Translate

Religion, Attitudes, and Social Behavior

Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality / edited by Raymond F.
Paloutzian, Crystal L. Park.
(p.274-291)
***************************************************************************
15

Religion, Attitudes, and Social Behavior

MICHAEL J. DONAHUE
MICHAEL E. NIELSEN


For many, this topic is central to the social-scientific study of religion. Never mind where religion came from, how it develops, or even how to measure it, does it “work” ? Are religious people “better” than others? This chapter considers findings concerning the relation between religiousness and a variety of interpersonal attitudes and behaviors: prejudice, altruism and prosocial behavior, honesty, sexuality, family relations, crime and delinquency, and politics and peace.

THE PSYCHOLOGY OF . . . WHAT?

But first we offer some thoughts about definitions. What is “religion”? While generally some variant of “the perceived relation between an individual and a powerful supernatural agent or agents” would seem to suffice, clever discussions can be constructed about exceptions to such a rule. For example, many point to Buddhism as an example of a world religion that does not have a God. “However, this argument is based on a specious account of [it];. . . . [Many contend that Buddhists] . . . don’t worship the Buddha, yet [they] treat him as a supernatural agent, especially in rituals” (Slone, 2004, p. 5; see also his Chap. 6). Others inquire whether conceptual systems that supposedly serve the same functions as religion for a given individual should be considered a sort of “implicit” religion (e.g., devotion to Apple computers; Lam, 2001). But as Lupton (1986) notes, such an overbroad definition results in unacceptably fuzzy categories, and so the interested researcher would be better served by accepting the fact that many people have no religious orientation rather than by diluting the definition of religion to the point of uselessness.
In general, most measures of religiousness are quite highly intercorrelated (e.g., Bassett et al., 1991). In addition, only a small subset of measures is used with any frequency: church attendance; single-item measures of religious commitment or salience; intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness; and so on. Thus most researchers use precisely the sort of definition of what it means to be “religious” that might be obtained from the “personin-the-street” (but see also the discussion of the concepts of “religion” and “spirituality” in Zinnbauer and Pargament, Chapter 2, this volume).


RELIGION AS SOCIAL

Why consider religion as a social phenomenon? James (1902/1985) defines religion as “the feelings, acts, and experiences of individual men in their solitude, so far as they apprehend themselves to stand in relation to whatever they may consider the divine” (p. 31). But religion can also be defined as an inherentlysocialphenomenon. For example, in Islam, theahadeeth (collections of the teachings of Mohammed on various specific topics, not unlike Jesus’ sermons in the Christian synoptic gospels; Matthew, Mark, and Luke) are filled with assurances from the Messenger of God that good works will ensure entrance into paradise: “Charity is prescribed for each descendant of Adam every day the sun rises . . . listening to the deaf, leading the blind . . . supporting the feeble with the strength of one’s arms—all of these are charity prescribed for you” (Fiqh-us-Sunnah, Vol. 3, no. 98).
In the Christian tradition, and most notably in the synoptic gospels, the social nature of the teachings is quite striking. In addition to the so-called Golden Rule (Matthew 7:12) and the “second greatest commandment,” love of neighbor as of self (Matthew 22:39), the Lord’s Prayer calls down upon believers the condition that God forgive them only to the degree that they forgive others (Matthew 6:12). Jesus promises his presence when “two or threehave gathered” (Matthew 18:20, emphasis added); it might be argued that there is no such thing as “one Christian.” Perhaps even more to the point, when Jesus speaks of how one is to be “saved,” thefailure to do goodfor others is itself a sufficiently grievous offense to result in eternal damnation (Lazarus and the rich man, Luke 16:19–21; the parable of the sheep and the goats, Matthew 25:31–46).
Thus, the inherently social nature of religion, and its relation to social psychology seems clear. Indeed, prominent theories in personality-social psychology have been employed in the analysis of religious behavior, including dissonance theory (Brock, 1962), attribution theory (Bulman & Wortman, 1977), and theories of altruism (Batson, Eidelman, Higley, & Russell, 2001). Therefore attention to religion as a context for social interaction would seem, if nothing else, representative of the research in the area.

DIFFERENT WAYS OF BEING RELIGIOUS: INTRINSIC, EXTRINSIC, QUEST

In light of the social nature of religion, it is perhaps not surprising that the most commonly used scales of religiousness, at least to study its relation to social phenomena, were designed by social psychologists. Since much of the research to be cited in what follows employs these measures, a brief introduction would seem appropriate.

Intrinsic and Extrinsic
According to Gordon W. Allport, in his seminal study of the roots of discrimination,The Nature of Prejudice(1954/1979), “The role of religion is paradoxical. It makes prejudice and it unmakes prejudice” (p. 444). This finding lead him to hypothesize that were two contrasting types of religious motivations. After a period of conceptual development us-ing a variety of terminologies (see Donahue, 1985, for a review), he ultimately settled on the terms intrinsic (I) and extrinsic (E) religiousness. In Allport’s view, “the extrinsically motivated person uses his religion whereas the intrinsically motivated lives his religion” (Allport & Ross, 1967, p. 434). A typical item from theIscale is: “My whole approach to life is based upon my religion”; a typical item from theEscale is: “What religion offers me most is comfort in times of trouble and sorrow” (Gorsuch & McPherson, 1989).
AlthoughIandEwere originally postulated as the ends of a single bipolar continuum, it was soon discovered thatIandEwere better conceptualized as two separate independent variables. The ensuing years saw fairly widespread use of the scales. By the end of 1982, nearly 70 articles had been published in English employing the scales (Donahue, 1985, p. 400) as well as more than 50 doctoral dissertations. A search of Englishlanguage research citations and doctoral dissertations reveals that in the 10 years between 1986 and 1995, some 200 journal articles and 160 dissertations appeared that involved the scales. And the rate has continued unabated. The period from 1996 to 2003 produced another 200 journal articles and 140 dissertations.
The scales have not been without their critics. Dittes (1971) complained that Allport ventured into the “prophetic” by attempting to determine the nature of “true” religion.
In the same year, Hunt and King published a factor analysis claiming thatIwas multidimensional, and that a singleEfactor could best be measured using only six of the 11 items in the scale. In contrast, Kirkpatrick (1989) concluded just the opposite: that Ihad a unitary structure, whileEmight involve as many as three dimensions.
Kirkpatrick and Hood (1990) published an article calling for more theoretically sophisticated research than had characterized the studies that had used I andEup to that time. Connolly (1999), presenting a review of Hunt and King’s (1971) and Kirkpatrick and Hood’s (1990) critiques, concluded that there was little point to continued use of the scales, and that “more able psychologists of religion” would probably stop using them and “involvement inI–Eresearch may well become a banner identifying second-rate psychologists of religion” (p.183). Or not. Spilka, Hood, Hunsberger, and Gorsuch (2003) cite research involvingIand/orEon 69 of the 543 pages of their text.

Quest
“Quest” (Q) religiousness was proposed by C. Daniel Batson to address a dimension of “growth” and “seeking” that he felt had been in Allport’s original conceptualization ofI, but that was not embodied in theIscale (see Batson, Schoenrade, & Ventis, 1993, for an overview). He developed a scale that was intended to focus on a “growth” or “seekership” quality in religious development. The scale includes items such as “It might be said that I value my religious doubts and uncertainties.” Critics ofQhave questioned whether it could be a measure of religion at all, since it fails to correlate with other, more established measures, and there was no clear evidence that religious groups scored higher onQthan less religious groups (Donahue, 1985), but Batson et al. (1993) cited evidence to the contrary. Hood and Morris (1985) tookQto task on the grounds that a measure that was negatively correlated with measures of doctrinal orthodoxy could not be a measure of religiousness, but Batson and Ventis (1985) begged to differ.
In view of the ongoing controversy, many researchers in the psychology of religion have opted to use all three scales,I, E, and Q, to examine the relation between religiousness and behavior. That body of research, along with a variety of other measures of religiousness, are examined in what follows.

PREJUDICE

Allport and Ross (1967) correlatedIandEwith several measures of prejudice: negative attitudes toward blacks, Jews, other non-Europeans, and mentally disturbed individuals.
He also included a scale of what he referred to as “a ‘jungle’ philosophy of life” reflecting the belief that “it’s a dog-eat-dog world.”Ewas generally correlated with such measures; Iwas not. Donahue (1985) reported that later studies found essentially the same pattern of relations.
But why isIuncorrelated with prejudice? Why doesn’t religion inhibit prejudice, as Allport’s conceptual approach predicted it would? One possible reason for a lack of negative correlation is that the relation is not linear, or “straight-line.” A consistent curvilinear relation between the two (prejudice is highest for those with moderate I scores, and lowest for those scoring high or low onI) would produce a nonsignificant correlation. The “positive correlation” represented by the “rising” side of the inverted-U shape would cancel out the “negative” correlation in the second half of the curve. In fact, Allport and Ross (1967) presented this possibility, and cited seven studies with a variety of religiousness measures that obtained that finding. Gorsuch and Aleshire (1974) examined 25 religion prejudice studies in which the curvilinearity hypothesis could be addressed, and concluded that “20 were consistent with the expectation that the marginal church member manifested more prejudice than either the nonactive or the most active members” (p. 285). Spilka et al. (2003) criticized this finding, however, on the grounds that many studies did not include “nonreligious” individuals.
Most recent studies have continued to find no correlations betweenIand prejudice (Bailey, 2000; Cannon, 2001; Lundblad, 2002). Beck and Miller (2000) found that those who scored high onIwere less likely to make snap judgments about others’ religious or moral orientation (Escores were not reported). Herek (1987) found intrinsics no less prejudiced against gays and lesbians thanextrinsics, but Fulton, Gorsuch, and Maynard (1999) found that prejudice against gay men and lesbians was correlated with Ionly in the case of morally based feelings about them rather than “nonmorally” based opinions (hate the sin, love the sinner). Using a “social distance” measure, they found that attitudes of those scoring high onItoward gay men and lesbians were no more negative than their attitude toward others who violated their moral code—for example, liars and racists. Among Lutheran pastors, Taylor (2000) found Iuncorrelated with either prejudice against or positive attitudes toward gay men;Epositively correlated with attitudes toward gay men (but not lesbians);Qcorrelated with positive attitudes toward both. A high score on an index combining both a measure of belief orthodoxy andQwas unrelated to attitudes toward gays and lesbians.
In general, then,Iis uncorrelated with measures of prejudice, although devoutly religious individuals prefer to “keep their distance” from people whom they consider “sinners.” A religion of social convention (as measured byE) is more likely to be related to prejudice against members of an outgroup.

PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND HELPING

Establishing a connection between religion and helping others would seem to be a “slam dunk.” The record of history points to Christianity as the inventor of the nonprofit hospital and of religious orders solely dedicated to serving in them. The presence of men and women religious ministering to the victims of various pandemics throughout history is well established. Indeed, Stark (1997) contends that it was likely that Christians’ response to a plague in Rome, and their decision to stay and care for the sick, rather than flee to the countryside, provided a major impetus for Christianity’s early growth. More recently, it should be noted that when the Nobel Peace Prize does not go to diplomats or organizations, it frequently goes to individuals motivated by strong religious beliefs (e.g., Jimmy Carter, Carlos F. X. Belo, the 14th Dalai Lama, Elie Wiesel, Desmond Tutu, Lech Walesa,  Teresa).

A variety of research studies have examined the religion-and-helping correlation.
Gallup Polls, for example, periodically assess the role of religion in helping. Among people Gallup surveyed in 1984, those who were highly spiritually committed were more than twice as likely to be currently working in giving service to the elderly, poor, or otherwise needy as those who were highly uncommitted. This pattern has held consistent in follow-up studies (Colasanto, 1989; Wuthnow, 1994). Other research has found that religion is more strongly associated with planned helping, as when people consider helping an AIDS program (Omoto, Snyder, & Berghuis, 1993) or other types of volunteer service organizations (Clary & Snyder, 1991, 1993). But the help religious people extend to the needy apparently has its limits, particularly when the person in need exhibits behavior that violates a religious standard (Jackson & Esses, 1997; Thurston, 2000). Here also the religious keep their distance from outgroup members. The role of “faith-based organizations” in providing services to people in need also awaits further study (Cnaan, 2002).
Contributions of people’s time and money to charities represents another area that has interested researchers. Americans donate about 1% of their incomes to religious charities and about 1% to other causes (Myers, 1992). Some suggest that much religious giving essentially serves the function of “club membership fees” rather than the function of charitable acts (Argyle, 2000). Indeed, an entire literature has grown up to examine the time “spent” in religious endeavors in the context of various “rational choice” or economic theories (Iannaccone, 1997; Young, 1996).
It is true that religious people give to religious organizations to further religious ends. But this giving is not only monetary; it also includes volunteer time. This time is estimated to be equivalent, on average, to approximately 40% of the value of their monetary contributions (Hoge, Zech, McNamara, & Donahue, 1998). To the extent that religiousness serves as a unique force to inhibit a wide variety of behaviors that are considered problematic—crime, premarital pregnancy, alcoholism, substance abuse—and does so in a way that government is forbidden to do—by instilling religious faith—then, even if indirectly, such giving is no less charitable and perhaps at least as effective as donations of time, treasure, and talent to secular charities.

HONESTY

Empirical studies of lying and religion lead to two primary conclusions. The first is thatthere are relatively few published studies assessing the impact of religiosity on lying. For example, a PsycINFO search for articles published during the past 20 years that link reli-gion with either lying or deception revealed only 34 articles, the vast majority of which deal with social desirability, psychohistory, or clinical, therapeutic concerns. The second is that although religiosity appears to be the best predictor of attitudes about honesty (Katz, Santman, & Lonero, 1994), religious respondents are sometimes, but not consistently, less likely to cheat or engage in deception than their nonreligious peers, despite a nearly universal religious injunction against dishonesty (Grasmick, Bursik, & Cochran, 1991; Perrin, 2000; Smith, Wheeler, & Diener, 1975). Indeed, one study even reported that children who attended a religious school may have cheated more frequently than those who attended a secular school (Guttman, 1984). What little research there is finds no consistent difference in the degree to which members of various religions (e.g., Christianity, Hinduism, Islam, and Jainism) value honesty (Kothari, 1994; Wolfe & Mourribi, 1985).
Nearly all such studies, however, have been conducted in school settings and so offer even less empirical evidence regarding the relation between religiousness and cheating or lying among adults than they do among students. Furthermore, from a psychological perspective, most studies of honesty have treated religion in a relatively unsophisticated way.
This fact, combined with the inconsistent results in the area, invites additional research.
For example, there is great potential for experimental studies that would enable researchers to examine the effect of religiousness conceived as a personality variable alongside situational inducements to perform an honest or a dishonest act. Given the paucity of experimental studies in the psychology of religion, and the core assumption that religion affects individuals’ honesty, this is an area sorely needing research attention.

SEXUALITY

If there is a single issue about which psychology and religion are perceived to be most at odds, it must surely be the area of sexuality. Shea (1992) asserts that by a conservative “estimate the number of castrations, whippings, incarcerations, burnings . . . and other executions attributable directly to [Christianity’s hostility to sex] to be in the millions. . . .[And they] continue to the present time” (p. 70). Shea offers no citations to support this assertion, simply stating it as if it was self-validating. Aside from the patent absurdity of the statement that they “continue to the present time,” even the statement that they were once common is no longer considered tenable. Stark’s (2003) review of recent historical research, and his own archival analyses, indicate that many beliefs about such a vicious and violent past are modern-day stereotypes, largely the result of the biases of certain 19th- and early 20th-century historians. Such presumptions likewise ignore the place of the medical profession, as opposed to religion, in advancing sexual repression while “Christianity gave America an ethic of sexual pleasure” (Gardella, 1985).
So if we can allow that the influence of religion on sexuality is not one of brutal repression, what does the research tell us about the relation between the two? The effect of religiosity on sexuality has been examined in many studies, frequently with nationally representative samples. For example, Cochran and Beeghley (1991) examined a subset of 15,000 U.S. respondents from the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) surveys conducted between 1971 and 1989. Analyses showed a strong (r= .51) correlation between religious commitment and belief that extramarital sex is wrong. Conservative denominations were more likely to condemn extramarital intercourse than were mainline denominations; Jews reported the least condemnation of extramarital sex. Within de-nominations, the correlation between religious commitment and condemnation of extramarital intercourse also varies in a manner roughly consistent with the degree to which the denomination is at odds with the dominant culture: stronger correlations between religious commitment and condemnation of extramarital sex were found among sects, while the weakest correlations were found for Episcopalians.
Similar patterns of correlations have been found for self-reports of engagement in premarital sex. Indeed, Benson, Donahue, and Erickson (1989) stated that major reviews that had been conducted by adolescent pregnancy researchers (e.g., Chilman, 1980; Hayes, 1987) and major nationwide interview data (Zelnik, Kantner, & Ford, 1981) indicated strong “constraining effects of religion on the likelihood of engaging in premarital intercourse” (p. 170).
Some research on this subject generates results that invite further questions. One such study surveyed over 2,700 U.S. adults and found that religious people were lesslikely to report having had extramarital affairs than were nonreligious people (Janus & Janus, 1993). Curiously, however, adults reporting themselves to be “religious” were less likely to have had affairs (26%) than were adults who reported themselves to be “very religious” (31%). This finding deserves some pursuit by researchers studying the connection between religious attitudes and social behavior, as a variety of social and psychological mechanisms might be at work.
The topic of sexual orientation and religion has begun to be investigated, but substantial gaps remain in our knowledge of this issue. While it is generally found that religious orthodoxy or conservatism is associated with greater prejudice toward homosexuals (Morrison & Morrison, 2002), the more interesting questions concern the psychological dynamics for this relation. One explanation for this effect is that religious groups unwittingly exacerbate a natural “us versus them” mentality that heightens prejudice toward people who are seen as threatening to the group (Altemeyer, 2003). Another promising explanation for the effect focuses on how the content of an individual’s beliefs accounts for prejudice toward homosexuals (Laythe, Finkel, & Kirkpatrick, 2001). Of course, these are not necessarily incompatible hypotheses, and we look forward to research that addresses them.
Another area worthy of further examination is the types of spiritual conflicts experienced by homosexuals (Buchanan, Dzelme, Harris, & Hecker, 2001; Rodriguez & Ouellette, 2000). Data from one small-scale study suggests that spiritual conflict affects approximately two-thirds of all gays and lesbians (Schuck & Liddle, 2001), but the extent to which this problem actually occurs remains unknown because we lack reliable statistical estimates garnered from large samples. Likewise, these analyses have not adequately addressed the extent to which cognitive dissonance theory, social identity theory, or other frameworks might best account for people’s experiences with sexual orientation and religion.

RELIGION-RELATED ABUSE

Is religion a risk factor for child abuse? Vivid reports in the 1980s of “satanic ritual abuse” and of ritualistic sacrifice of infants were later found to be spurious (Richardson, Best, & Bromley, 1990). Curiously, a small number of therapists reported a relatively large number of such incidents among their clients; most therapists never worked with even a single client who claimed to be a victim of satanic abuse (Bottoms, Shaver, Good-man, & Qin, 1995). More recent headlines have focused on sexual abuse of children by Catholic priests. There is no denying that terrible incidents of abuse did occur, and that there may have been cases of malfeasance by bishops to preserve the reputation of the Catholic Church. But one of the most striking findings of the National Review Board for the Protection of Children and Young People (2004) established to examine the scandal was the lack of research into relevant questions. Is the rate of abuse by Catholic priests more or less than the rate of abuse by ministers in Protestant denominations? More or less than the rates among men in similar positions of authority, such as Boy Scout leaders or teachers? Some 80% of the priests engaged in sexual contact with postpubertal boys, an act technically known asephebophilia, rather thanpedophilia. Research indicates that the clinical profiles of ephebophiles and pedophiles differ markedly, and the two terms should not be interchanged (McGlone, 2004). The commission of these crimes peaked in 1980, with a major decline since then. Is this somehow related to Catholic Church history or was there such a pattern in society at large? No one knows the answers to these questions because there is little or no relevant research.
Child physical abuse has received somewhat more research attention but remains little understood. The most consistent effect appears to be that fundamentalist religious beliefs are associated with a greater likelihood of violence among Jewish (Shor, 1998) and
Christian (Ellison, Bartkowski, & Segal, 1996) families. The effects of such abuse are just now beginning to be the subject of empirical research. Compared to a group of victims whose abuse did not involve religion, victims of religion-related physical abuse showed greater levels of depression, anxiety, hostility, psychoticism, and other psychological problems years later (Bottoms, Nielsen, Murray, & Filipas, 2004). To the extent that these data are replicated in other samples, they suggest that when abuse is connected to religion negative effects are compounded. Additional research is sorely needed so that we can better understand the extent of the problem, the psychological mechanisms by which it occurs, and the possibility that child sexual abuse, religion-related medical neglect, and other forms of abuse might show similar effects. Theoretical perspectives such as attachment theory (Kirkpatrick, 1997) and the role of God in coping (Pargament, 2001) could be useful in explaining the long-term effects of religion-related abuse (Bottoms et al., 2004). In the case of attachment theory, the notion of God as an attachment figure who substitutes for weak parental attachments would suggest that the person who suffers religion-related abuse is likely to be deprived of a close attachment to God as well as to parents. Pargament’s research on religious coping suggests that the victim would be deprived of the significant positive effects of using God as a resource for coping with distressful events. These theories provide readily testable hypotheses for researchers investigating religion-related child abuse, whether physical or sexual in nature.

CRIME AND PUNISHMENT

By definition, criminal acts are antisocial. Research addressing religion’s role in promoting or inhibiting crime has a long history. A recent meta-analysis of the area (Baier & Wright, 2001) examined 79 effect sizes across 60 studies. They found that “the mean reported effect size wasr= –.12, and the median wasr= –.11 . . . none of [the correlations reported] were positive. . . . These findings show that religious behavior and beliefs exert a significant, moderate deterrent effect on individuals’ criminal behavior” (p. 14).
The authors also went on to examine some related hypotheses. One is the hypothesis that the deterrent effect of religion is increased when one is immersed within a religious community; they found that the data supported this conclusion. In addition, they found that nonvictim crimes (e.g., gambling and drug use) were also more likely to be deterred by religion.
Attitudes regarding the punishment of criminals are related to religiosity. For example, 75% of Americans in general favor the death penalty, but among those who say religion is important to them the figure increases to 84% (Gallup & Lindsay, 1999). Of course, this finding speaks of religion in general, with no fine distinctions being made among denominations or specific religious beliefs. While Christian orthodoxy correlates positively with endorsement of the death penalty, we begin to see that such attitudes are malleable when we consider the case of Roman Catholics living in the United States. Recent research conducted by Bjarnasson and Welch (2004) found that church attendance by Catholics is positively correlated with endorsement of Cardinal Bernadin’s (1984) statement regarding “a seamless garment” on “life issues,” which denounces both capital punishment and abortion. After Bernandin’s framing of the issue in this way, U. S. Catholic support for capital punishment changed markedly. Whereas Catholics were generally more likely than non-Catholics to support the death penalty during the early 1970s, this difference has declined, particularly among parishioners who were highly integrated into their parish (Bjarnasson & Welch, 2004). This pattern also is consistent with the notion that one’s social identity helps guide individual attitudes.
Many different theoretical viewpoints are available for application to these issues.
Whether the subject is crime, family violence, or prosocial behavior, analytic perspectives can draw from a range of theories that emphasize the “micro,” such as cognitive dissonance theory, or the “macro,” such as rational choice theory. Using such a multilevel approach to do this promises at least two important benefits: it can improve our understanding of social behavior and religion, and it can facilitate interdisciplinary dialogue because of the wide variety of theories available. For example, psychological (e.g., theories of attitude–behavior consistency, social identity theory) and socioeconomic (e.g., rational choice theory) approaches could be examined profitably in the context of volunteerism. Ideally, these approaches could be examined jointly in order to gain a more nuanced understanding of the way individual- and social-level influences combine to account for people’s behavior.

PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION, POLITICS, AND PEACE

The psychological study of religion sits at a crossroads between psychology and religious studies. Social psychology is at a similar crossroads with other social sciences, and this fact presents the opportunity for cross-fertilization among these disciplines. We focus now on the relevance of the psychology of religion on two such areas of inquiry.

Religion and Politics
The political and religious spheres are often tightly intertwined. Psychological theories can be helpful in understanding such interconnections. As one example, consider the finding that in 1990, before the demise of the Soviet Union, merely 15% of Ukrainians identified themselves as Orthodox Christians. Seven years later, however, after Ukraine established its independence, 70% of Ukrainians so identified themselves (Kolodny, 1997).
Such an enormous change in religious identity illustrates the powerful interconnections among religious, political, and national identities.
Political leaders also use religion in order to garner and maintain power. For example, most Middle East studies scholars agree that the House of Saud endorses Wahabbism in order to maintain its power (Esposito, 1987). Likewise, most U.S. political scientists agree that George W. Bush reaches out to Evangelical Christians because they provide him with an important base of support (Rozell, 2003). Just as religion can serve political ends, so too can politics serve an individual’s religious goals. Recent survey data illustrate this point. Most Americans (79%) agree with the notion of keeping church and state separate, but conservative Protestants and Evangelicals, a significant portion of the U.S. populace, desire religion to have a greater influence on the U.S. political scene, whereas non-Evangelicals do not (Gallup & Lindsay, 1999).
Among the more informative projects in this area is the Clergy Study Project, which examined the roles of clergy in Judaism, Unitarian-Universalism, and in 16 Christian religious bodies in the 2000 national election (Smith, 2004). The majority of clergy engaged in some form of political activity, including delivering sermons on politics, organizing study groups, or performing some form of activist work on behalf of candidates. Whether they were conservative Evangelicals who view the world as being in a state of moral decline (Guth et al., 2003) or Unitarian-Universalists working toward a more liberal social–political agenda (Green, 2004), the clergy viewed their efforts as a natural part of their moral obligation to be involved in society. Thus, religious belief expresses itself as engagement with society and with political structures. This effect is moderated, however, by the degree to which an individual’s religion is accepted by society. In a separate study, U. S. Hindus, Buddhists, and Muslims expressed a high degree of alienation from society and were less likely to become involved in political activity (Wuthnow & Hackett, 2004).
History abounds with examples of the religious affecting the political, and vice versa.
Psychological theories regarding attitude–behavior consistency, leadership, and decision making have much to contribute to our understanding of the way in which individuals’ attitudes and values shape their decisions and are quite relevant to questions concerning when and how people construe religion and politics, and how they maintain separate versus combined goals in those spheres. As we seek to understand these phenomena, however, we must extend our knowledge base to include other disciplines, either by independent study or, more preferably, by working with colleagues in other disciplines. By drawing from the expertise of colleagues in disciplines such as political science or sociology, and by integrating their broad, “macro” analyses with more “micro” psychological theories, we will add breadth and depth to our understanding of the ways in which religion and other social institutions affect people. We also can gain insight into the underlying question of religion as a means or as an end, a question that has been a prime concern at least since Allport articulated the I/Etypology (Batson, Schoenrade, & Ventis, 1993).

Religion, Peace, Conflict, and War
Significant implications exist for religious attitudes and beliefs on people’s views regarding war. Indeed, the U.S. Naval War College recognizes this connection, offering an elective course titled “Faith and Force: Religion, War and Peace.” This is an important, yet generally neglected, area of study by psychologists of religion (but see Silberman, Chapter 29, this volume, for a discussion of religious terrorism). A good base for examining these relations is found in the work of Christie, Wagner, and Winter (2001), who distinguish between direct and structural violence.Direct violenceentails actions that directly, immediately, and adversely affect another person’s life. It is intentional, dramatic, and can kill people outright, as in cases of war or hate crimes.Structural violenceis more indirect and chronic, and results in long-term adverse effects that decrease one’s lifespan, often dramatically.
Recent history offers examples of religious bodies and individuals advocating direct violence for religious reasons (Juergensmeyer, 2003). Religious leaders also may advocate support of war, as when the Southern Baptist Convention president announced, “We will enlist prayer warriors as special forces to pray for our troops and their families” (Graham, 2003). When leaders encourage people to draw connections between the religious and the martial by using religious language to describe the righteousness of their cause or to describe the enemy as evil, however, such “tough talk” promotes authoritarianism and polarization in a conflict, working against the prospects for a peaceful end to the conflict (Pettigrew, 2003).
The relation between religion and structural violence can be more subtle than that between religion and direct violence. To the extent that societal resources are allocated inequitably, structural violence is being done (Christie et al., 2001). Religions that support such inequities would be considered as contributing to structural violence. White supremacist Christianity in the United States and extremist interpretations of Islam in Afghanistan or Saudi Arabia, among others, illustrate religion’s role in maintaining structural violence.
Efforts to reduce direct violence are known as peacemaking. While such work takes different forms, its proponents advocate nonviolent means to reduce direct violence; they react to specific events; they act in a defined time at a defined place; and they tend not to disrupt the current power structure (Christie et al., 2001). Religion can play an importantrole in understanding the peaceful resolution of conflict. On an institutional level, religions may issue statements (Atlanta Journal-Constitution, 2003), hold vigils, and make other efforts to convey their message to political leaders and to the public (e.g., National Council of Churches, 2003). Although such peacemaking efforts may be associated with relatively liberal forms of religion, peacemaking is also evident in religions that are conservative, as was the case when The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints helped to defeat the MX intercontinental ballistic missile system in the 1980s (Nielsen, 2004). Religion also can be an important element contributing to dramatic and heroic personal interventions in the midst of war (Oliner & Oliner, 1988).
Actions to address structural violence are known as peace building. Many religions encourage such efforts, which may be consistent with their stated mission in social outreach activities and represent important expressions of religious belief and attitudes. Examples of peace building at an institutional level include the many Roman Catholic pastoral letters and encyclicals written during the past century that call for the fair and equitable distribution of the world’s resources, equal access to political power, social justice, and fairness (Pennock, 2000). The 2003 World Council of Churches statement advocating peace through passive resistance, education, and other means also illustrates principles of peace building. On an individual level, religious peace building is exemplified by Mohandas Gandhi, whose promotion of peace drew from his ecumenism. Gandhi’s use of civil disobedience to achieve civil rights inspired Martin Luther King, A. J. Muste, and many others (Barash & Webel, 2002; Muste, 1952/2002).
For psychologists of religion, people’s efforts in peacemaking and peace building represent a prime opportunity to examine important, tangible effects of religious belief. Po-tentially useful theoretical perspectives for such research are truly diverse, including moral judgment, social exchange theory, theories regarding norms, social influence theory, social learning theory, conflict resolution, and many others. For example, stereotyping research could examine the effect on people’s beliefs of learning that Palestinian “suicide bombers” are often educated, middle-class, and without deep religious commitment (Pettigrew, 2003). Religion can play an important part in people’s efforts to cope with conflict, as it did during the contentious overthrow of Philippine president Joseph Estrada in 2001 (Macapagal & Nario-Galace, 2003; for more on religious coping, see Pargament, Ano, & Wachholtz, Chapter 26, this volume). Additional research into religion’s roles in fomenting and resolving conflict and war is warranted, and would be an important contribution that psychology of religion can make to psychology.

POSSIBLE THEORETICAL FRAMEWORKS

We have described a wide range of subjects that have received, and in our view should continue to receive, research attention from psychologists of religion. There are several different theoretical frameworks that may be useful in conducting this research. We discuss a few here, recognizing that this list is brief and that there are many others that would be useful. As we improve our understanding of psychology and religion by pursuing research in these areas, we also can promote the study of religious behavior and belief by other psychologists. We move from examples illustrating distinctly “psychological” issues to those that are more sociological and philosophical.
One core issue in social psychology concerns the degree to which attitudes and behavior are consistent (Bagozzi & Burnkrant, 1979; Fazio, Herr, & Olney, 1984; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1974; Kahle & Berman, 1974; Ostrom, 1969; Salancik & Conway, 1975; Wicker, 1969, 1971; Zanna, Olson, & Fazio, 1980). Some of the classic studies relevant to the psychology of religion, such as When Prophecy Fails(Festinger, Riecken, & Schachter, 1956), have been based on relevant theories such as cognitive dissonance theory. While there has already been interesting and insightful research using this paradigm, its value is by no means exhausted for the psychology of religion. The same is true of related questions dealing with attitude–behavior causality and attitude change. Attitude researchers have used religion as a content area in which to test out various theories and processes, but in our view this area of research is far from exhausted.
Social identity theory is adopted by many involved in peace psychology (e.g., Christie et al., 2001), and may be of value for psychologists of religion. It appears in theI/E/Q paradigm as a social-extrinsic religious orientation (Kirkpatrick, 1989), although most attention has focused on intrinsicness instead. Social identity theory also resonates with some recent writing in the psychology of religion, such as Buddhist psychology (de Silva, 2000). From this view, people’s alienation from society, or “identity crisis,” ultimately generates a pathological society. Careful examination of the ways that religion affects identity and may be used to promote positive or harmful social interactions is warranted, as the effects appear to be vitally important (Keen, 1986).
Sociology of religion has devoted a great deal of attention to the theory of secularization: that religious adherence declines as societies become more technologically advanced.
Support for this theory, while not completely gone (Bruce, 2001), has declined of late, as typified by the title of Stark’s (1999) article, “Secularization, R.I.P.” Similarly, the active involvement of fundamentalist groups in changing societal structures (Marty & Appleby,1993a, 1993b), argues against the Marxist “opiate of the masses” image. But whether it is religious decline, religious ferment, or religious scandal, change in the public face of religion is highly relevant to the psychology of religion in terms of people’s doubts and uncertainty (see Paloutzian, Chapter 18, this volume). A social-psychological understanding of religion is incomplete without examining how such processes apply to the individual as well as to society.
Conservatism, humanism, and other value systems provide a foundation for the psychology of religion, and for religious individuals themselves (see Geyer & Baumeister, Chapter 23, this volume). Despite this fact, the relation between values, attitudes, and religion remains a subject that has received less attention by psychologists than it deserves.
Indeed, a PsycINFO search combining these terms returned merely two dozen entries, fewof which were recent. Conceptual analyses of the differences between values and attitudes, the translation of general values to specific attitudes, and the various expressions of values and attitudes in everyday life all warrant further research. Religion provides an excellent context for doing this.

CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS

Religion’s impact on social life is perhaps the most vigorous area of study in the psychology of religion, not only in terms of the number of studies conducted, but in terms of the range of phenomena and relevant theories. In this brief survey, we have seen that religion affects social attitudes and behavior in myriad ways. Prejudice and helping, honesty and sexuality, child abuse and other crimes, and politics and peace all are highly impacted by one’s religious beliefs and behaviors. These results point to the complex nature of religion in people’s lives and in society—on the one hand, religion can promote prejudice, intolerance, and war. On the other hand, it can promote understanding, tolerance, and peace.
Working out the details concerning when religion does each will undoubtedly continue to occupy researchers for years to come.
Just as the social implications of religion are diverse, so too should be the methods and theoretical perspectives of the psychologists who study them. By using different methods to examine attitudes and social behavior, researchers improve our measurement of constructs and better establish validity. Social psychology and related disciplines offer numerous methods readily applicable to the topics covered in this chapter (see Hood & Belzen, Chapter 4, this volume; Reis & Judd, 2000). Likewise, researchers can draw from a wide variety of theories selected from psychology and other relevant disciplines. Because religion represents an intersection of many interests, examining its social implications necessarily requires a willingness to consider divergent theoretical and even disciplinary perspectives. Studying religion, for example, by using theI/E/Q paradigm and variations of the “lost letter” technique, while also doing content analyses of material published by various religions or denominations, will serve the psychology of religion far better than reliance on only one research method or theoretical perspective. Depending on one’s focus, theories from fields as diverse as anthropology, criminology, and political science would be useful in such research, although they are rather rarely used at present. Forming partnerships with colleagues in those disciplines who share an interest in religious phenomena is a fruitful way to begin such work.
Finally, the importance of the issues examined in this chapter can be seen on at least two levels beyond the obvious goal of advancing the psychology of religion. First, ad-dressing the social aspects of religion can also enhance our understanding of basic psychological matters, as Festinger et al. (1956) demonstrated. Perhaps more importantly, because they also deal with significant societal issues, the topics addressed in this chapter ultimately can exert a significant practical effect on people and on society. If there is a common theme to the research in this area, it must be that religion engages others, whether for better or for ill.


REFERENCES


Allport, G. W. (1979).The nature of prejudice(unabridged, 25th anniv. ed.). Reading, MA: AddisonWesley. (Original work published 1954)
Allport, G. W., & Ross, M. J. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,5, 432–443.
Altemeyer, B. (2003). Why do religious fundamentalists tend to be prejudiced?The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion,13, 17–28.
Argyle, M. (2000).Psychology and religion: An introduction. New York: Routledge.
Atlanta Journal-Constitution. (2003, March 22). Statements on war by religious institutions and leaders, p. B2.
Bagozzi, R., & Burnkrant, R. E. (1979). Attitude organization and the attitude–behavior relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,37, 913–929.
Baier, C. J., & Wright, B. R. E. (2001). “If you love me, keep my commandments”: A meta-analysis of the effect of religion on crime.Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency,38, 3–21.
Bailey, S. D. (2000). Religious orientation and the expression of racial prejudice among graduate students in the field of psychology.Dissertation Abstracts International-B,61, 3048.
Barash, D. P., & Webel, C. P. (2002).Peace and conflict studies.Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bassett, R. L., Camplin, W., Humphrey, D., Dorr, C., Biggs, S., Distaffen, R., et al. (1991). Measuring Christian maturity: A comparison of several scales.Journal of Psychology and Theology,19, 84–93.
Batson, C. D., Eidelman, S. H., Higley, S. L., & Russell, S. A. (2001). “And who is my neighbor?” II: Quest religion as a source of universal compassion.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 40, 39–50.
Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L. (1993)Religion and the individual: A social-psychological perspective.New York: Oxford University Press.
Batson, C. D., & Ventis, W. L. (1985). Misconception of quest: A reply to Hood and Morris.Review of Religious Research,26, 398–407.
Beck, R., & Miller, C. D. (2000). Religiosity and agency and communion: The relation to religious judgmentalism.Journal of Psychology,134, 315–324.
Benson, P. L., Donahue, M. J., & Erickson, J. A. (1989). Adolescence and religion: A review of the literature from 1970 to 1986.Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion,1, 151–179.
Bernadin, J. L. (1984).The seamless garment.Kansas City, MO: National Catholic Reporter.
Bjarnasson, T., & Welch, M. R. (2004). Father knows best: Parishes, priests and American Catholic parishioners’ attitudes toward capital punishment.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 43, 103–118.
Bottoms, B. L., Nielsen, M. E., Murray, R., & Filipas, H. (2004). Religion related child physical abuse:  AlthoughDissertation Abstracts Internationalcitations are given in the reference list, full-text versions (rather than abstracts) were consulted via ProQuest Digital Dissertations (www.lib.umi.com/dissertations/; accessed July 9, 2004).
Characteristics and psychological outcome.Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma, 8, 87–114.
Bottoms, B. L., Shaver, P. R., Goodman, G. S., & Qin, J. (1995). In the name of God: A profile of religion-related child abuse.Journal of Social Issues,51, 85–111
Brock, T. C. (1962). Implications of conversion and magnitude of cognitive dissonance.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,1, 198–203.
Bruce, S. (2001). Christianity in Britain, R.I.P.Sociology of Religion,62, 191–203.
Buchanan, M., Dzelme, K., Harris, D., & Hecker, L. (2001). Challenges of being simultaneously gay or lesbian and spiritual and/or religious: A narrative perspective.The American Journal of Family Therapy,29, 435–449.
Bulman, R. J., & Wortman, C. B. (1977). Attributions of blame and coping in the real world: Severe accident victims react to their lot.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 351–363.
Cannon, C. E. (2001). The influence of religious orientation and white racial identity on expressions of prejudice.Dissertation Abstracts International-B,62, 598.
Chilman, C. S. (1980).Adolescent sexuality in a changing American society: Social and psychological perspectives.Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.
Christie, D. J., Wagner, R. V., & Winter, D. D. N. (2001).Peace, conflict and violence: Peace psychology for the 21st century.Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (1991). A functional analysis of altruism and prosocial behavior: The case of volunteerism. In M. Clark (Ed.),Prosocial behavior(pp. 119–148). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (1993). Persuasive communications strategies for recruiting volunteers. In D. R. Young, R. M. Hollister, & V. A. Hodgkinson (Eds.),Governing, leading, and managing nonprofit organizations(pp. 121–137). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Cnaan, R. (2002).The invisible caring hand: American congregations and the provision of welfare. New York: New York University Press.
Cochran, J. K., & Beeghley, L. (1991). The influence of religion on attitudes toward nonmarital sexuality: A preliminary assessment of reference group theory.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,30, 45–62.
Colasanto, D. (1989, November). Americans show commitment to helping those in need.Gallup Report, No. 290, 17–24.
Connolly, P. (1999). Psychological approaches. In P. Connolly (Ed.),Approaches to the study of religion(pp. 135–192). London: Continuum.
de Silva, P. (2000).An introduction to Buddhist psychology(3rd ed.). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
Dittes, J. E. (1971). Typing the typologies: Some parallels in the career of church-sect and extrinsic–intrinsic.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,10, 375–383
Donahue, M. J. (1985). Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness: Review and meta-analysis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,48, 400–419.
Ellison, C. G., Bartkowski, J. P., & Segal, M. L. (1996). Conservative Protestantism and the parental use of corporal punishment.Social Forces,74, 1003–1028.
Esposito, J. L. (1987).Islam and politics(4th ed.). Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University.
Fazio, R. H., Herr, P. M., & Olney, T. J. (1984). Attitude accessibility following a self-perception process.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,47, 277–286.
Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956).When prophecy fails: A social and psychological study of a modern group that predicted the destruction of the world.Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1974). Attitudes towards objects as predictors of single and multiple behavioral criteria.Psychological Bulletin,81, 59–74.
Fulton, A. S., Gorsuch, R. L., & Maynard, E. A. (1999). Religious orientation, antihomosexual sentiment, and fundamentalism among Christians.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 38, 14–22.
Gallup, G., Jr., & Lindsay, D. M. (1999).Surveying the religious landscape: Trends in U.S. beliefs. Harrisburg, PA: Morehouse.
Gardella, P. (1985).Innocent ecstasy: How Christianity gave America an ethic of sexual pleasure. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gorsuch, R. L., & Aleshire, D. (1974). Christian faith and ethnic prejudice: A review and interpretation of research.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,13, 281–307.
Gorsuch, R. L., & McPherson, S. E. (1983). Intrinsic/extrinsic measurement: I/E-revised and singleitem scales.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,28, 348–354.
Graham, J. (2003). Comments on war in Iraq, March 20, 2003. Accessed March 29, 2003, at www.prestonwood.org/sites/document.asp?=did3100.
Grasmick, H. G., Bursik, R. J., Jr., & Cochran, J. K. (1991). “Render unto Caesar what is Caesar’s”: Religiosity and taxpayers’ inclinations to cheat.Sociological Quarterly,32, 251–266.
Green, J. C. (2004). A liberal dynamo: The political activism of the Unitarian-Universalist clergy. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,42, 577–590.
Guth, J. L., Beail, L., Crow, G., Gaddy, B., Montreal, S., Nelsen, B., et al. (2003). The political activity of evangelical clergy in the election of 2000: A case study of five denominations.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,42, 501–514.
Guttman, J. (1984). Cognitive morality and cheating behavior in religious and secular school children.Journal of Educational Research,77, 249–254.
Hayes, C. D. (Ed.). (1987).Risking the future: Adolescent sexuality, pregnancy, and childbearing. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Herek, G. M. (1987). Religious orientation and prejudice: A comparison of racial and sexual attitudes.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,13, 34–44.
Hoge, D. R., Zech, C. E., McNamara, P. H., & Donahue, M. J. (1998). The value of volunteers as resources for congregations.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,37, 470–480.
Hood, R. W., Jr., & Morris, R. J. (1985). Conceptualization of quest: A critical rejoinder to Batson. Review of Religious Research,26, 391–397.
Hunt, R. A., & King, M. B. (1971). The intrinsic–extrinsic concept: A review and evaluation.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,10, 339–356.
Iannaccone, L. R. (1997). Skewness explained: A rational choice model of religious giving.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,36, 141–157.
Jackson, L. M., & Esses, V. M. (1997). Of Scripture and ascription: The relation between religious fundamentalism and intergroup helping.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,23, 893–906.
James, W. (1985).The varieties of religious experiences: A study in human nature.New York: Penguin Classics. (Original work published 1902)
Janus, S. S., & Janus, C. L. (1993).The Janus report. New York: Wiley.
Juergensmeyer, M. (2003).Terror in the mind of God: The global rise of religious violence(3rd ed.). Berkeley: University of California Press.
Kahle, L. R., & Berman, J. J. (1974). Attitudes cause behaviors: A cross-lagged panel analysis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,37, 315–321.
Katz, R. C., Santman, J., & Lonero, P. (1994). Findings on the revised Morally Debatable Behaviors Scale.Journal of Psychology,128, 15–21.
Keen, S. (1986).Faces of the enemy: Reflections of the hostile imagination. San Francisco: Harper &  Row.
Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1989). A psychometric analysis of the Allport–Ross and Feagin measures of intrinsic–extrinsic religious orientation. Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion, 2, 3–28.
Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Hood, R. W., Jr. (1990). Intrinsic–extrinsic religious orientation: The boon or bane of contemporary psychology of religion?Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 29, 442–462.
Kolodny, A. N. (1997, September).National churches in the context of national renaissance of Ukraine. Paper presented at the conference Religion at the End of the Twentieth Century, Sevastopol, Ukraine.
Kothari, S. (1994). Impact of religion upon development of moral concepts.Psycho-Lingua, 24(2), 65–72.
Lam, P.-Y. (2001). May the force of the operating system be with you: Macintosh devotion as implicit religion.Sociology of Religion,62, 243–262.
Laythe, B., Finkel, D., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2001). Predicting prejudice from religious fundamentalism and right-wing authoritarianism: A multiple-regression approach.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,40, 1–10.
Lundblad, R. T. (2002). Social, religious, and personal contributors to prejudice.Dissertation Abstracts International-B,63, 589.
Lupton, H. E. (1986). Use of the notion “implicit religion” in psychological study: A discussion paper. In J. A. Belzen & J. M. van der Lans (Eds.),Current issues in the psychology of religion: Proceedings of the Third Symposium on the Psychology of Religion in Europe(pp. 44–55). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Rodopi.
Macapagal, M. E. J., & Nario-Galace, J. (2003). Social psychology of People Power II in the Philippines.Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology,9, 219–233.
Marty, M. E., & Appleby, R. S. (Eds.). (1993a).Fundamentalisms and society: Reclaiming the sciences, the family, and education. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Marty, M. E., & Appleby, R. S. (Eds.). (1993b).Fundamentalisms and the state: Remaking polities, economies, and militance. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
McGlone, G. J. (2004). The pedophile and the pious: Towards a new understanding of sexually offending and non-offending Roman Catholic priests.Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma,8, 115–131.
Morrison, M. A., & Morrison, T. G. (2002). Development and validation of a scale measuring modern prejudice toward gay men and lesbian women.Journal of Homosexuality,43, 15–27.
Muste, A. J. (2002).Of holy disobedience.Wallingford, PA: Pendle Hill. (Original work published 1952)
Myers, D. M. (1992).The pursuit of happiness.New York: Morrow.
National Council of Churches. (2003).Taking action to avert war. Accessed March 17, 2003, at www.ncccusa.org.
National Review Board for the Protection of Children and Young People. (2004).A report on the crisis in the Catholic Church in the United States.Washington, DC: United States Conference of Catholic Bishops.
Nielsen, M. E. (2004, Spring). Mormonism and psychology: A broader vision for peace.Dialogue: A Journal of Mormon Thought,37(1), 109–132.
Oliner, S. P., & Oliner, P. M. (1988).The altruistic personality: Rescuers of Jews in Nazi Europe.New York: Free Press.
Omoto, A. M., Snyder, M., & Berghuis, J. P. (1993). The psychology of volunteerism: A conceptual analysis and a program of action research. In J. B. Pryor & G. D. Reeder (Eds.),The social psychology of HIV infection(pp. 333–356). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ostrom, T. M. (1969). The relationship between the affective, behavioral, and cognitive components of attitude.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,5, 12–30.
Pargament, K. I. (2001).The psychology of religious coping: Theory, research, practice. New York: Guilford Press.
Pennock, M. (2000).Catholic social teaching: Learning and living justice. New York: Ave Maria Press.
Perrin, R. D. (2000). Religiosity and honesty: Continuing the search for the consequential dimension. Review of Religious Research,41, 534–544.
Pettigrew, T. F. (2003). Peoples under threat: Americans, Arabs, and Israelis.Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology,9, 69–90.
Reis, H. T., & Judd, C. M. (Eds.). (2000).Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Richardson, J. T., Best, J., & Bromley, D. G. (Eds.). (1991).The satanism scare.New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Rodriguez, E. M., & Ouellette, S. C. (2000). Gay and lesbian Christians: Homosexual and religious identity integration in the members of a gay-positive church.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,39, 333–347.
Rozell, M. J. (2003). Evangelicals inside the beltway.Religion in the 2004 Election[Special supplement].Religion in the News,6(3), pp. 6, 18.
Salancik, G. R., & Conway, M. (1975). Attitude inferences from salient and relevant cognitive content about behavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,32, 827–840.
Schuck, K. D., & Liddle, B. J. (2001). Religious conflicts experienced by lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals.Journal of Gay & Lesbian Psychotherapy,5, 63–82.
Shea, J. D. (1992). Religion and sexual adjustment. In J. F. Schumaker (Ed.),Religion and mental health(pp. 70–84). New York: Oxford University Press.
Shor, R. (1998). The significance of religion in advancing a culturally sensitive approach towards child maltreatment.Families in Society,79, 400–409.
Slone, D. J. (2004).Theological incorrectness: Why religious people believe what they shouldn’t.New York: Oxford University Press.
Smith, C. (Ed.). (2004). Clergy as political activists [Special section].Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,42, 495–604.
Smith, R. E., Wheeler, G., & Diener, E. (1975). Faith without works: Jesus people, resistance to temptation, and altruism.Journal of Applied Social Psychology,5, 320–330.
Spilka, B., Hood, R. W., Jr., Hunsberger, B., & Gorsuch, R. L. (2003).The psychology of religion: An empirical approach(3rd ed.) New York: Guilford Press.
Stark, R. (1999). Secularization, R.I.P.Sociology of Religion,60, 249–273.
Stark, R. (2003).For the glory of God: How monotheism led to reformations, science, witch-hunts, and the end of slavery. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Taylor, T. S. (2000). Is God good for you, good for your neighbor?: The influence of religious orientation on demoralization and attitudes towards lesbians and gay men.Dissertation Abstracts International-A,60, 4472.
Thurston, N. S. (2000). Evangelical and fundamentalist protestants. In P. S. Richards & A. E. Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and religious diversity(pp. 131–153). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The relationship of verbal and overt responses to attitude objects.Journal of Social Issues,25, 41–78.
Wicker, A. W. (1971). An examination of the “other variables” explanation of attitude–behavior inconsistency.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,19, 18–30.
Wolfe, G., & Mourribi, A. (1985). A comparison of the value systems of Lebanese Christian and Muslim men and women.Journal of Social Psychology,125, 781–782.
Wuthnow, R. (1994).God and mammon in America.New York: Free Press.
Wuthnow, R., & Hackett, C. (2004). The social integration of practitioners of non-Western religions in the United States.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,42, 651–668.
Young, L. (1996).Rational choice theory and religion: Summary and assessment. New York: Routledge.
Zanna, M. P., Olson, J. M., & Fazio, R. H. (1980). Attitude–behavior consistency: An individual difference perspective.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,38, 432–440.
Zelnik, M., Kantner, J., & Ford, K. (1981).Sex and pregnancy in adolescence.Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.





********************************************************************************
HANDBOOK OF THE PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION AND SPIRITUALITY--- RAYMONDF. PALOUTZIAN CRYSTALL. PARK (p.216-234)

©2005 The Guilford Press
A Division of Guilford Publications, Inc.
72 Spring Street, New York, NY 10012
www.guilford.com
All rights reserved




Komentar

Postingan populer dari blog ini

Triandis’ Theory of Interpersonal Behaviour

Tahap Help

Values for Community Psychology-Nelson