The role of desire in the prediction of intention: The case of smoking behavior
Jurnal:
Kova V B
& Rise, J. (2011). The Role of Desire in The Prediction of Intention: The Case
of Smoking Behavior. Swiss Journal of
Psychology, 70 (3), 2011, 141–148 DOI 10.1024/1421-0185/a000049
Kovač, V. B., & Rise, J. (2011).
The role of desire in the prediction of intention: The case of smoking
behavior. Swiss Journal of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift für
Psychologie/Revue Suisse de Psychologie, 70(3), 141.
Note: Ini hanya sebuah catatan, jangan dijadikan rujukan. Silahkan merujuk ke sumber aslinya
The
role of desire in the prediction of intention: The case of smoking behavior
Abstract.This
paper is based on the notion that desire represents an important motivational
aspect of the decision-making process. Thus, we examined the hypotheses that
desire (1) predicts behavioral intentions and(2) mediates the effects of theory
of planned behavior (TPB) components and past behavior on an individual’s
intention to quit smoking. The analysis is based on three separate conditions
in which the intention to quit smoking during the next 1, 4, or 6 months,
respectively, was measured; the three conditions contained identical variables.
The results of the hierarchical regression analysis show that there are
sufficient grounds for including desire as an additional predictor in the TPB
model. The results also show that desire mediates the effects of attitudes,
norms, and past behavior on intention. However, the mediating role of desire
was not obtained for therelationship between PBC and intention. Theoretical
implications and recommendations for future research are suggested.
Keywords: TPB, desire, intention, past behavior, decision making
Introduction
The
concept of behavioral intention is a well-established predictor of planned
behavior (Sheeran, 2002). For instance, the theory of planned behavior (TPB;
Ajzen, 1991), arguably the most popular model in the domain of health behavior,
posits that the most immediate determinant of actual behavioral performance is
the intention to engage in that particular behavior. Behavioral intention
reflects an individual’s decision to exert effort to perform the behavior and
is assumed to be a function of (1) an individual’s attitude toward the
behavior, that is, a positive or negative evaluation of the behavior; (2)
subjective norms that refer to an individual’s perceived social pressure to
perform the behavior; and (3) perceived behavioral control (PBC) in terms of
the perceived ease and difficulty in performing the behavior. These three
concepts – attitudes, subjective norms, and PBC – are explicitly taken to
encompass not only the reasons for action, evaluation of social pressure, and
feasibility of intended actions, but also the motivation for future actions
(Ajzen, 1991).
The TPB
has been successful in providing a parsimonious account of planned behaviors as
evidenced in the metaanalytical literature, which found that TPB components
accounted on an average for approximately 40% of the variance in intentions and
approximately 30% of the variance in behavior (Armitage & Conner, 2001;
Godin & Kok, 1996). Despite the relative success of the TPB components in
predicting intentions across various behavioral domains, Ajzen (1991)
entertained the possibility that other predictors besides the traditional TPB
components might be useful in predicting intentions, by their accounting for
additional variance beyond the TPB components. This general approach, which
aims to increase the number of predictors in the original model, is commonly
termed “theory broadening” in the literature (Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001),
and a number of additional predictors have been suggested (see Conner &
Armitage, 1998; Conner & Sparks, 2005; O’Keefe, 2002). More specifically,
meta-analyses found that descriptive norms (Rivis & Sheeran, 2004),
anticipated regret (Sandberg & Conner, 2008), and self-identity (Rise, Sheeran,
& Hukkelberg, 2010) account for 5%, 7%, and 6%, respectively, of additional
variance across a number of behavioral domains. When it comes to smoking
behavior, the TPB has been successfully used to account for intentions to quit
smoking (see Rise, Kova2, Kraft, & Moan, 2008, for an overview).
Furthermore, additional predictors, such as moral norms, anticipated affect,
group identity, and past behavior, have also been shown to have predictive
value for the intention to quit smoking (see Falomir & Invernizzi, 1999;
Moan & Rise, 2005; Rise & Ommundsen, 2010).
Despite
the work of Bagozzi and colleagues (Bagozzi, 1992; Perugini & Bagozzi,
2001, 2004b; Perugini & Conner, 2000), the role of desire as a central
psychological process within the general research on TPB across various behavioral
contexts remains underexplored. One possible explanation for this general lack
of interest in the concept of desire might be related to the established
theoretical premise in contemporary literature that desire is either conceptually
similar to behavioral intentions (Fishbein & Stasson, 1990) or implied in
TPB components (Ajzen, 1991). In contrast, Bagozzi and colleagues (Perugini
& Bagozzi, 2004b), building upon philosophical traditions (e.g., Davis,
1984), argue that empirical research should discriminate between desire and
intention, based on the idea that the theoretical components of TPB do not
explicitly include the motivation to act. The argument is that evaluative
appraisals in terms of attitudes, perceived control, and perceived social
influence merely provide reasons to act but fail to address more personal
motivational commitments to act. Following this line of thinking, the TPB
components are taken to be reason-based judgments or evaluations of future
actions and as such lack motivational energy in order to “move” the individual
in a specific direction (e.g., Perugini & Bagozzi, 2004b). For instance, a
passionate smoker may very well agree that quitting is a good idea and feel
pressure to do so from important individuals in their innermost social circle;
they may also feel capable of quitting yet simultaneously lack the desire to do
so. It follows that intention implies desire, while desire does not necessarily
imply intention (Bagozzi, 1992; Perugini & Bagozzi, 2004b). As such,
desires are conceptually connected with the wishing process, where realistic
possibilities as well as pro and cons have not yet been considered, while
intentions are per definitionem connected with volition and imply
self-regulative efforts at some point in the future. Furthermore, when it comes
to deliberative processes, desires are general wishes that by definition are
not restricted to specific time spans and do not imply efforts. As such,
desires precede intentions in time, and many human desires remain nonrealistic wishes
which are never realized. However, if a passionate smoker sets a specific date
with respect to their personal desire to quit smoking, he/she automatically
translates the desire into behavioral intention and is forced to consider the
feasibility aspects of this decision as the quitting date approaches.
The lack
of research in including the concept of desire in the equation may sound
strange considering its potential dual theoretical role in terms of theory
broadening, that is, as an additional predictor in the TPB and as a theory
deepener, that is, by illuminating the role of existing theoretical predictors
(Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001). Several studies across different contexts have
supported the role of desire as an additional predictor in the TPB (Bagozzi &
Kimmel, 1995; Leone, Perugini, & Ercolani, 1999, 2004). This line of
research indicates that desire also mediates most of the effects of TPB
components on intention (Bagozzi, Dholakia, & Basuroy, 2003; Leone et al.,
2004; Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001; Shaw, Shiu, Hassan, Bekin, & Hogg,
2007; Taylor, Bagozzi, & Gaither, 2005). In other words, this research
shows that, in addition to being an important predictor of intention in terms
of theory broadening, desire might also be important in terms of theory deepening
by providing a better understanding of the theoretical mechanism underlying the
effects of the existing theoretical predictors on behavioral intention
(Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001). Nevertheless, these issues have scarcely been
explored in the context of smoking behavior. In fact, to our knowledge, this is
the first study that specifically aims to explore desire’s usefulness as a
predictor of intention and mediator of TPB components on intentions in the context
of smoking behavior. The selection of the smoking context might be particularly
interesting in order to highlight the conceptual difference between these two concepts
when considering the fact that many smokers might simultaneously have a strong
desire to quit but are reluctant to form a strong intention to quit based on
the belief that the quitting process represents a difficult aim to achieve.
In
addition to exploring desire as an additional predictor and mediator, we
included a measure of past quitting attempts as an indicator of past behavior.
Past behavior has previously been suggested to be an important variable in the
intention formation process, and a direct effect of this predictor has been
taken as an indication that important, unmeasured behavioral causes have been
left out of the equation (see Ouelette & Wood, 1998; but also Ajzen, 1991,
2002b). We used this measurement in two ways. First, we included past behavior
as a statistical control taking into consideration that the role of this
concept is unclear in the context of TPB (Ajzen, 2002b). Second, we explored whether
desire in addition to TPB components also mediates the effect of past behavior
on intention. Past quitting attempts have been found to be related to the
intention to quit smoking (Kovac, Rise, Moan, 2010; Moan & Rise, 2005;
Norman, Conner, & Bell, 1999; Rise et al., 2008), although the direct
effect has been noted as weak (Moan & Rise, 2005).
In sum,
the present paper sheds some light on these issues in the context of smoking
behavior. More specifically, we analyze the relationship between traditional
TPB components, desire, past behavior, and intention to quit smoking in three
separate timeframe conditions (viz., during the next 1, 4, or 6 months).
Following the reasoning of Bagozzi and colleagues (e.g., Taylor et al., 2005),
we test the hypotheses that desire (1) represents an important predictor of
intention and (2) mediates the effects of the TPB components and past behavior
on intention.
Method
Participants and Procedure
The
participants in the 1- and 6-month conditions were students approached on
campus while smoking and asked to fill out a questionnaire concerning
components of the TPB and smoking. The participants in the 4-month condition were
daily smokers who responded to an invitation to participate in the study, which
was displayed in 15 national online newspapers. Apart from the temporal framing
of the intention to quit smoking (viz., during the next 1, 4, or 6 months), all
three questionnaires were identical. Respondents were confronted with items
measuring their intention to quit smoking at the end of the questionnaire in
order to not affect the responses to previous items. Participants in the
1-month condition (N= 96) had a mean
age of 23.83 (SD= 3.56); 68% were females and had been smoking for an average
of 8.15 (SD= 3.47) years, consuming 9.68 (SD=
4.47) cigarettes per day. The corresponding figures for the 4-month condition
were: N= 939, mean age 35.81 (SD= 11.71) years, 49% females, smoking for 18.65
(SD=10.87) years on average, consuming 13.53 (SD= 7.44) cigarettes per day. The
corresponding figures for the 6-month condition were: N= 128, mean age 24.43
(SD=5.88) years, 67% females, smoking for 7.90 (SD= 5.95) years on an average,
consuming 10.90 (SD= 6.51) cigarettes per day.
Measurements
Attitude
Attitude
was measured with 5 items. A sample item is “In my opinion, quitting smoking
will be (bad/good).” The 5 items were rated on a 7-point response scale ranging
from (–3) bad to (+3) good. Cronbach’s αs were .88 (1 month), .76
(4 months), and .83 (6 months).
Subjective Norm
Subjective
norm was assessed by 2 items such as “People who are important to me think I
should quit smoking” and “People who are important to me wish that I would quit
smoking.” The items were rated on a 7-point response scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to7 (completely agree). The correlations
(Pearson’sr) between the 2 items were .78,p< .001 (1 month), .85,p< .001
(4 months), and .79, p< .001 (6 months).
Perceived Behavioral Control
Perceived
behavioral control was assessed using 5 items, for example, “How much control
do you have over quitting smoking?” and “How confident are you that you will
quit smoking?” The items were rated on a 7-point response scale ranging from 1
(no control) to7 (a high amount of control). Cronbach’sαs were .88 (1 month), .89 (4
months), and .85 (6 months).
Desire
Desire was
assessed by 2 items (“My general desire to quit smoking is usually . . .” and
“I often have the desire to quit smoking on a daily basis.”) The first item is
designed to capture a general and established desire to quit smoking, the
second item to capture the frequency of desire on a daily basis, which in turn
is assumed to be related to a more general desire to quit. The items were rated
on a 7-point response scale ranging from 1 (very
weak/often) to7 (very strong/often).
The correlations (Pearson’s r) between the 2 items were .72, p <.001(1 month),
.69, p < .001 (4 month), and .72, p < .001 (6 months). Note that,
although the measurements of intention were restricted to 1, 4, or 6 months,
the items that comprised the measure of desire were not limited to a specific
timeframe. This difference is based on the theoretical conceptualizations of
desires and intentions (Perugini & Bagozzi, 2004b) and will be further
considered in the discussion.
Past Quitting Attempts
Past
quitting attempts, a measure of past behavior, was assessed directly in terms
of self-reported number of previous attempts to quit smoking.
Intention to Quit Smoking
The
participants’ intention to quit smoking was measured by the following items: “During
the next (month/4 months/6 months): (1) “I intend to quit smoking,” (2) “I am going
to quit smoking.”” The two items were rated on a 7-point response scale ranging
from (1) very unlikely to (7) very likely. The correlations
(Pearson’sr) between the two items were .93, p< .001 (1 month),
.85,p<.001(4 months), and .84,p< .001 (6 months).
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Table 1
presents the means, standard deviations, andp-values that illustrate sample
differences regarding background variables such as age, cigarettes smoked per
day, smoking duration, and past behavior. The 1-month and 6-month samples
differed only with respect to past behavior, which was found to be
significantly different on thep< .05 level.
However,
the 4-month sample differed significantly from the other two samples with
respect to the background variables. This finding was expected considering that
the 1- and 6-month samples were taken from student populations, while the
4-month sample stemmed from the general population of smokers. Table 2 presents
the correlations between study variables and intention in the three conditions (samples).
Intention was significantly correlated with the number of cigarettes smoked per
day, attitude, past behavior, and desire in all three samples. Significant
correlations between subjective norm and intention were obtained only in the 4-
and 6-month conditions, while PBC and intention correlated significantly in the
1- and 4-month conditions.
One
unexpected and unusual result was the negative nonsignificant correlation
between intention and PBC in the 6-month condition.
Hierarchical Regression Analysis
In order
to test the two hypotheses, we conducted the hierarchical linear regression
analysis in four steps. Considering the fact that the 4-month sample was significantly
different from the other two samples, age, cigarettes smoked per day, and
smoking duration were entered in the first step to statistically control the
impact of these variables on the overall results. The TPB components were
entered in the second step, past behavior in terms of past quitting attempts were
entered in the third step, followed by desire in the fourth step. The results
of the regression analysis clearly supported the first hypothesis, namely, that
desire is an important predictor of the intention to quit smoking (see Table 3
for details). The increase in explained variance in all three conditions after
the measure of desire was included in the third step of the regression analysis
was considerable (24%, 11%, and 24%, respectively). Furthermore, the impact of desire
was statistically significant in all three analyses.
For the
second hypothesis, we conducted a series of hierarchical regression analyses to
determine whether the effects of the TPB components and past behavior were
mediated by desire. Tests for mediation were conducted following the
instructions by Baron and Kenny (1986).
Mediation
effects are confirmed when a mediating variable accounts for a relationship
between two other variables in the sense that the effects of predictor
variables are significantly reduced when a hypothesized mediating variable is included
in the regression analysis (Baron & Kenny, 1986).
Table 3
shows that the reduction of beta values in the third step, after desire was
included, was substantial for attitudes, SN, and past behavior. In order to
test whether this reduction was statistically significant, a series of Sobel tests
were conducted. The results of these tests were significant for attitudes (z=
3.08,p< .01;z= 12.27,p< .001; z= 4.79,p< .01; for 1, 4, and 6 months,
respectively) and past behavior (z= 2.29,p< .05;z= 11.45,p< .001;z= 2.29,p<
.05; for 1, 4, and 6 months, respectively) in all three conditions. We also
found significant meditation effects of desire on the relationship between SN
and intention in the 4-month and 6-month conditions (z= 11.73,p< .001; z=
3.64,p< .001, respectively), while that for the 1-month condition was found
to be nonsignificant (z= 1.95,p< .06). In sum, these results clearly
indicate that desire, in addition to being an important predictor of intentions
in terms of increased explained variance, also serves as an important mediator
providing a more comprehensive account for the relation between attitudes, SN,
and past behavior, on the one hand, and behavioral intentions on the other.
However, there was no meditation effect of desire on the relation between PBC
and intention as indicated by the nonsignificant change of beta values of PBC
in the third step of the regression analyses when desire was included (Table
3).
Table 1 Descriptive
statistics for the three samples
Predictors
|
1 month
|
4 months
|
6 months
|
|||||
Mean
|
SD
|
p
|
Mean
|
SD
|
p
|
Mean
|
SD
|
|
Age
|
23.8
|
3.56
|
***
|
35.8
|
11.71
|
***
|
24.3
|
5.88
|
CPD
|
9.6
|
4.47
|
***
|
13.5
|
7.44
|
***
|
10.9
|
6.50
|
SD
|
8.1
|
3.47
|
***
|
18.6
|
10.87
|
***
|
7.9
|
5.95
|
PB
|
2.9
|
5.59
|
ns
|
3.12
|
2.94
|
***
|
1.6
|
2.78
|
Note.
p-values for the 1- and 6-month conditions are nonsignificant except for PB,
which is significant at the .05 level. CPD (cigarettes per day), SD (smoking
duration), PB (past behavior). *p< .05, **p< .01, ***p< .001.
Table 2
Correlations
between study variables and intention in the three conditions
Predictors
|
1-month condition
|
4-month condition
|
6-month condition
|
Age
|
–.05
|
–.02
|
–.12
|
CPD
|
–.23*
|
–.15***
|
–.23*
|
SD
|
–.04
|
–.05
|
–.18
|
Attitude
|
.34**
|
.48***
|
.52***
|
SN
|
.07
|
.31***
|
.31***
|
PBC
|
.28**
|
.37***
|
–.11
|
PB
|
.28**
|
.25***
|
.21*
|
Desire
|
.43***
|
.60***
|
.70***
|
Note. CPD
(cigarettes per day), SD (smoking duration), SN (subjective norm), PBC
(perceived behavioral control), and PB (past behavior). *p< .05, **p<
.01, ***p< .001.
Discussion
The
starting point for the present research is the idea that desire represents a
potentially important predictor variable which is frequently overlooked in
theoretical models aimed at accounting for people’s behavioral intentions
(Bagozzi, 1992; Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001). The results clearly show that
desire contributes to theory broadening and hence should be included as an
additional predictor of behavioral intentions in extended models of planned
behavior. Multiple regression analyses showed that desire enhanced the prediction
of quitting intentions quite considerably after the components of the TPB and
past behavior were taken into account (24%, 11%, 24% in the 1-, 4- and 6-month
conditions, respectively). According to the two criteria proposed by O’Keefe
(2002), any potential additional predictor variable in the TPB should
contribute to a large amount of explained variance in the outcome variable, and
not simply a statistically reliable increment. In our view, values in the range
of 11% to 24% additional variance satisfy this criterion. As to the second
criterion (viz., that the efficacy of a candidate predictor must be
demonstrated across a wide range of behaviors), this needs to be confirmed in
further studies.
In terms
of theory broadening, the results also show that desire represents an important
mediator and may thus account for how traditional TPB variables are
theoretically interpreted (Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001). Our findings support
the work of Bagozzi and colleagues, who persuasively argued that evaluative
aspects of attitudes and normative aspects of SN do not necessarily imply
connections to behavioral intention if the object or course of action is not desired
(Leone et al., 2004; Shaw et al., 2007). For instance, with increased knowledge
about the negative aspects of smoking, it may be fair to say that an increased
number of individuals agree that quitting smoking represents a sensible course
of action. Hence, very few individuals would argue that the decision underlying
personal smoking cessation is bad or report that important people in their
social circle would like them to continue smoking. However, the positive attitudes
toward quitting smoking and supportive normative opinions of significant others
will remain static in terms of action if not first translated into personal
desires, which in turn entail a motivation to act. Without desire at work,
attitudes and SN might frequently be representatives of the self-detached
evaluations on a favor-disfavor continuum, which are not embedded in commitment
toward intended action. In other words, the results indicate that positive or
negative attitudinal aspects and normative pressures must be connected to
personal motivational processes before they can successfully be translated into
intention (Shaw et al., 2007). The same logic applies to the finding that the
effects of past behavior are mediated by desire in the sense that past behavior
(similar to attitudes and SN) is linked to intention to the extent that it is
based on a desire to pursue a given action.
Table 3 Hierarchical
multiple regression analyses of quitting intention timeframe on Age, CPD, and
SD (Step 1), Attitude, SN, and PBC (Step 2), PB (Step 3), and Desire (Step 4)
for the three samples
Step
|
Predictors
|
1 month
|
4 months
|
6 months
|
||||||
β
|
R2
|
F
|
β
|
R2
|
F
|
β
|
R2
|
F
|
||
1
|
Age
|
–.21
|
.08
|
.20
|
||||||
CPD
|
–.15
|
–.15
|
– .20
|
|||||||
SD
|
.13
|
–.09
|
–.21
|
|||||||
.01
|
1.12
|
.02
|
7.94***
|
.04
|
2.35
|
|||||
2
|
Age
|
–.25
|
–.06
|
.07
|
||||||
CPD
|
.05
|
.02
|
–.08
|
|||||||
SD
|
.10
|
.02
|
–.10
|
|||||||
A
|
.29*
|
.36***
|
.42***
|
|||||||
SN
|
.14
|
.18***
|
.14
|
|||||||
PBC
|
.10
|
.32***
|
–.11
|
|||||||
.11
|
3.83*
|
.34
|
148.59***
|
.25
|
10.38***
|
|||||
3
|
Age
|
–.21
|
–.05
|
.13
|
||||||
CPD
|
–.03
|
.02
|
–.12
|
|||||||
SD
|
.08
|
–.02
|
–.16
|
|||||||
A
|
.24
|
.33***
|
.41***
|
|||||||
SN
|
.11
|
.15***
|
.13
|
|||||||
PBC
|
.12
|
.34***
|
–.09
|
|||||||
PB
|
.17
|
.20***
|
.22*
|
|||||||
.13
|
2.14
|
.38
|
51.65***
|
.29
|
6.14*
|
|||||
4
|
Age
|
–.28*
|
–.07
|
|||||||
CPD
|
.01
|
.00
|
–.12
|
|||||||
SD
|
.17
|
.03
|
.63
|
|||||||
A
|
.00
|
.14***
|
.19*
|
|||||||
SN
|
.02
|
.08**
|
.02
|
|||||||
PBC
|
.15
|
.32***
|
.03
|
|||||||
PB
|
.12
|
.07*
|
.08
|
|||||||
D
|
.57***
|
.45***
|
.60***
|
|||||||
.37
|
27.24***
|
.49
|
197.24***
|
.53
|
48.25***
|
Note. CPD
(cigarettes per day), SD (smoking duration), A (Attitude), SN (subjective
norm), PBC (perceived behavioral control), PB (past behavior), and D (desire). *p<
.05, **p< .01, ***p< .001.
However,
contrary to our hypothesis, we did not find support for a meditational role of
desire when it comes to the relation between PBC and intention (for similar
findings, see Leone et al., 2004; Shaw et al., 2007). One possible explanation
for this finding might be the conceptual difference between the concepts of
attitudes and SN, on the one hand, and PBC, on the other (Ajzen, 2002a).
Perceptions of control over future actions represent an estimation of people’s
sense of control over ease or difficulty at the moment of performance of a
given action. In contrast to attitudes and SN, reporting accurate levels of
control represents by definition a difficult estimation task, especially when
it comes to behaviors that require a great deal of volition, effort, and
personal capability, as is the case with the process of quitting smoking. Thus,
the concept of PBC, in contrast to attitudes and norms, represents a
constrained thinking, which is conceptually closely connected to
selfregulation, as it entails perceptions of accurate judgments of performance
as well as the prediction of future behavior, that is, a proxy measure for
actual control (Ajzen, 1991).
In
contrast, the concept of desire, similar to attitudes and SN, is not by
definition related to self-regulation, but is mainly based on unconstrained
thinking about possible and often unrealistic behavioral alternatives. For
instance, Ajzen (2002a) noted that in the case of behaviors which are difficult
to perform, PBC might be a strong predictor of intention, even when desire is
entered into the equation.
Similarly,
Fishbein and Stasson (1990) challenged the idea of making a conceptual
distinction between intention and desire, proposing that the distinction is
easier to make between desire and behavioral expectations. Thus, when it comes
to quitting smoking, a smoker may have a strong desire to quit while at the
same time realizing that he or she will not try to stop smoking after taking
into account that this might become a difficult project. Furthermore, according
to Perugini and Bagozzi (2004a), the concepts of desire and intention are
distinct in terms of perceived performability (desired actions are less
performable) or action-connectedness (desires are at a higher level of
abstraction). By this account, feasibility concerns are less salient with
desire than with intention, implying that desire should be a less than optimal
mediator of the effect of perceived control on intention, and that perceived
control should be a better predictor of intention than of desire. These
propositions provide sensible explanations for the lack of support for the meditational
role of desire between PBC and intention. Indeed, several other studies have
obtained similar results and proposed that when it comes to difficult personal
projects in terms of self-regulation, desire should be associated with PBC to a
lesser extent and hence be disqualified as a potential mediator between control
beliefs and intention (Leone et al., 2004; Shaw et al., 2007). However, it is
important to note that these theoretical accounts of the lack of support in
favor of the study findings are still speculative and offer a posthoc
explanation which need to be formally tested in future research.
Theoretical Implications and
Recommendations
The
present results might also shed light on the conceptual distinction between
desire and intention (Malle & Knobe, 2001). Perugini and Bagozzi (2004a)
noted that the temporal dimension is a key issue with regard to the conceptual difference
between these terms, in the sense that behavioral intention, relative to
desire, is more closely connected to action in terms of a specified timeframe.
Thus, while intention is relatively now-oriented and action-connected, desire tends
to be more future-oriented, entailing postponement of action considerations
until the actual decision to go through with the action is reached. In other
words, desire resides on a level of abstraction where practical aspects of action
are not yet considered. This implies that desire by definition should be more
strongly related to long-term decisions than to short-term ones. This is the
rationale behind why we decided to provide a global measure of desire, that is,
not to limit the measure of desire to a specific timeframe.
Although
the majority of studies have used specific measures of desire (in which desire
was measured at the same level of specificity as the TPB variables), we posit
that a specific measure of desire is difficult to distinguish conceptually from
a specific measure of intention. The methodological concern was that in the
case of a time-limited measurement of desire, we would obtain “intention-like”
responses where participants would be forced to limit their desire-based
cognitions and consider practical and realistic aspects of future actions.
Consistent with this finding, the present data indicate that when the intended
targets are situated in the distant future calling for no immediate action, measures
of behavioral intention run the risk of being overly abstract and therefore
transform themselves into “desirelike” cognitions. More specifically, our data
show that correlations between intention and desire tended to become stronger
as the timeframe for planned actions increased (r= .43, .60, and .71 for 1, 4,
and 6 months, respectively). The differences in correlations between intention
and desire were found to be significant for all three conditions (.43, .60, and
.71, respectively;p< .05). This pattern of correlation indicates that
measurements of desire and intention tend to become more similar over time.
Hence, if people are asked to evaluate actions placed in the distant future, they
might actually report their desires rather than their behavioral intentions. It
further implies that assessing intentions in the very distant future might then
be incompatible with the theoretical conceptualization of intention, which per
definition is connected with short-term actions (Perugini & Bagozzi,
2004b). In terms of future research, we suggest that measurements of (1) desire
should not be timeconstrained or at least not limited to short periods of time,
and (2) intention should be time-constrained to relatively short timeframes.
Thus, although this issue depends on the behavior in question, the idea is that
measuring intention over a period of, say, 2 years would be as useless as
measuring desire for, say, 1 month.
Limitations
Some
limitations of the present study should be mentioned.
First,
some of the measures employed only two items; thus, the study would benefit by
including more items in order to reduce possible measurement errors. Second,
although measures of behavioral intentions are fairly standardized in TPB research,
this can hardly be the case when it comes to measuring desire. Hence, in order
to promote the argument that these concepts are distinct, one should first
develop a reliable standardized measure of desire, subsequently presenting
evidence of discriminating validity between the two measurements (see Perugini
& Bagozzi, 2004a). Third, although we argue that desire is conceptually
distinct from behavioral intention, the nature of the behavior in question might
be of importance. Thus, the distinction between desire and intention might be
especially pronounced when it comes to requiring personal projects in terms of
self-regulation. In this context, our results might be inflated due to the fact
that quitting smoking is a difficult goal to achieve for which the
discrepancies between “I want,” “I should,” “I ought,” and “I can” are greater
than for behaviors in which wanting and intending have a similar direction
(e.g., planning a vacation). Fourth, the samples in the present study were not
specifically selected for testing the two study hypotheses. The 1- and 6-month
samples were taken from the student population whereas the 4-month sample was
from general population (see Table 2). Participants in the 4-month sample had
significantly different values regarding common background variables. However,
although these differences might have confounded the results, the general
pattern supporting the basic reasoning behind the hypotheses is clearly
detectable. Finally, there was the unexpected negative effect of PBC on
intention in the 6-month sample. This unusual result might have confounded the
results as well as represent measurement error. Notwithstanding these
limitations, the results of the present study are important in terms of
providing support for empirical and theoretical distinctions between the key
concepts involved in the decision-making process. This finding also suggests
that future research should pay closer attention to the processes and
conditions under which desires are successfully translated into behavioral
intentions, taking into consideration that intention is generally accepted to
be the best predictor of actual performance (Ajzen, 1991). However, our
findings also emphasize the importance of developing the theoretically grounded
and reliable measurement of desire, which represents a central startingpoint
for the inclusion of this concept into decision-making models.
Conclusions
Over the
years, considerable empirical evidence has accumulated to support the inclusion
of various theoretical constructs to increase the proportion of explained
variance in behavioral intention of the TPB (Conner & Sparks, 2005).
Following
this line of research, the present results may have important theoretical
implications for the inclusion of desire as an additional predictor in the TPB.
Although future research needs to confirm the present results before more
definite conclusions can be made, these results clearly show that desire (1)
has a significant and direct predictive effect on the intention to quit
smoking, and (2) represents an important mediator between attitudes, SN, and past
behavior, on the one hand, and behavioral intentions, on the other.
References
Ajzen, I.
(1991). The theory of planned behavior.Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
Ajzen, I.
(2002a). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the
theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied
and Social Psychology, 32, 1–20.
Ajzen, I.
(2002b). Residual effects of past on later behavior: Habituation and reasoned
action perspectives. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 6,107–122.
Armitage,
C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behavior: A
meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 40, 471–499.
Bagozzi,
R. P. (1992). The self-regulation of attitudes, intentions, and behavior.Social
Psychology Quarterly, 55, 178–204.
Baggozi,
R. P., & Kimmel, S. K. (1995). A comparison of leading theories for the
prediction of goal-directed behaviors. British
Journal of Social Psychology, 34,437–461.
Bagozzi,
R. P., Dholakia, U. M., & Basuroy, S. (2003). How effortful decisions get
enacted: The motivating role of decision processes, desires, and anticipated
emotions. Journal of Behavioral Decision
Making, 16, 273–295.
Baron, R.
M., & Kenny D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in
social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical
considerations.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
Conner,
M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the theory of planned behavior: A
review and avenues for further research. Journal of Applied Social psychology,
28, 1429–1464.
Conner,
M., & Sparks, P. (2005). The theory of planned behavior and health
behaviors. In M. Conner & P. Norman (Eds.),Predicting health behavior (pp.
171–222). Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.
Davis, W.
A. (1984). The two senses of desire.Philosophical Studies, 45, 181–195.
Falomir,
J. M., & Invernizzi, F. (1999). The role of social influence and smoker
identity in resistance to smoking cessation. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 58,
73–84.
Fishbein,
M., & Stasson, M. (1990). The role of desires, self-predictions, and
perceived control in the prediction of training session attendance.Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 20, 173–198.
Godin, G.,
& Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned behavior: A review of its
applications to health-related behaviors.American Journal of Health Promotion,
11, 87–98.
Kova2, V.
B., Rise, J., & Moan, I. S. (2010). From intentions to quit to actual
quitting process: The case of smoking behavior in the light of TPB. Journal of
Applied Biobehavioral Research, 14, 181–197.
Leone, L.,
Perugini, M., & Ercolani, A. P. (1999). A comparison of three models of
attitude-behavior relationships on studying behavior domain.European Journal of
Social Psychology, 29, 161–189.
Leone, L.,
Perugini, M., & Ercolani, A. P. (2004). Studying, practicing, and
mastering: A test of the model of goal-directed behavior (MGB) in the software
learning domain.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 1945–1973.
Malle, B.
G., & Knobe, J. (2001). The distinction between desire and intention: A
folk conceptual analysis. In B. G. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin
(Eds.),Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition(pp.
45–67). Cambridge, MA: MIT.
Moan, I.
S., & Rise, J. (2005). Quitting smoking: Applying an extended version of
the theory of planned behavior in predicting intention and behavior. Journal of
Applied Biobehavioral Research, 10,39–68.
Norman,
P., Conner, M., & Bell, R. (1999). The theory of planned behavior and
smoking cessation.Health Psychology, 18, 89–94.
O’Keefe,
D. J. (2002).Persuasion: Theory and research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Ouelette,
J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The
multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future
behavior.Psychological Bulletin, 124,54–74.
Perugini,
M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2001). The role of desires and anticipated emotions
in goal-directed behaviors: Broadening and deepening the theory of planned
behavior.British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 79–98.
Perugini,
M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2004a). An alternative view of prevolitional
processes in decision making: Conceptual issues and empirical evidence. In G.
Haddock & G. R. Maio (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on the psychology of
attitudes: The Cardiff symposium(pp. 169–201). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Perugini,
M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2004b). The distinction between desires and
intentions.European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 69–84.
Perugini,
M., & Conner, M. (2000). Predicting and understanding behavioral volitions:
The interplay between goals and behaviors.European Journal of Social
Psychology, 30, 705–731.
Rise, J.,
& Ommundsen, R. (2011). Predicting the intention to quit smoking: A
comparative study among Spanish and Norwegian students.Europe’s Journal of
Psychology, 7,143–163.
Rise, J.,
Kova2, V. B., Kraft, P., & Moan, I. S. (2008). Predicting the intention to
quit smoking and quitting behavior: Extending the theory of planned
behavior.British Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 291–310.
Rise, J.,
Sheeran, P., & Hukkelberg, S. (2010). The role of selfidentity in the
theory of planned behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 40, 1085–1105.
Rivis, A.
J., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Descriptive norms as an additional predictor in
the theory of planned behavior: A metaanalysis.Current Psychology, 22, 264–280.
Sandberg,
T., & Conner, M. (2008). Anticipated regret as an additional predictor in
the theory of planned behavior: A metaanalysis.British Journal of Social
Psychology, 47, 589–606.
Shaw, D.,
Shiu, E., Hassan, L., Bekin, C., & Hogg, G. (2007). Intending to be
ethical: An examination of consumer choice in sweatshop avoidance.Advances in
Consumer Research, 34, 31–38.
Sheeran,
P. (2005). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. In
W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology (Vol.
12, pp. 1–36). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Taylor, S.
D., Bagozzi, R., & Gaither, C. A. (2005). Decision making and effort in the
self-regulation of hypertension: Testing two competing theories.British Journal of Health Psychology, 10,505–530.
Velibor
B. Kovač
|
University
of Agder
Department
of Education
PO Box
422
4604
Kristiansand
Norway
bobo.kovac@uia.no
|
Komentar
Posting Komentar