional basic scientist who chose to study a topic like violence would
typically consider it as a special case of a theoretical concept—for instance,
as an example of aggression within the frustration aggression theory. Basic
scientists would be less likely to plan an intervention to reduce societal violence,
but if they did do so, it would be mainly for the purpose of providing support for
the theory that they were using.
Thus
there may be considerable overlap between the activities of basic and applied
scientists. Both may plan studies to gather new information. Both may stress
theories as ways of understanding a social phenomenon, though that is more apt
to be the central focus of interest of a basic scientist. Both may carry out
interventions designed to change some social phenomenon, but that is more
likely to be the main goal of an applied scientist. The key difference is in
the scientists' goals rather than their activities—are they interested primarily
in developing, supporting, or refuting a theory; or are they hoping to
contribute toward solving a social problem?
Of
course, many studies and many scientists are both basic and applied. The same
woman or man can switch back and forth between the two types of scientific
work. And some studies—often the most valuable ones of all—seem quite clearly to
be both theory oriented and problem-oriented. Such dual-purpose studies provide
good examples of applied research on social problems at the same time that they
contribute new theoretical knowledge about the world we live in.
Further
aspects of theory-oriented versus problem oriented research are discussed in a
very useful chapter by Morton Deutsch (1980).
A Value Orientation
The
claim that conventional science is value-free seeking "knowledge for its
own sake"—has often been challenged (for instance, in the famous appendix on
values in Myrdal's 1944 book, An American Dilemma; also by Ring, 1967).
However, there is no controversy about the status of applied science—it
definitely is not value-free. The definition of some topic as a social
"problem" obviously requires a negative value judgment, which is, in
the final analysis, always a personal one by the investigator. In some
societies and historical periods, killing another person has not been
considered a serious offense, whereas in our society murder is strongly
condemned. Even in our society, some people see the more than 20,000 murders
committed in the United States every year as a social problem requiring strenuous
action, such as handgun registration and control, whereas other people see them
merely as "a price we pay for freedom" (a quote from congressional
testimony by an official of the National Rifle Association).
The
above example illustrates an important point about value orientations. Our
complex, pluralistic society contains many groups, each of which may have its
own typical value system, and these varying systems are often at least
partially incompatible. Similarly, as individuals, we often find ourselves in situations
where two positive values are in conflict and we have to choose between them
(Smith, 1978). The freedom to bear arms versus the desire to reduce murders;
the value of better medical care for developing nations versus the wisdom of investing
those same funds in birth control assistance to reduce overpopulation or in
emergency food supplies to prevent widespread famine—these are value conflicts where
no general consensus is likely. Yet applied social scientists who want to work
on such problem areas must start with a value position that will help them determine
for themselves what circumstances constitute a social problem that needs solving.
A number of social psychologists have offered such guidelines for desirable value
positions for researchers (e.g., Kelman & Warwick, 1978; Opotow, 1990; Smith,
1975, 1976). It is also possible that applied research findings can influence people's
values, as we have seen in the last several decades in changing American
attitudes toward gender equality (cf. Chapter 9).
As
mentioned above, one suggested value standard is improvement of people's
quality of life (Mayo & La France, 1980). This approach embodies a positive
and proactive stance of actively fostering people's overall well-being rather
than merely reacting to the negative aspects of life, which we term social
problems. It also requires applied social scientists to adopt an explicit value
commitment as a basis for their work.
Social Utility
Implicit
in the two features discussed above is the assumption that the knowledge and methods
of social psychology will be useful in achieving social goals.
This
was a cardinal principle of Kurt Lewin (1948), one of the most important
figures in American social psychology. Lewin proposed the concept of action research
to denote scientific work firmly grounded in theory but at the same time
directed toward "resolving social conflicts."
The
theme of social psychology's usefulness was extended by Michael Saks (1978), who
advocated a "high-impact applied social psychology." He proposed that
applied scientists should focus their efforts on the specific aspects of a
social problem where they would have the most impact in resolving it. For
example, an applied researcher might concentrate on trying to prevent crime rather
than to rehabilitate criminals or to console victims of crime. This general
principle can be applied to the choice of problems to study, the selection of
variables to concentrate on within a given problem, the question of whether to
treat a problem's consequences or try to prevent them from occurring in the
first place, and the decision about what kind of interventions to use in
attacking the problem.
KURT
LEWIN, PROPONENT OF A USEFUL PSYCHOLOGY
One
of the most influential founders of modern social psychology, Kurt Lewin was
born in Prussia in 1890. He studied at Freiburg and Munich and earned his Ph.D.
at the University of Berlin in 1914. After serving in the German Army in World
War I, he returned to teach at the University of Berlin.
There
he became a noted member of Berlin's famous school of Gestalt psychology and
later developed his own theoretical approach, termed field theory. After some international
teaching and travels, he left Germany to escape Nazism in 1932 and taught
briefly at Stanford and Cornell before settling at the University of Iowa. In
1944 he moved to M.I.T. to found the Research Center for Group Dynamics, which
was
transferred to the University of Michigan after Lewin's untimely death in 1947.
Lewin
preeminently combined applied and basic scientific interests and activities. He
was noted as a theorist for his development of psychological field theory. Yet he
strongly emphasized the value of applied work on social problems, and he
originated the concept of "action research," combining basic
theoretical research and social action in a coordinated program. He became
famous for studies of democratic and autocratic group leadership methods, group
discussion and decision processes, and group participation methods in
organizational management. He also worked on practical projects to lessen
prejudice and reduce public attitude problems during World War II. His concepts
of "group dynamic" processes were instrumental both in the scientific
study of human groups and in the founding of the National Training Laboratory
at Bethel, Maine, where the "T-group method" was developed as a means
of improving group effectiveness and personal social adjustment.
+
+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + +
+ +
Another
aspect of social utility is that social scientists' work, no matter how important,
must be understandable to others before it will be readily adopted and used. In
order to be seen as relevant and useful, we must simplify all unnecessary
complexity in describing our work, and express it in plain English rather than
technical jargon. In addition, we should try to communicate to the general
public through nontechnical journals, the mass media, and popular lectures or workshops
(Posavac, 1992).
A Focus on Social Situations
A
characteristic that both basic and applied social psychology share is emphasis
on the power of social situations to affect people's beliefs, feelings, and
behavior.
This
emphasis on the power of situations is in contrast to some other areas of
psychology, which tend to stress the importance of physiological factors or
personality characteristics. For instance, a prominent concept in social psychology
is the fundamental attributional error—the common tendency to underestimate the
situational causes of other people's behavior and exaggerate the dispositional or
personality causes (see Chapter 2 for more details).
In
short, social psychologists usually think of situational factors as the most important
ones in influencing behavior.
A Broad Approach
To
be as useful as possible, applied social psychology needs to be comprehensive
and inclusive in its approach to social problems. This means, for instance,
considering the whole range of variables that might influence a particular area
of concern. This may also be referred to as a macro rather than micro level of
analysis. In studying a topic such as expression of racial prejudice, for
example, we need to know not only individual attitudes and experiences and
immediate stimulus events, but also the social norms and expectations people have
learned during their lives, and the characteristics of the overall social system
within which they live (Lott & Maluso, 1995; see Chapter 9). Often such
considerations will lead to an interdisciplinary approach, in
which
sociological, economic, and political factors are considered in addition to
psychological ones.
Field Settings
It
is clear that applied social psychologists are more inclined than most psychologists
to do research in field settings—that is, natural settings where people live,
work, or play, and consequently feel comfortable and behave in their usual ways.
This naturalness is in marked contrast to the artificial atmosphere of most
laboratory experiments.
In
traditional social psychology the well-controlled laboratory experiment has been
the research method of choice. As we will see in Chapter 4, this approach
allows a few variables to be selected for study out of the multitude of factors
that may influence a phenomenon of interest, and these variables can then be
carefully controlled or manipulated, and their effects can be precisely measured.
In contrast to controlled experiments, another common research tradition in
social psychology has been the use of nonmanipulative or correlational methods,
which will be discussed in Chapter 5.
Though
correlational studies can be conducted in field settings, they are most often
done in classroom settings, using undergraduates as the research subjects
(Sears, 1986).
Thus,
in typical social psychological research the use of field settings, though often
advocated, has been largely absent. During the 1960s and 1970s, studies done in
field settings constituted no more than 10% of the articles appearing in the
major social psychological journals (Fried, Gumpper, & Allen, 1973;
Helmreich, 1975; Mark, Cook, & Diamond, 1975). Even the Journal of Applied
Social Psychology gave only about 25% of its space to field studies at that
time. These patterns have continued, only slightly abated, in the mainline social
psychological journals. However, by the 1990s, the balance of articles in the
Journal of Applied Social Psychology had shifted to about 60% field studies
(Schultz & Butler, 1996).
In
addition, many of the field studies of applied social psychologists are published,
not in general social psychological journals, but in more specialized journals.
These journals may focus solely on evaluation research or on particular content
areas, such as criminal justice, health care, the environment, the mass media,
educational research, consumer psychology, or organizational research. Each of
these areas of research will be taken up at length in later chapters of this
book.
Practical Considerations
Applied
social psychology, much more than traditional social science disciplines, has
to pay attention to practical considerations. To start with, much applied research
is done in response to the needs or formal requests of a client or sponsoring
agency. As a result, it
must
often be conducted under severe time constraints in order to be useful to the
client or sponsor. These characteristics are quite different from the typical
course of basic research in academic settings, in which investigators have relatively
complete freedom to choose the topic, method, and pace of their work.
For
research results to be applicable in solving social problems, they must first
be strong enough to have
practical
importance. Second, it is important for them to generalize to other
situations—that is, to different tasks, measuring instruments, research subjects,
organizations, and subcultures. We will return to these issues near the end of
this chapter.
Another
practical consideration that has received increasing attention is cost-benefit
comparisons. Even if research results are strong and broadly generalizable,
they may be too expensive to implement in a practical social program. For
instance, the U.S. national 55 mph speed limit, initiated during the energy
shortages of the 1970s, was shown to have saved many lives and much gasoline, yet
many truckers and motorists strenuously objected to it because they felt its
benefits did not compensate for its personal costs to them. As a result, the
national 55 mph limit was finally terminated in 1996.
In
recent years the computation of benefit-to-cost ratios has become common in decision
making about governmental programs and industrial and commercial investments. This
approach requires applied social scientists to develop quantitative estimates
about both the costs and the expected benefits of social programs that they hope
to implement. Since program benefits are often previously unquantified concepts
like job satisfaction or improved mental health, workers in the field have had
to develop and test new techniques for estimating their dollar value. An
interesting example is the calculation that every dollar spent on high-quality
preschool programs would save the nation seven dollars in later costs for special
education, school dropouts, welfare, delinquency, and other social problems
(Barnett, 1992).
A
final practical factor that applied scientists must consider is the political
feasibility of programs. For example, every year in the U.S., handguns are used
in approximately 15,000 of the nation's 20,000 murders and in half a million other
crimes. Research results have shown that major handgun registration and control
laws could decrease these totals significantly (e.g., Podell & Archer,
1994). For decades, a large majority of the U.S. population, even including gun
owners, has been in favor of stricter handgun control laws (Moore &
Newport, 1994). Yet, with the exception of Massachusetts, New York, and
Washington, D.C., most states and cities found it politically impossible to
pass handgun control laws until very recently. The reason is that very powerful
lobbying organizations, such as the National Rifle Association, have shot down
all other previous attempts to pass gun restriction or gun control laws (Kleck,
1991; Schumer, 1993). Other kinds of social programs may also be politically
unfeasible at a given time because of strongly entrenched opposition by
powerful political groups or individuals.
Basic versus Applied Science
The
above distinctions between "basic" and applied scientific approaches,
as well as other_related points, have been summarized in a shorthand form by Bickman
(1981—see Table 1-1). As the table shows, the differences in purposes and
activities between applied and basic science also produce differences in their
context, methodologies, and participants.
USES OF THEORY IN APPLIED WORK
In
view of all the practical considerations of applied work, what good are
theories to the applied social scientist? Isn't applied scientific work often
described as nontheoretical? Aren't theories often contrasted with practical
reality, and theorists referred to as impractical dreamers? Yes, but these
criticisms overlook the very real value of theories in everyday life as well as
in science.
"Practical"
people operate, whether they know it or not, on the basis of principles that
were first suggested and later verified by theorists. The "simple"
act of driving a car, for example, involves the use of many scientific principles,
including gravity, centrifugal force, rolling friction, inertia, peripheral
vision, human reaction time, and size constancy of objects. Even though we may
not think about the concepts themselves, we still depend on our essential
understanding of them to get us safely home.
In
science, theories have several important functions:
1.
Theories provide the ideas that guide our steps in research.
2.
Like the map of an unfamiliar city, theories help us understand the findings of
research. They provide a con-text into which we can place demonstrated facts,
see how and why they fit together, and note where they are inconsistent and
need further exploration.
3.
Theories give us a basis for predicting what will happen in the future under a
given set of conditions. In turn, such predictions provide one of the best
tests of a theory's adequacy.
4.
Valid theories help us control events. They specify the variables or conditions
we must control or manipulate to develop programs or interventions that will be
likely to reach our goals.
Just
as in basic scientific research, valid theories are also useful in solving
social problems. Fifty years ago Kurt Lewin stressed this point in a famous
quotation:
Many
psychologists working today in an applied field are keenly aware of the need
for close cooperation between theoretical and applied psychology. . . .There is
nothing so practical as a good theory. (1944/1951, p. 169)
Scientific
theories vary widely in their scope or range of applicability. A few, such as
the theory of relativity in physics, are broad, encompassing a tremendous range
of phenomena. Psychological theories don't have such a broad scope; psychoanalysis
may come closest to this extreme. At the other extreme are mini-theories
dealing with a very limited set of events and circumstances. One example in social
psychology is Latane and Darley's (1970) theory of diffusion of responsibility among
bystanders in emergency situations. Between the broad, general theories and the
mini-theories are many midrange psychological theories with varying degrees of
scope—for instance, cognitive dissonance, learned helplessness, or social
exchange theories. Any or all of these diverse theories can be useful to the
applied social scientist who is trying to solve a practical problem, such as
group morale, juvenile delinquency, or public health. In the following chapter
we will present more extensive illustrations of social psychological theories
and concepts that can be used in dealing with applied problems.
ROLES AND ACTIVITIES
Turning
from the use of theories and other typical features of applied social
psychology, let's consider the possible roles of people working in this field.
We'll discuss them roughly in the order from traditional to newly developing
roles and activities.
Research
The
traditional role of scientists has been to do research. In applied social
psychology, at least as much
as
in other scientific fields, many unanswered questions still require
investigation. In addition to the collection of empirical data, there are
several other aspects of the research role. Scientific scholarship often
involves searching through many scattered sources in the literature in order to
find relevant facts and hypotheses.
It
may also involve culling these facts, integrating conflicting information, and building
theories about the topic. An occasional aspect of the research role is serving
as an expert witness before courts or legislative committees. All of these
aspects of research will continue to be a key function of applied social
psychologists.
Evaluation
Evaluation
is another aspect of research, but it has been growing by leaps and bounds, and
it merits separate discussion. Like other scientists, evaluation researchers
frequently state hypotheses and collect and analyze systematic data to support
or reject them. However, they have specialized topics of study—the success or
failure of particular experimental interventions or social programs.
In
the last 25 years, the laws or regulations initiating many government programs have
mandated an
evaluation
research component to help determine the program's success or failure. There
has also been an increasing demand for evaluation of other kinds of programs, such
as businesses' capital investment decisions. In these activities, there are
important roles both for outside evaluators and for employees of the
organization who function as inside evaluators. We will describe evaluation
research in detail in Chapter 7.
Consultation and Change Agentry
Another
kind of role for applied social psychologists is consultation with organizations,
aimed at accomplishing desired changes in their operational methods or results.
Often these organizations are businesses, or they may be civic organizations or
government agencies.
Such
organizations hire many different kinds of consultants—lawyers, financial advisors,
and advertising agents—as well as applied social scientists, who can help them
apply the theories and findings of psychology and sociology to their organizational
goals and problems. Social science consultants operate under many different names:
organization development (OD) specialists; management consultants; marketing,
communications, public relations, job training, and personnel selection experts.
If they try to apply social science principles, we will refer to them as social
science consultants.
In
a useful classificatory scheme, Hornstein (1975) described several types of
consultants. Among the factors considered in his scheme were the kind of
service the change agent provides to the client organization, the level in the organization
at which the consultant works, and the kinds of targets the consultant tries to
change (external relationships, internal organizational processes, or the
personal functioning of individuals).
In
addition to a first type of change agent (the "outside pressure"
type), which we will discuss shortly, Hornstein distinguished the following
basic types:
(2)
the people change (PC) technology type who works to change individual
functioning in organizations through such techniques as sensitivity training, behavior
modification and need achievement training;
(3)
the organization development (OD) type who works to improve a system's problem-solving
capabilities by changing the norms and values regulating behavior; (4) the
analysis for the top (AFT) type who works primarily with business and
government units to improve efficiency and output and employs analytic procedures
to develop expert advice. (1975, pp. 218-219)
Policy Advice
Another
role for applied social psychologists is to provide policy advice. The
recipients may be either public, governmental agencies, or civic or business organizations
of many different types. Hornstein's type-4 change agent actually fits best
under the heading of a policy advisor. That is, such a person simply gives
advice to organizational managers, rather than working in or on the
organization to bring about change. Of course, policy advice must be based on a
study of the particular organization or agency, as well as on application of
general scientific principles and findings. But it does not involve direct
attempts to change the organization's processes or personnel.
Policy
science has become a popular term for the attempt to make findings from all the
social sciences relevant to governmental and organizational policymaking. An influential
book on this subject (Weiss, 1977b) gave examples of social science research
bearing on policies for the public schools, judicial procedures, health care, congressional
decisions, and State Department diplomacy. A particularly fascinating example
of policy advice was a study in which Hammond and Adelman (1976), using research
on human judgment, public opinion measurement techniques, and analysis of expert
ballistic information, helped the city council of Denver decide what kind of
bullets their police officers should be authorized to use.
In
Chapter 17 of this book we will consider at length how successful social scientists
have been in offering policy advice, how they can be more successful in the
future, and what pitfalls and dangers there are for social scientists in giving
policy advice.
Management of Organizations
Usually
a social scientist's most influential role is offering advice to organizational
managers. Occasionally, however, applied scientists become managers themselves.
Particularly in this era of limited academic job possibilities, many more
social scientists are taking jobs in business organizations or in government
agencies, where they may eventually assume management responsibilities. In such
situations, of course, they do not shed all their scientific knowledge and
skills, and one hopes that they use their scientific background to perform relevant
aspects of their job better than they could without that training.
Thus,
though it is an unusual activity for social scientists, management is a
legitimate role for them. A
few
examples of psychologists who attained high government positions are: John Gardner,
Secretary of Health, Education, and Welfare under President Johnson; Richard
Atkinson, head of the National Science Foundation under President Carter and more
recently president of the 9-campus University of California system; and clinical
psychologist Leonard Haber, who was elected mayor of Miami Beach. Numerous
social scientists have reached high positions in business and educational organizations,
such as Judith Rodin, president of the University of Pennsylvania.
A
particularly unusual example was Pat Carrigan, who became the first woman and
the first psychologist to manage a General Motors auto assembly plant (Cordes,
1982b).
Social Activism
Whereas
managers work on the inside of organizations, social activists typically try to
influence them from outside. Though some social activists attempt to work from
the inside to change organizations, that is usually a difficult (and often
short-lived) position. A more typical kind of social activist is Homstein's
(1975) first type of change agent:
(1)
the outside pressure (OP) type who works to change systems from the outside
through the application of pressure using such tactics as mass demonstration,
civil disobedience, and violence. (p. 218)
There
are also social activists who apply outside pressure through less extreme
techniques such as legislative lobbying, media publicity, legal suits,
grassroots organizing, and effective marshaling of research evidence to get
organizations and government agencies to change their ways. An outstanding
example of this sort is Ralph Nader, whose organization called Public Citizen
has spawned other social action groups in the fields of consumer affairs,
environmental protection, and health care. Nader himself, like many other
activist group organizers, is a lawyer, but many of the staff members who work
in his several organizations are applied scientists—some in research roles, and
some in social activist roles. In Chapter 16 we will discuss the role of the
social activist at length.
++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++
JUDITH RODIN, AN APPLIED PSYCHOLOGIST
The
first woman to be named president of an ivy league university, Judith Rodin has
headed the University of Pennsylvania since 1994. She earned her B.A. at that
university and her Ph.D. in social psychology at Columbia under Stanley Schachter.
She taught briefly at New York University before moving in 1972 to Yale, where she
served successively as a psychology faculty member, department chair, dean, and
provost.
Rodin's
early research focused on the topic of obesity, and later expanded to many
aspects of human health and behav ior. She has headed an international research
network studying health and behavior and written over 200 articles and ten
books, including the chapter on applied social psychology in the Handbook of Social
Psychology and Body Traps, which examines the role of physical appearance in women's
psychological health. She has been honored by election to the American Academy of
Arts and Sciences and the National Academy of Sciences, and has served on President
Clinton's Committee
of Advisors on Science and Technology.
++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++
IS SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY REALLY APPLICABLE?
In
view of the impressive breadth of social psychological applications cited
above, it may astonish you to learn that there has been controversy about
whether social psychology can be or should be applied. We will sketch a few of
the key issues and responses here.
Has Social Psychology Been Applied?
One
aspect of the challenge to social psychology's applicability is the question of
how much it has in fact
been
applied. This question was examined in an early paper by Leo Meltzer (1972). At
that time he noted that, though there was much potentially applicable research,
relatively little of it had been used to design specific programs aimed at
changing real-world problems. In order to include such programs in his list of
successful applications, he also required that they had to be based on both
well-established social psychological theory and clear empirical findings, and
that theirresults must have been carefully evaluated. These were such stringent
criteria that he found few studies which qualified. However, he concluded:
Much
of the literature of traditional social psychology... is not only applicable to
effecting changes in the real world, but has actually been effectively applied.
The point is that there is a huge depository in our discipline's literature of applicable
findings. The discipline has power. (Meltzer, 1972, p. 18)
Since
that time, many more social psychological findings have moved from the potentially
applicable status to actually being applied. Other authors have pointed out the
wide range of areas in which social science knowledge has been effectively
applied by businesses and government (e.g., Tornatzky et al., 1982).
The Applied Versus Theoretical Conflict
There
seems to be something of an identity problem within the field, for relatively
few psychologists identifies themselves primarily as applied psychologists,
even if they are mainly doing applied work. In part, this may be due to the
lower prestige which is often attached to applied work within the field, as
compared to "basic" or theoretical work. In addition, it undoubtedly
stems from divisions between the separate content areas of the field, for many
applied psychologists identify themselves by the topic area in which they
work—for example, as organizational, educational, consumer, or health
psychologists.
The
split between applied social psychology and basic or theoretical social
psychology has received much attention. Morton Deutsch (1975) traced the split
back to a conflict between two groups of Kurt Lewin's followers that erupted
shortly before Lewin's death in
1947.
The practitioners among Lewin's disciples concentrated on propagating new
techniques for working with groups—called T-group methods, or sensitivity
training—and they deemphasized empirical data collection. In contrast, the post-Lewinian
researchers tended to retire to their laboratories and to give up work on
important social problems. Though there were offsetting events, such as the
1936 founding of the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues (SPSSI)
by socially involved researchers, including Lewin himself, the split between
applied and research oriented social psychologists widened during the 1950s and
1960s.
This
applied-theoretical split in social psychology has been lamented by many
authors (e.g., Helmreich, 1975). More recently, there are some signs of greater
integration of these two aspects of the field, for instance in large-scale multivariate
research that captures more of the complexity of the natural world, and in the
tendency for major researchers to work in both theoretical and applied areas.
Should Social Psychology Be Experimental?
As
mentioned earlier, the dominant research approach in American social psychology,
at least since the 1940s, was the laboratory experiment. Surveys of the
literature in the 1960s and 1970s showed that between 70% and 90% of the
studies published in major social psychological journals were laboratory experiments
(Fried et al., 1973; Helmreich, 1975; Higbee & Wells, 1972). Applied social
psychology research also included many experiments, though not as high a
proportion. By the 1990s, only 35% of articles appearing in the Journal of Applied
Social Psychology were laboratory experiments (Schultz & Butler, 1996).
Alongside their advantages of precise measurement and control of variables, laboratory
experiments have the serious disadvantages of artificiality, weak effects, and
considering only a few variables at a time. These disadvantages have led some
social psychologists to call for their deemphasis as a tool of our field, or
even their abandonment.
Suggestions
for how to replace the laboratory experiment have taken several forms. The
first suggestion was to move out of the laboratory into the field, where theory-oriented
experimental research could be conducted in "real-world" settings (e.g.,
Bickman & Henchy, 1972). Others recommended placing more reliance on
large-scale, multivariate, correlational studies in place of experimental
research (e.g., McGuire, 1973).
The
most extreme suggestion has been to almost entirely abandon the experimental
method (e.g., Silverman, 1977; Wallach & Wallach, 1994). Beyond the common
criticisms of artificiality and triviality in laboratory experiments, these critics
have pointed out serious problems in the kind of naturalistic field experiments
that have been advocated by many social psychologists. Even in real-life situations,
Silverman argued, for ethical reasons we can use only weak manipulations, for
brief periods of time, and thus can study only trivial questions. He concluded
that, from such studies: attempts to generalize to the issues that spawned the
research are folly. The ongoing molar phenomena of social development and behavior
are as unreachable for the psychologist by the experimental method as the
movements of the planets are for the astronomer. And social psychology can only
begin to grow into an authentic discipline when we abandon the experiment as a modus
operandi. (Silverman, 1977, p. 356)
Certainly
this is an extreme view, going further in its criticisms than would most social
psychologists. Despite the admitted limitations of experiments, many
psychologists still consider them the research method of choice, or at least a
key part of the researcher's tool kit.
In
this debate about the usefulness of experimental methods, this text adopts a
middle position. The critics are correct in saying that laboratory experiments
are often trivial and may not generalize to real-world situations, but the
careful control and crucial comparisons of randomized experiments still provide
the most dependable answers to scientific questions. Even "trivial and
artificial" experiments can help to establish scientific principles of
social behavior (Calder, Phillips, & Tybout, 1981; Schaller et al., 1995).
And, particularly when they are large in scale and conducted in natural
situations, experiments can overcome some of their usual limitations and give
us useful and generalizable knowledge about real-world behavior. Yet
experiments alone are not enough, for they are often impossible for practical or
ethical reasons. Therefore, other research methods, such as quasi-experiments, correlational
methods, descriptive observation, and archival data analysis, are also important
scientific tools. We will consider examples of all of these techniques,
together with their strengths and limitations, in Chapters 3 through 7.
Can Social Science Influence Public Policy?
A
final question about social psychology's applicability is whether it can or
does affect policy decisions,
even
in situations where it has relevant knowledge to offer. Or, on the other hand,
do personal, practical, political, or ethical considerations prevent the use of
established social science knowledge in forming public policy, and if so, where
and why? This important question should be kept in mind, but it would be
premature to try to answer it at this point. We will consider it in detail in
Chapter 17.
Other Responses Concerning Applicability
In
addition to the above discussion of social psychology's applicability, let us
look at some other viewpoints supporting the effort to make social psychology
relevant to important social issues. An early proponent of this view was George
Miller (1969) who, as president of the American Psychological Association,
called for its members to "give psychology away" to all who could
benefit from its knowledge and methods. Shortly thereafter, a large-scale
survey of psychologists clearly demonstrated their beliefs that psychology could
be applied constructively to the social problems of our society, but that too
little had yet been done to achieve relevance (Lipsey, 1974). A recent survey
of members and leaders of the American Psychological Association confirmed that
applications of psychology are still considered to be among the most important
issues for the profession (Oakland, 1994).
Another
type of answer is to reverse the applicability challenge and assert that social
psychology cannot not be applied. This answer highlights the universal tendency
for social decision-makers, like all human beings, to act on what they think
they know about human behavior. Though individuals' supposed knowledge may be
incorrect or incomplete, they will use it in making decisions about policies or
programs, hiring employees, planning social gatherings, or whatever. The
resulting social actions are largely based on the "knowledge" of the
time (for instance, that the world is flat, or that frustration leads to aggression).
As one example, the U.S. Supreme Court explicitly cited social science knowledge
in its 1954 decision requiring desegregation of public schools, but in 1896 it
had also relied on the purported "knowledge" of the time—that Black
Americans were socially inferior to Whites—in its separate-but-equal-facilities
decision in the case of Plessy v. Ferguson.
The
point of this argument is that, whether social psychologists try to apply their
science or not, many of their findings will inevitably be disseminated (perhaps
in distorted forms) by the popular media and used in one way or another by
people who have heard of them.
Many
social psychologists have concluded that, since research knowledge will often
be used in some form by social planners and practitioners, social scientists
should take an active part in disseminating their findings and trying to direct
their application in relevant areas of social life (e.g., Shippee, 1979).
Though
the major journals in social psychology continue to publish mostly theoretically
oriented laboratory research, many other journals feature applied research in their
special fields, such as community psychology, law and criminal justice, or health
psychology. Also, the traditional federal agencies that provide research funds
have become more receptive to applied research projects, and many single-mission
agencies such as the National Institute of Justice (NIJ) have been established,
which provide support for a wide variety of applied social research. Finally, a
number of graduate schools have developed programs that emphasize applied
aspects of social psychology, and their graduates are increasingly finding jobs
in nontraditional positions in community agencies, contract research firms, industrial
companies, or government organizations.
PROBLEMS FOR APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
Many
problems are implicit in the typical features and roles of applied social
psychology. Here we will discuss the following major areas of potential
problems in the field: the research evidence, the generalizability of the evidence,
unintended consequences, and ethical questions.
What Is the Evidence?
A
key problem in applying any science is to assess the available scientific
knowledge. It is not enough to have theories, for there must be firm supportive
evidence that the theories are correct before we can feel safe in using them to
build a bridge or design a social program. We need to ask: What studies have
been done? What did they show? How were the data obtained? Unfortunately,
methods used in the social sciences sometimes are not appropriate to support
the conclusions drawn, and in other cases the methods are appropriate but not
powerful enough to produce an effect. If the methods are acceptable, we should
ask: How strong are the results? Are the effects large enough to be of
practical importance as well as being statistically significant? If so, we have
satisfied the criterion of internal validity of the research (Campbell &
Stanley, 1966; Cook & Shadish, 1994).
Is the Evidence Generalizable?
The
next question, and a major problem for applied social science, is the external
validity or generalizability of the research findings. If research findings are
to be applied, we must know under what circumstances the findings will hold true
and what other circumstances will produce different results. In this regard the
extremely artificial conditions in many laboratory experiments may limit the
generalizability of their findings to other situations (though the extent to
which they do is an empirical question).
In
engineering, the physical principles that apply to building a bridge in one
nation apply equally to building a bridge somewhere else. But this is often not
the case in social science. Some social psychological principles are applicable
in most nations or cultures, but other findings are limited to the culture,
subculture, or setting in which they were obtained (Heller, 1990). For
instance, the effect of an absent father on children's development may be very
different in a cultural milieu featuring strong extended family networks (such as
many Black and Hispanic groups) than in subcultures where the nuclear family
has no such support networks. Clearly, a social program to compensate for
absent fathers would have different implications and chances of success in
these two cultural settings.
In
addition to cultural patterns, individual personal characteristics and task demands
often produce research findings that are not broadly generalizable. A prominent
example is in research on jury decision making. Much of this research has used
"mock juries" composed of college students, who had about half an
hour to read a written summary of a trial transcript and then gave their
individual vote on the defendant's guilt or innocence, without any group
discussion or deliberation. Obviously, there are so many differences between
this situation and the typical courtroom trial that there is no way of knowing
whether such research results will apply to real-life trial situations or not.
Fortunately, some investigators have been sensitive to these limitations and
have devised various methods to increase or at least check the generality of
their jury research findings. We will discuss this topic at greater length in
Chapter 15, which deals with legal issues.
Unintended Consequences
Even
if research is internally valid and is applicable to the social situation in which
intervention is planned, unintended consequences of the intervention may arise.
One notable case of this sort was a study in which aged residents of retirement
homes were visited on various schedules by college students. After the study
was completed and the visits ceased, the groups of oldsters who had initially benefited
from the visits exhibited "precipitous declines" in their mental and physical
health (Schulz & Hanusa, 1978).
Another
example of unintended consequences stemming from social programs is seen in various
crime-control programs which have been successful in decreasing crime rates in
one locality, while apparently raising crime rates in surrounding communities
by driving many criminals there. On the other hand, the intended consequences of
interventions are not always evident without careful research. For example, some
crime-control or educational-improvement programs may seem to have little
success, whereas comparison of their results with appropriate control groups
would show that the apparent small improvement was distinctly better than the worsening
conditions in other communities (cf. Lipsey & Wilson, 1993).
HERBERT KELMAN, SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY'S CONSCIENCE
Born
in Vienna, Austria, in 1927, Herbert Kelman fled there with his family in 1939
to escape Nazi persecution. After a year as a refugee in Belgium, he arrived in
the United States, and by 1947 he had earned two bachelor's degrees. He
completed his Ph.D. in social psychology at Yale in 1951 and then did full-time
research for several years. After five years on the Harvard University faculty,
he moved to the University of Michigan, but returned to Harvard in 1968 as the
holder of an endowed chair in social ethics.
The
major themes of Kelman's research and writing have been international
relations, conflict resolution and peace research, and ethics in social science
research. He helped found the Journal of Conflict Resolution, edited a research
volume on International Behavior, and has been intensely involved in programs
of action research applying conflict resolution principles to workshops
bringing together Arab and Israeli intellectual and governmental leaders. In
the area of ethics, he has written or coauthored several volumes on human
values and social research, obedience to authority, and The Ethics of Social Intervention.
Among his many honors, Kelman has been elected president of two divisions of
the American Psychological Association (APA), and six other psychological
organizations, including the Peace Science Society, the Interamerican Society
of Psychology, and the International Society of Political Psychology. He has
also received the Kurt Lewin Award, the APA award for Psychology in the Public Interest,
and the SocioPsychological Prize of the American Association for the Advancement
of Science.
+
+ + + + + ++ + +++ + + + + + + + + ++ + + + ++ + + + + + + + + ++ + + ++
The
problem of unintended consequences often goes unnoticed because, if evaluation research
is done at all, it is usually limited to the research participants or, at most,
to the community in which the research was carried out. This fact highlights the
importance, not only of appropriate control groups and research designs, but also
of an even broader systems approach to understanding the effects of social
programs. Social interventions are not discrete, isolated programs; they are
embedded in a whole social system of related events and processes. As such,
they are bound to have some consequences beyond their stated goals and target
populations, and applied social scientists need to recognize and consider these
possible unplanned effects.
Ethical Issues
The
unintended consequences of social programs or research activities raise ethical
issues. But beyond the problem of unintended consequences, many ethical issues
can arise in applied social science, and applied scientists need to be
constantly alert to avoid them if possible or, if not, at least to minimize their
impact.
Unfortunately,
the solutions to ethical issues are usually not clear-cut because they often
involve conflicting. values and differing perceptions—for instance, what social
situations are serious enough to constitute a "social problem"?
A
fundamental question at the outset of any applied project is whether or not to
intervene in the given social situation. The issue is not just a pragmatic one
of whether we have useful methods or knowledge, but also an ethical one of
whether they should be used. If we decide to participate in the project, then
we will have to be ready for a later decision about when and how to terminate
the intervention. "Intervention entails both taking responsibility and
letting go of it" (Mayo & La France, 1980, p. 91).
There
are many different ways to intervene in a social system, and they have differing
ethical implications. Kelman and Warwick (1978) suggested a useful typology of kinds
of interventions, based on the amount of power or control left in the hands of
the people affected by the proposed social program. Its categories range from coercion,
through manipulation and persuasion, to facilitation of others' own goals.
It
is important to consider the ethics of intervention in each specific social
situation that arises. A thoughtful volume on this topic, edited by Bermant,
Kelman, and Warwick (1978), discussed ethics in a wide range of social
interventions, including behavior modification, encounter groups, organization development
programs, community education programs, community disputes, income maintenance experiments,
federally funded housing programs, and family planning programs. Other papers
or books have suggested ethical standards for human research in general
(Kimmel, 1988; Sieber, 1992), for organizational research (Mirvis &
Seashore, 1979), evaluation research (Newman & Brown, 1996; Shadish et al.,
1995), community research with street people (Sieber & Sorensen, 1992), and
psychological intervention in the criminal justice system (Monahan, 1980).
Though
many of the above areas are relatively new ones, general ethical guidelines can
be applied. The American Psychological Association has had a strong ethical
code ever since 1953, and it has been updated every few years to include new
concerns and developing areas of research and practice. For applicable
formulations, see publications by the American Psychological Association (1982,
1992) and Box 1-4. A volume edited by Bersoff (1995) has elaborated on many specific
topics of ethical concern. However, there are still numerous unresolved ethical
issues in applied scientific work, especially in the newly developing field of policy
science, which we will discuss in Chapter 17.
Starting
in the mid-1960s, various branches of the federal government adopted ethical
regulations to control research and experimental treatment programs with human participants
(e.g., U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1971). These
regulations were proposed and adopted mainly because of concerns over
biomedical research done without adequate safeguards to protect research
subjects from possible harmful effects—for example, injection of senile
patients with live cancer cells for research purposes, without their understanding
what was being done.
Though
the government ethical regulations also apply to psychological research, in
most cases the potential risks of such studies are minimal compared to medical
research with new drugs and unproved procedures.
That
fact was officially recognized in 1981, when the federal regulations governing psychological
research were greatly simplified and redirected toward the few studies that do
carry potential risks for the participants (Fields, 1981).
Box 1-4
EXAMPLES OF ETHICAL GUIDELINES FOR
PSYCHOLOGISTS
The
following are very brief extracts from the latest revision of the Ethical
Principles of Psychologists (American Psychological Association, 1992). In the
complete document each of these general principles is elaborated, and they are
followed by eight sections on ethical standards, including over 100 subsections
stating specific ethical responsibilities -(e.g., avoiding harm, explaining
assessment results, avoiding false statements, avoiding sexual intimacies,
maintaining confidentiality, accuracy in teaching, compliance with law, reporting
ethical violations).
Preamble. Psychologists work to develop
a valid and reli able body of scientific knowledge based on research. . .and where
appropriate, to apply it pragmatically to improve the condition of both the
individual and society. . . .Psychologists dents, respect and protect human and
civil rights and do not knowingly participate in or condone unfair
discriminatory practices.
A.
Competence. Psychologists strive to maintain high standards of competence in
their work. They recognize the boundaries of their particular competencies and
the limitations of their expertise. They provide only those services and use
only those techniques for which they are qualified by education, training, or
experience.
B.
Integrity. Psychologists seek to promote integrity in the science, teaching,
and practice of psychology. . . .In describing or reporting their
qualifications, services, products, fees,
research, or teaching, they do not make statements that are false, misleading,
or deceptive. . . .Psychologists avoid improper and potentially harmful dual
relationships.
C.
Professional and Scientific Responsibility.
Psychologists
uphold professional and scientific standards of conduct, clarify their professional roles and
obligations, accept appropriate responsibility for their behavior, and adapt
their methods to the needs of different populations.
D.
Respect for People's Rights and Dignity. Psychologists accord appropriate
respect to the fundamental rights, dignity, and worth of all people. They
respect the rights of people to privacy, confidentiality, self-determination,
and autonomy. .
E.
Concern for Others' Welfare. Psychologists seek to contribute to the welfare of
those with whom they interact, professionally. In their professional actions,
psychologists weigh the welfare and rights of their patients or clients, student
supervisees, human research participants, and other affected persons, and the
welfare of animal subjects of research. . . .they do not exploit or mislead
other people during or after professional relationships.
F.
Social Responsibility. Psychologists are aware of their professional and
scientific responsibilities to the community and the society in which they work
and live. They apply and make public their knowledge of psychology in order to
contribute to human welfare. Psychologists are concerned about and work to
mitigate the causes of human suffering. When undertaking research, they strive
to advance human welfare and the science of psychology. Psychologists try to
avoid misuse of their work.
Source: American Psychological Association, 1992.
Ethical Principles of Psychologists and
Code of Conduct. Copyright 0 1992 by the American
Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission
++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++++
Regardless
of what the federal regulations may be, applied scientists should feel
ethically accountable for their activities. That means that there must be some
basic ultimate standard for deciding what activities are acceptable. What might
such an ethical guideline be?
One
common suggestion has been the welfare of the client (individuals or groups). But
is that a sufficient guideline? Questions such as the following have been
raised about it (Deutsch, 1975, p. 10):
·
Does a social psychologist have any moral responsibilities with respect to how
a client uses research information the psychologist has collected?
·
Should a psychologist allow a client's public distortion of research findings
to go unchallenged?
·
Should a client be allowed to use research information to influence third
parties without their consent (e.g., to influence voters, consumers, or
employees)?
Similar
questions have been raised about how to handle situations when organizations
misuse information or resist social interventions (Ballard, Brosz, &
Parker, 1980). Deutsch (1975) has answered these questions by proposing that the
ultimate standard should not be the welfare of the client (though that is
important), but the general well-being of humankind.
That
seems an excellent standard to keep in mind, though of course applying it may
be a complex and difficult process.
Unfortunately,
in many cases applied social science has failed to follow the guideline of the
welfare of humankind. For example, we tend to define social problems in terms of
the behavior of individual people, rather than focusing on the environmental
situations that help to produce and maintain that behavior. This
person-centered approach often leads us to "blame the victims" (e.g.,
of racism or poverty) for their own misfortunes (Dressel, Carter, &
Balachandran, 1995; Herbert & Dunkel-Schetter, 1992).
Specific
Ethical Precepts. The various ethical
codes that have been developed are in relative agreement on a number of
specific precepts that applied social scientists should observe. There is not
enough space here to discuss them, but we will list the major points, together
with useful references for further reading.
·
Social scientists should avoid harmful consequences to research participants or
clients (Warwick, 1982).
·
All research participants should be told enough about the research and its
likely impact on them so that they can give meaningful informed consent to participate
(Murray, 1982).
·
Clients and research subjects should not undergo any unusual invasion of privacy
(Kelman, 1977; Murray, 1982).
·
Any proposed deception of clients or research participants should be strictly
reviewed by ethics committees before it is carried out, and it should be
limited to cases where it is essential to the accomplishment of a highly
desirable goal (Christensen, 1988; Elms, 1982).
Research
participants should be debriefed soon after the research is completed in order
to remove any deception, inform them about the research, and allay any
remaining anxieties (cf. Christensen, 1988; Aronson, Brewer, & Carlsmith,
1985).
In
later chapters on specific content areas, we will consider further the ethical
issues that are prominent in each area.
SUMMARY
The
many fascinating examples of the application of social psychology to practical
problems range from individual concerns, such as conquering shyness, through group
and organizational problems, to issues of governmental policy. A suggested
broad-gauge definition of applied social psychology is: applications of social
psychological methods, theories, principles, or research findings to understanding
or solution of social problems. Applied work can contribute usefully to
fundamental psychological theories, principles, and methods, as well as the other
way around. This text organizes its coverage of the field of applied social
psychology into various content areas, such as the environment, health, and
legal issues; and it also emphasizes the important uses and the limitations of
the major types of social psychological research methods.
Applied
social psychology differs from other fields of psychology in several key ways.
It typically starts with a concern for a particular social problem, and this
starting point makes explicit its value orientation toward improving people's
quality of life. It aims to have social utility and to adopt a broad, comprehensive
approach to social problems. Most of its work is done in natural field settings,
and it has to be especially concerned about the strength and generality of its
findings as well as considerations of benefit-to-cost ratios and political
feasibility. Theories are useful in applied work, just as in basic science, to
help explain, predict, and influence the events and variables of interest.
Six
different roles can be performed by various individuals working as applied social
psychologists. The most traditional is the role of researcher. Recently developing
roles are evaluator of social programs, organizational consultant or change
agent, and-policy advisor to administrators or legislators. Rarer social scientist
roles include organizational manager and social activist.
Debates
over the applicability of social psychology to resolving social problems largely
stem from the unfortunate split between the applied and theoretical sides of the
field, and also from the limitations of tightly controlled laboratory
experiments. In one sense it is impossible for individuals and groups nor to apply
whatever social science "knowledge" (either correct or incorrect) they
possess. However, many social psychologists favor more active efforts to apply
their discipline and increase its relevance to society.
Among
the major potential problem areas in this field are: the adequacy of research
methods and findings, the generalizabiliry of the evidence to other settings
and individuals, and the unintended consequences of social interventions. There
are also ethical issues involving theexercise of power over other people and
decisions about when and how to intervene in social situations and when and how
to stop. Though our goal should be the wellbeing of humankind in general, there
are unfortunate examples where some applied social scientists have failed to
follow that standard or to adhere to the commonly accepted ethical precepts of
the field.
SUGGESTED READINGS
Bermant, G., Kelman, H. C., & Warwick, D. P. (Eds.). (1978). The
ethics of social intervention. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.—An excellent
discussion of ethics in many different areas, including behavior modification,
organization development, and income maintenance experiments.
Helmreich, R. (1975). Applied social psychology: The unfulfilled
promise. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1, 548-560.---An
influential article that raised several crucial questions about the field.
Levy-Leboyer, C. (1988). Success and failure in applying psychology.
American Psychologist, 43, 779-785.—Good examples of the work of an applied
social psychologist.
Sieber, J. E. (1992). Planning ethically responsible research: A guide
for students and internal review boards.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.—Practically oriented advice for avoiding
ethical problems in all aspects of research.
Weiss, C. H. (Ed.). (1977).
Using social research in public policy making. Lexington, MA:
Lexington.—Focuses on the policy advice role of applied social scientists, in
areas as varied as health care, school desegregation, the judicial system, and
foreign policy.
Komentar
Posting Komentar