10 Interpersonal Aggression 357
Interpersonal Aggression
To live without killing is a thought which could
electrify the world, if men were only capable of staying awake long enough to
let the idea soak in.
—Henry Miller
On
October 2, 2002, at around 6:00 P.M., James D. Martin was standing in the
parking lot of a Wheaton, Maryland, grocery store. He was there to buy
groceries for his church. From out of nowhere came the crack of a rifle and
moments later Martin lay dying on the ground in the parking lot. Just a few
hours later at 7:40 A.M.on October 3, 2003, James Buchanan was gunned down in
the same way while he was cutting the grass at an automobile dealership in
White Flint, Maryland. So began a shooting spree that would claim the lives of
seven more unsuspecting victims and seriously wound several others. The only
connection between the victims was that they were victims of the “Beltway
Sniper.” The victims were seemingly chosen at random. For 3 weeks the Beltway
Sniper terrorized residents of Maryland, Virginia, and Washington, D.C.
As
one might expect, the police mounted a massive hunt for the sniper. Early on,
unreliable reports and profi les led police to look for someone in a white van,
most likely a white male. For three weeks, police were stumped as the shooting
spree continued. Finally, a break in the case came when police received a tip
from a truck driver who spotted a car matching one the police were seeking in
connection with the sniper attacks. The car had a hole bored into the trunk
through which the sniper could shoot and then quickly leave the scene. The car
was a mobile sniper’s nest. Based on the tip, police arrested two individuals:
John Allen Muhammad and Lee Boyd Malvo. Muhammad was a 41-year-old Gulf War
veteran who was highly rated as a marksman. Malvo was 17 years old at the time
of the shooting. Police found a Bushmaster XM-15 rifle in Muhammad’s car and
ballistic tests showed that the rifle was used in the beltway shooting spree.
As
police began to unravel the case they discovered that there may have been more
than one motive for the killings. One motive was to extort 10 million dollars
from the U.S. government. Another was that Muhammad was going to use the random
killings to set up the murder of his ex-wife with whom he was having a custody
dispute. Whatever the motive or motives, the results remain the same: nine
people dead and several more wounded.
What
possessed Muhammad and Malvo to murder nine innocent, unsuspecting people? Were
they disturbed individuals, or were they a product of their environment? Were
they frustrated? Had they somehow learned that violence was an acceptable way
to solve one’s problems? The Beltway Sniper case also raises other important
questions. For example, what can be done to lessen the use of violence and
aggression as a form of conflict resolution? What steps can individuals and a
society take to prevent such a tragic event from occurring again? These are
some of the questions addressed in this chapter.
What
Is Aggression?
What
exactly is aggression? The term tends to generate a certain amount of
confusion, because a layperson’s concept of
aggression differs somewhat from what social psychologists study. In day-to-day
life we hear about the aggressive salesperson who will not take no for an
answer and the aggressive businessperson who stops at nothing to win a
promotion. These usages convey forceful, overbearing, or overly assertive
behavior.
Social
psychologists, however, define aggressionas any behavior that is intended to
inflict harm (whether psychological or physical) on another organism or object.
There are several important things to note about this definition. First, a
crucial element of the definition is intent: A person must have intended to
harm in order for the act to be classified as aggressive. If someone
deliberately hits a neighbor with a baseball bat during an argument, it is
considered aggressive. If the person accidentally hits the neighbor with a
baseball bat while playing ball in the yard, it is not considered aggressive.
Note,
too, that the harm intended by an aggressive act need not be physical. A navy
commander who continually sexually harasses a female subordinate, causing
stress, anxiety, and depression, may not be doing her any overt physical harm;
he is, however, causing her psychological harm. Third, aggression is not
limited to actions directed toward living organisms. Aggression also can be
directed toward inanimate objects. A person might smash the window of a
neighbor’s car in retaliation for some real or imagined conflict
with that neighbor.
This
broad definition covers a great deal of ground, but it requires further
elaboration. Using this definition, we would be tempted to liken the actions of
a police officer who kills a murder suspect in the line of duty with those of a
paid assassin who kills for profit. Because such a wide range of behavior can
be called aggressive, psychologists have defined several different types of
aggression, which we look at next.
Levels
and Types of Aggression
Clearly,
aggression exists on many different levels and is made up of several types of
behavior. All aggression, for example, does not stem from the same underlying
motives and intentions. Some, referred to as hostile aggression, stems from
angry and hostile impulses (Feshbach, 1964), and its primary goal is to inflict
injury on some person or object. For example, when a gay man named Matthew
Shepard was murdered, one of his assailants, Aaron McKinney, was apparently
angry over a purported “pass” made by Shepard toward McKinney. Acts of
aggression that stem from such emotional states are examples of hostile
aggression. Instrumental aggression stems from the desire to achieve a goal.
For example, such aggression could be involved in the desire to get rid of a
rival.
Hostile
aggression and instrumental aggression are not mutually exclusive. One can
commit an aggressive act having both underlying motives. In 1994, when Baruch
Goldstein killed over 30 Palestinians in a mosque in Hebron, he had two motives.
He was motivated by intense hatred of Palestinians, whom he perceived as trying
to take away land that rightfully belonged to Jews. He also was motivated by
the hope of derailing the fragile peace talks between the Palestine Liberation
Organization and the Israeli government. His act, thus, had a hostile component
(hatred) and an instrumental component (derailing the peace talks).
Another
distinction can be made between direct aggression and indirect aggression. (The
origin of these terms is difficult to trace, so we shall not attempt to
specifically identify who coined these terms. Suffice it to say that this is a
distinction made by a variety of aggression researchers.) Direct
aggressionrefers to overt forms of aggression such as physical aggression (hitting,
punching, kicking, etc.) and verbal aggression (name calling, denigration,
etc.). Indirect aggressionis aggression that is social in nature (social
ostracism, deliberate social exclusion).
A
form of aggression that has elements of both direct and indirect aggression is
relational aggression(Archer, 2004). This form of aggression involves using
social ostracism and rejection (indirect aggression), but can also be directly
confrontational (direct aggression). An example of the direct aspect of relational
aggression is when a child tells another child that she will stop liking her
unless the other child does what she wants (Archer, 2004).
In
some forms of aggression the target is harmed verbally through gossip,
character assassination, damage to the victim’s
property (Moyer, 1987), or interference with the victim’s advancement toward a goal. This form of aggression is called
symbolic aggression.
For example, if a person spreads rumors about a coworker in order to keep her
from being promoted, the person has used symbolic aggression. Although no
physical harm was done, the coworker was blocked from achieving a goal.
The
forms of aggression just noted can be either hostile or instrumental. The
office worker may have spread rumors because she was angry at her coworker—a
case of hostile aggression. Alternatively, she may have spread rumors to secure
the promotion for herself at her coworker’s
expense—a case of instrumental aggression.
Yet
another form of aggression is sanctioned aggression. A soldier taking aim and
killing an enemy soldier in battle engages in sanctioned aggression.
Self-defense, which occurs when a person uses aggression to protect himself or
herself or others from harm, is another example of sanctioned aggression.
Society declares that in certain situations, aggression is acceptable, even
mandatory. A soldier who refuses to engage in aggressive behavior may be
subject to disciplinary action or even have his or her military service
abruptly ended. Typically, sanctioned aggression is instrumental in nature.
Soldiers kill each other to save their own lives, to follow orders, to help win
a war. There need not be anger among enemy soldiers for them to try to kill one
another.
Gender
Differences in Aggression
One
of the most striking features of aggression is the difference in its expression
by males and females. Certainly females can be aggressive, but males show
higher levels of physical aggression (Archer, Pearson, & Westeman, 1988).
This is true among humans (Eagly & Steffen, 1986) as well as animals
(Vallortigara, 1992). A meta-analysis by John Archer (2004) on studies
investigating “real-world aggression” (i.e., self-reported aggression, peer
ratings of aggression, and observational methods) confirmed that males are more
aggressive than females, especially for direct aggression (e.g., physical
aggression). This gender difference was consistent across age and peaked
between 20 and 30 years of age. The gender difference was also consistent
across cultures. Archer also found that females used more indirect aggression
(e.g., social ostracism), but only during late childhood and adolescence and
when an observational method was used.
That
males use more direct, physical forms of aggression is clear. However, the role
of gender in the use of indirect, relational aggression is still an open
question. As noted, greater female use of indirect aggression was shown only
for a limited age range of females. Another study suggests that the difference
between males and females in the use of indirect aggression is small
(Salmivalli & Kaukiainan, 2004). In only one subgroup of females was
indirect aggression predominant: highly aggressive females. In a study using an
observational method (that is, children were observed during free-play
situations and aggression was measured), preschool-aged females showed more
indirect aggression than males (Ostrove & Keating, 2004). Males and females
did not differ on the levels of anger underlying aggression. Additionally,
males tend to favor aggression, verbal or physical, as a method of conflict
resolution (Bell & Forde, 1999; Reinisch & Sanders, 1986). They also
are more likely to be the target of physical aggression (Archer et al., 1988).
There
are further gender differences in the cognitive aspects of using aggression.
Females report more guilt over using aggression than do males and are more
concerned about the harm their aggression may inflict on others (Eagly &
Steffen, 1986). This difference is especially pronounced when physical
aggression is used.
Why
do these differences exist? Possible causes fall into two major areas:
biological factors and social factors. Biological factors include both brain
mechanisms and hormones. Most research in this area centers on the male hormone
testosterone. Higher levels of this hormone are associated with heightened
aggression in both humans and animals. There is also evidence that there is a
gender difference in brain neurochemistry related to aggression (Suarez &
Krishnan, 2006). Suarez and Krishnan found that for both males and females, the
predisposition of expressing anger verbally was related to higher levels of
“free plasma tryptophan” (TRP), which is a precursor to a serotonin-related
neurotransmitter. However, elevated levels of TRP were associated with a
greater predisposition toward hostility and an outward expression of anger
among females, but not males.
Despite
hormonal and other physiological differences between males and females,
differences in aggressive tendencies and expression may relate more closely to
gender roles than to biology (Eagly & Steffen, 1986). Both boys and girls
are encouraged to engage in gender-typed activities, and activities deemed
appropriate for boys are more aggressive than those for girls (Lytton &
Romney, 1991). For example, parents, especially fathers, encourage their sons
to play with war toys such as GI Joe figures and their daughters to play with
Barbie dolls. Socialization experiences probably further reinforce the inborn
male push toward being more aggressive.
Yet
another possible reason for the observed differences in aggression between
males and females is that females tend to be more sympathetic and empathic
(Carlo, Raffaelli, Laible, & Myer, 1999). Carlo and colleagues studied the
relationship between sympathy, parental involvement, and aggression (Carlo et
al., 1999). They found that individuals with high levels of sympathy and
empathy were less likely to be aggressive. Males scored lower on these
dimensions but higher on aggressiveness. Additionally, if an individual
perceived that his or her parents were highly involved in childrearing,
aggression was lower for both males and females. Thus, prosocial motives (on
which females tend to outscore males) and level of parental involvement are
important mediators of physical aggression.
It
is important to note that although social psychological research (both in the
laboratory and in the field) shows a consistent difference between males and
females in aggression, this difference is very small (Eagly & Steffen,
1986; Hyde, 1984). Further, gender differences in aggression appear to be
situation dependent. Males are more aggressive than females when they are
unprovoked, but males and females show equivalent levels of aggression when
provoked (Bettencourt & Miller, 1996). Males and females also respond differently
to different types of provocation. Bettencourt and Miller (1996) report a large
gender difference when different forms of provocation are used. If provocation
involves an attack on one’s intellectual ability, then males are
much more aggressive than
females. However, if provocation takes the form of a physical attack or a
negative evaluation of one’s work, males and
females respond similarly. In other words, although males and females
differ in levels of aggression, we should not conclude that gender is the
only—or even a predominant—factor in aggression. It is evident that the
relationship between gender and aggression is more complex than meets the eye.
Nevertheless,
we must also note that there are relatively large gender differences in
real-life expressions of aggression. Statistics for violent crimes show that
males are far more likely to commit violent offenses than females by a wide
margin. According to statistics compiled by the FBI, in 2004, 88.5% of
individuals arrested for murder were male. Similarly, 79.2% of arrestees for
aggravated assault were male. With respect to murder, the gap between males and
females has widened over the years. In 1976, males committed 83.4% of murders
compared to 16.6% for females, and in 1988, males committed 88% of murders
compared to 12% for females (Flanagan & Maguire, 1992). So, even though the
difference between the genders in measurable acts of aggressiveness is small,
in any specific real-world situation, this difference is magnified and
elaborated.
Explanations
for Aggression
We
turn now to the broad question, What causes aggression? As suggested here, both
biological and social factors contribute to aggressive behavior. Additionally,
research shows that frustration often leads to aggression. These factors are
considered in the next sections.
Biological
Explanations for Aggression
Biological
explanations for aggression occur on two levels, the macro and the micro. On
the macro level, aggression is considered for its evolutionary significance,
its role in the survival of the species. On the micro level, aggression is
investigated as a function of brain and hormonal activity. We consider here two
theories of aggression on the macro level—the ethological and sociobiological
approaches—and then turn to the physiology and genetics of aggression. We also
consider the effects of alcohol on aggression.
Ethology
Ethologyis
the study of the evolution and functions of animal behavior (Drickamer &
Vessey, 1986). Ethological theory views behavior in the context of survival; it
emphasizes the role of instincts and genetic forces in shaping how animals
behave (Lorenz, 1963). From an ethological perspective, aggression is seen as
behavior that evolved to help a species adapt to its environment. Aggression is
governed by innate, instinctual motivations and triggered by specific stimuli
in the environment. Aggressive behavior helps establish and maintain social
organization within a species.
For
example, many species mark and defend their territories, the space they need to
hunt or forage. If they didn’t do this, they
wouldn’t survive. Territorial defense occurs when one member of a species attacks
another for crossing territorial boundaries. The intruder is driven off by
aggressive displays or overt physical attacks—or loses his territory to the
intruder. Aggression also is used to establish dominance hierarchies within
groups of animals. Within a troop of baboons, for example, the dominant males
enjoy special status, ascending to their positions of power by exercising
physical aggression.
Although
animals use aggression against each other, few species possess the power to
kill a rival with a single blow (Lorenz, 1963). In most species, furthermore,
there are biological inhibitions against killing another member. When a
combatant makes a conciliatory gesture, such as rolling over and exposing its
neck, the aggressive impulse in the other animal is automatically checked.
Thus, aggression may involve merely exchanging a few violent actions; the fight
soon ends with no major harm done.
How
does ethological theory relate to the human animal? First of all, humans
display territorial behavior just as animals do. Konrad Lorenz, the foremost
ethologist of the century, believed that aggression had little to do with
murderous intent and a lot to do with territory (Lorenz, 1963). Ethologists,
for example, see aggressive behaviors among gang members as a matter of
protecting one’s turf, such as when members of
urban street gangs physically attack members of rival gangs who cross
territorial boundaries (Johnson, 1972).
Second,
there is evidence that aggression plays a role in the organization of dominance
hierarchies in human groups just as it does among animals. In one study,
researchers organized first- and third-grade children into play groups and
observed the development of dominance hierarchies within those groups (Pettit,
Bakshi, Dodge, & Cole, 1986). Aggression was found to play a significant
role in establishing dominance among both groups. Interestingly, however, among
the older children, another variable emerged as important in establishing
dominance: leadership skills. Leaders did not always have to use aggression to
control the group.
Finally,
ethological theory points out that humans still possess the instinct to fight.
Unlike most animals, however, humans can make the first blow the last.
Technology has given us the power to make a single-blow kill (Lorenz, 1963).
According to Lorenz (1963), human technological evolution has outpaced
biological evolution. We have diminished the importance of conciliatory cues;
bombs dropped from 30,000 feet cannot respond to a conciliatory gesture.
Sociobiology
Like
ethology, sociobiologyis the study of the biological basis of behavior.
Sociobiologists, however, focus on the evolution and function of social
behavior (Drickamer & Vessey, 1986; Reiss, 1984). Like ethological theory,
sociobiology emphasizes the biological origins and causes of behavior and views
aggression as a behavior with survival value for members of a species. For
sociobiologists, aggression, like many other behaviors, plays a natural role in
the intricate balance that keeps species alive and growing.
Sociobiologist
E. O. Wilson (1975) suggested that the principal function of aggression within
and between species is to resolve disputes over a common limited resource.
Competition can be divided into two categories: sexual competition and resource
competition. Sexual competition occurs when males compete for females at mating
time. The stronger male drives the weaker male off and then mates with the
female. As a result, the species becomes stronger. Resource competition occurs
when animals must vie for environmental resources such as food, water, and
shelter. Again, the stronger animals are able to win these competitive
situations with the use of aggression.
Aggression,
then, is one of many behaviors that are genetically programmed into a species
and passed along from generation to generation, according to sociobiologists.
Patterns of aggression (often mere displays of pseudoaggression) steer the
course of natural selection. Also programmed into a species are behaviors and
gestures of submission. An animal can choose not to fight or to withdraw from a
competitive situation. There is, thus, a natural constraint on aggression
within a species. It is kept at an “optimal level,” allowing the species to
secure food and shelter and to resolve disputes over mating partners.
Aggression, a potentially destructive behavior, actually contributes to the
biological health of a species, according to sociobiologists (Wilson, 1975).
In
both ethology and sociobiology, then, aggression is viewed as a genetically
programmed behavior with evolutionary significance. Human beings display
aggression under various circumstances because it is part of their biological
heritage. However, as noted earlier, biology plays another role in aggression.
We next consider another biological approach to aggression that focuses on
physiological forces within the individual that cause aggressive behavior.
Genetics
and Aggression
Later
in this chapter we shall discuss extensively the social learning explanation
for aggression. Briefly, this approach suggests that aggression is a behavior
that is learned during childhood primarily through the mechanism of
observational learning. The social learning approach places a great deal of
emphasis on the role of various aspects of the environment (e.g., parents,
peers, media sources) in the formation of aggressive behaviors. However, it
does not leave much room for the possibility that genetics also may influence
aggressive behavior. In this section we shall explore the role of genetics in
aggressive behavior.
The
extant research on genetic influences on aggressive behavior suggests that
there is a genetic component to aggression that operates along with the
environment. For example, a meta-analysis by Miles and Carey (1997) found that
both genetics and common environment (e.g., aspects of the social environment
shared by siblings) account for individual differences in aggressive behavior.
They also reported that genetic factors were slightly more important for males
than females and that genetic factors were less powerful among younger
subjects. In a study comparing monozygotic twins (twins that develop from a
single egg and share genetic material) and dizygotic twins (twins that develop
from two separate eggs and share less genetic material), Hines and Saudino
(2004) found that “intimate partner aggression” (physical and psychological)
has a genetic component. Hines and Saudino concluded that “familial resemblance
in psychological aggression arises because members share the genes that
influence this behavior” (p. 714). They suggest that children inherit genes
from their parents that predispose the children for aggression. Interestingly,
Hines and Saudino suggest that whether the aggressive behavior is expressed
overtly may be more strongly related to affiliation with aggressive peer groups
than parental use of partner aggression.
In
addition to the two studies just discussed, other studies also support the idea
that aggression is at least partially determined by one’s
genetic makeup (e.g., Vierikko, Pulkkinen, Kaprio, Viken, & Rose, 2003).
However, we need to be cautious when interpreting the results from these
studies for a number of reasons. First, the number of studies establishing the
genetic-aggression link is relatively small. Clearly, more research is needed
in this area. Second, the degree of contribution of genetics depends on the
methodology used. For example, observational studies tend to show stronger
links between heredity and aggression than do laboratory studies (Miles &
Carey, 1997). Finally, we must underscore that it is important to keep results
that show a genetic influence in their proper perspective. There is little
evidence that genetics has a direct effect on aggression. Instead, genetics
appears to influence characteristics (e.g., personality characteristics) that
predispose a person to aggression. Just because someone has a genetic
predisposition toward aggression does not mean that the person will behave
aggressively.
The
Physiology of Aggression
The
brain and endocrine systems of humans and animals play an intricate role in
mediating aggression. Research on the physiology of aggression has focused on
two areas: brain mechanisms and hormonal influences. The sections that follow
explore each of these.
Brain
Mechanisms
Research
on brain mechanisms has focused on the brain structures that mediate aggressive
behavior. Researchers have found, for example, that aggressive behavior is
elicited when parts of the hypothalamusare stimulated. The hypothalamus is part
of the limbic system, a group of brain structures especially concerned with
motivation and emotion. Stimulation of different parts of the hypothalamus
(called nuclei) produce different forms of aggressive behavior.
In
one study, researchers implanted electrodes in the brains of cats in various
parts of the hypothalamus (Edwards & Flynn, 1972). A small electric current
was then passed through these structures. When one part of the hypothalamus was
stimulated, the cats displayed the characteristic signs of anger and hostile
aggression: arched back, hissing and spitting, fluffed tail. This reaction was
nondiscriminating; the cats attacked anything placed in their cage, whether a sponge
or a live mouse. When another part of the hypothalamus was stimulated, the cats
displayed selective predatory aggression. They went through the motions of
hunting; with eyes wide open, they stalked and pounced on a live animal, but
they ignored the sponge.
Research
shows that other parts of the brain are also involved in aggression. There is a
neural circuit in the brain, including parts of the limbic system and the
cortex, that organizes aggressive behavior. No single brain structure is the
master controller of aggression.
Furthermore,
brain stimulation does not inevitably lead to aggression. In one study, brain
stimulation led to an aggressive response if a monkey was restrained in a chair
(Delgado, 1969). But if the monkey was placed in a cage with another docile
monkey, the same brain stimulation produced a different behavior: The monkey
ran across the cage making repeated high-pitched vocalizations. The expression
of aggressive behavior also depended on a monkey’s
status within a group. If a more dominant monkey was present, brain stimulation did
not lead to aggression. If a less dominant monkey was present, stimulating the
same part of the brain did lead to aggression. Thus, even with brain
stimulation, aggressive behavior occurred only under the “right” social
conditions.
Hormonal
Influences
Researchers
also have investigated the role of hormones in aggressive behavior. As
mentioned earlier, high levels of the male hormone testosterone are generally
associated with increased aggression (Christiansen & Knussmann, 1987).
However, the influence of testosterone on aggressive behavior—like the effect
of brain stimulation—is complex.
Hormones
come into play twice during the normal course of development in humans: first,
during prenatal development, and later, at puberty. Prenatally, testosterone
influences the sex organs and characteristics of the unborn child. Testosterone
levels are higher for a genetic male than for a genetic female. The hormone
permeates the entire body, including the brain, making it possible that the
male brain is “wired” for greater aggression. Early in life, testosterone
exposure serves an organization function, influencing the course of brain
development. Later in life, it serves an activation function (Carlson, 1991),
activating behavior patterns, such as aggression, that are related to
testosterone levels.
These
two effects were shown clearly in an experiment conducted by Conner and Levine
(1969). Conner and Levine castrated rats either neonatally (immediately after
birth) or as weanlings (about 3 weeks after birth). (In rats, the critical
period for exposure to testosterone is within a day or so after birth.
Castrating males immediately after birth effectively prevents exposure to the
necessary levels of testosterone for normal masculinization. The rats castrated
as weanlings were exposed to the early necessary levels of testosterone and
were masculinized normally.) Other rats were not castrated. Later, as adults,
the castrated rats were exposed either to testosterone or to a placebo.
The
experiment showed that for the rats castrated neonatally, the levels of
aggression displayed after exposure to testosterone as adults did not differ
significantly from the levels displayed after exposure to a placebo. For the
weanling rats, exposure to testosterone as adults increased the level of
aggression compared to that of the rats receiving the placebo. The levels of
aggression after exposure to the testosterone or placebo did not differ for
noncastrated rats.
This
study showed that early exposure to male hormones is necessary in order for
later exposure to a male hormone to increase aggression. Those rats castrated
at birth missed the “organizing function” of the male hormone; the normal
process of masculinization of the brain did not occur. Later injections of
testosterone (activation function) thus had little effect. Rats castrated as
weanlings were subjected to the organization function of the male hormone.
Their brains were normally masculinized and more receptive to the activation
function of the testosterone injections received later in life. We can conclude
that high testosterone levels are effective in elevating levels of aggression
only if there is normal exposure to male hormones early in life. Another
experiment demonstrated that hormonal influences interact with social
influences to affect aggression. In this experiment, male rats were castrated
and then implanted with a capsule (Albert, Petrovic, & Walsh, 1989a). For
some rats the capsule was empty; for others it contained testosterone. These
rats were then housed with another rat under one of two conditions. Half the
rats were housed with a single feeding tube, requiring the animals to compete
for food. The other half were housed with two feeding tubes, so no competition
was necessary. The treated rats were then tested for aggression.
The
results were striking. Testosterone increased aggression only if the rats
competed for food. If the rats were not required to compete, the levels of
aggression were quite low, about the same as those for the rats implanted with
the empty capsule.
Another
example of how situational factors can affect testosterone levels and
aggression is provided by Kleinsmith, Kasser, and McAndrew (2006), who
conducted an experiment to see if handling a gun would increase testosterone
levels and aggression. Kleinsmith et al. informed male participants that they
would be taking part in an experiment on how taste sensitivity is affected by
attention to detail. Kleinsmith et al. obtained a saliva sample as soon as
participants arrived at the lab. Testosterone levels were measured with the
saliva sample. Then participants were led into another room where they would
perform an attention task. Some participants were given a pellet gun that was a
model of a Desert Eagle automatic pistol. Other participants were given the
child’s game Mousetrap. Both groups of
participants were instructed to write a set of instructions on how to assemble
or disassemble the gun or game. Following this task another saliva sample was
obtained. Next, participants were given a cup of water that had a drop of hot
sauce in it. Participants were told that a previous participant had prepared
the sample. After drinking the water sample, participants rated the sample.
Finally, participants were told to prepare a water sample for the next
participant. They were provided with a small cup of water and a bottle of hot
sauce and told to add as much hot sauce to the water as they wished. The
results of the experiment showed that participants who handled the gun showed a
large increase in testosterone level when pre- and post-manipulation saliva
samples were analyzed (average change was 62 pg/ml). Participants who handled
the game showed a negligible increase (average change was .68 pg/ml).
Additionally, participants who handled the gun added far more hot sauce to the
water (average was 13.61 grams) than participants who handled the game (average
was 4.23 grams).
Female
aggression may also be mediated by hormones. In another study, the ovaries were
removed from some female rats but not from others (Albert, Petrovic, &
Walsh, 1989b). The rats were then housed with a sterile yet sexually active
male rat. Weekly, the male rat was removed and an unfamiliar female rat was
introduced into the cage. Female rats whose ovaries had been removed displayed
less aggression toward the unfamiliar female than those whose ovaries had not
been removed, suggesting a role of female hormones in aggression among female
rats.
Alcohol
and Aggression
Our
final topic relating physiology and aggression is to explore the relationship
between alcohol (a powerful drug affecting the nervous system) and aggression.
There is ample evidence showing a connection between alcohol consumption and
aggression (Bushman & Cooper, 1990; Quigley & Leonard, 1999). What is
it about alcohol that increases violent behavior? Is there something about the
drug effects of alcohol, or is it a function of the social situations in which
alcohol is used?
There
is no question that alcohol has pharmacological (drug-related) effects on the
body, especially on the brain. Alcohol becomes concentrated in organs with a
high water content, and the brain is one such organ. Alcohol lowers reaction
time, impairs judgment, and weakens sensory perception and motor coordination.
Under the influence of alcohol, people focus more on external cues, such as
people or events in the situation that seem to encourage them to take action,
and less on internal ones, such as thoughts about risks and consequences.
Although
alcohol is a central nervous system depressant, it initially seems to act as a
stimulant. People who are drinking at first become more sociable and assertive.
This is because alcohol depresses inhibitory brain centers (Insel & Roth,
1994). As more alcohol is consumed, however, the effects change. Drinkers often
become irritable and are easily angered. Levels of hostility and aggressiveness
increase. Considering all the effects of alcohol, it is not surprising that it
is a major factor not only in automobile crashes and fatal accidents of other
kinds (such as drownings, falls, and fires) but also in homicides, suicides,
assaults, and rapes.
Research
confirms that levels of aggression increase with the amount of alcohol consumed
(Kreutzer, Schneider, & Myatt, 1984; Pihl & Zacchia, 1986; Shuntich
& Taylor, 1972). In one study, participants who consumed 1.32 g/kg of 95%
alcohol were more aggressive than participants receiving a placebo
(nonalcoholic) drink or no drink at all (Pihl & Zacchia, 1986). The type of
beverage consumed affects aggression as well (Gustafson, 1999; Pihl, Smith,
& Farrell, 1984). As shown in Figure 10.1, participants who consumed a
distilled beverage gave more severe shocks to a target than those who consumed
wine or beer (Gustafson, 1999). Gustafson also found that longer shocks were
given after consuming a distilled beverage compared to wine and beer. In
another study, participants in a bar were approached and asked a series of
annoying questions. In this natural setting, bar patrons drinking distilled
beverages displayed more verbal aggression toward the interviewer than those
drinking beer (Murdoch & Pihl, 1988).
How
does alcohol increase aggression? Most likely, alcohol has an indirect effect
on aggression by reducing a person’s
ability to inhibit behaviors that are normally suppressed by fear, such as
aggression (Pihl, Peterson, & Lau, 1993). Although the precise brain
mechanisms that are involved in this process are not fully known, there is
evidence that alcohol is associated with a significant drop in the amount of
brain serotonin (a neurotransmitter), which makes individuals more likely to
engage in aggression in response to external stimuli (Badaway, 1998; Pihl &
Lemarquand, 1998). Serotonin, when it is operating normally, inhibits
antisocial behaviors such as aggression through the arousal of anxiety under
threatening conditions (Pihl & Peterson, 1993). When serotonin levels are
reduced, anxiety no longer has its inhibitory effects, but intense emotional
arousal remains, resulting in increased aggression under conditions of threat
(Pihl & Peterson, 1993).
Figure
10.1Mean shock severity as a function of type of alcoholic beverage consumed.
Based
on data from Gustafson (1999)
Alcohol
has also been found to influence the functioning of the prefrontal cortex of
the brain, disrupting executive cognitive functioning (ECF), or functions that
help one use higher cognitive processes such as attention, planning, and
self-monitoring (Hoaken, Giancola, & Pihl, 1998; Pihl, Assad, & Hoaken,
2003). These executive functions play a major role in one’s ability to effectively regulate goal-directed behavior (Hoaken et al.,
1998). In individuals with low-functioning ECF, aggression is more likely than
among individuals with high-functioning ECF, regardless of alcohol consumption
(Hoaken et al., 1998). If the ECF remains active after alcohol consumption,
alcohol-related aggression is lower than if the ECF is inhibited (Giancola,
2004). It is apparent, then, that the inhibitory effect of alcohol on ECF is
one factor contributing to increased aggression after alcohol consumption.
When
in an intoxicated state, one can override the effects of alcohol if properly
motivated (Hoaken, Assaad, & Pihl, 1998). Hoaken and his associates (1998)
placed intoxicated and sober individuals into a situation where they could
deliver electric shocks to another person. Half the participants in each group
received an incentive to deliver low levels of shocks (the promise of money).
The results showed that intoxicated participants were just as able as their sober
counterparts to reduce the severity of shocks delivered when the incentive was
provided. However, when no incentive was provided, intoxicated participants
delivered higher shock levels than the sober participants.
Although
the amount and type of alcohol consumed affect aggression, research shows that
one’s expectations about the effects of alcohol
also have an impact on aggression (Lang, Goeckner, Adesso, & Marlatt, 1975;
Leonard, Collins, & Quigley, 2003; Kreutzer, Schneider, & Myatt, 1984;
Rohsenow & Bachorowski, 1984). Generally, participants in experiments who
believe they are drinking alcohol display elevated levels of aggression, even
if in reality they are drinking a nonalcoholic placebo. The mere belief that
one has consumed alcohol is enough to enhance aggression. In fact, even the
experimenter’s knowledge of who has consumed alcohol can
affect the level of aggression observed in experiments like this. An analysis
of the literature shows that the effects of alcohol on aggression are smaller
when the experimenter is blind to the conditions of the experiment (Bushman
& Cooper, 1990).This relationship also holds outside the laboratory.
Leonard, Collins, and Quigley (2003) conducted a study in which male
participants were asked about aggressive events that happened to them in bars.
Leonard et al. measured several personality and situational variables. They
found that a belief that alcohol was the cause for aggression was related to
the occurrence (but not severity) of an aggressive encounter in a bar.
Expectations
cannot account for the entire effect of alcohol, however. In some cases even
when there is an expectation that alcohol may lead to aggression, such an
expectation does not increase aggression, whereas actual alcohol consumption
does (Quigley & Leonard, 1999). Social cues, expectations, and attitudes
play some part in mediating alcohol-induced aggression. However, the
pharmacological effects of alcohol on the body and brain are real. Probably
through a combination of reducing inhibitions and increasing irritability and
hostility on the one hand, and giving the drinker “permission” to act out in
social situations on the other, alcohol has the net effect of enhancing
aggressive behavior.
Finally,
the alcohol-aggression link is mediated by individual characteristics and the
social situation. Individuals, especially men, who are high on a characteristic
known as dispositional empathy(an emotion associated with helping behavior) are
less likely to behave aggressively after alcohol consumption than those low on
this characteristic (Giancola, 2003). Cheong and Nagoshi (1999) had
participants engage in a competitive game with a bogus participant. The game
was played under one of three conditions. In one condition, the real
participant was told that his opponent could deliver a loud noise in an attempt
to disrupt his performance (aggression). In the second condition, the real
participant was told that his opponent would use the loud noise to keep the
real participant alert during the boring task (altruism). In the third
condition, the real participant was given ambiguous information about his
opponent’s motives (maybe aggression or maybe altruism).
Furthermore, before engaging in the task, participants consumed either
alcoholic drinks or a placebo. One-half of the placebo participants were told
they were consuming an alcoholic beverage (expectancy for alcohol) and the
other half were told their drinks were placebos. Finally, participants
completed a personality measure of their impulsiveness and sensation-seeking
tendencies.
The
results of this experiment showed that alcohol-mediated aggression depended on
the nature of the situation (aggression vs. altruism), personality, and alcohol
consumption. Specifically, participants who scored highly on the measure of
impulsiveness/sensationseeking were the most aggressive after consuming
alcohol, but only when they believed their opponent was using the loud noise
aggressively. When the opponent’s motive was either altruistic
or ambiguous, this effect did not occur. Thus, whether an individual behaves
aggressively after consuming alcohol depends on the nature of the situation and
one’s predisposition toward impulsive
behavior or sensation-seeking.
Physiology
and Aggression: Summing Up
What
can we learn from this research on the physiological aspects of aggression in
animals? How much of it can be applied to human beings? Not many people would
attribute John Muhammad and Lee Malvo’s
murderous behavior to an overabundance of testosterone or abnormal brain
circuitry. Research with animals supports the general conclusion that
aggression does have a physiological component. However, in humans, biological
forces cannot account for all, or even most, instances in which aggression is
displayed (Huesmann & Eron, 1984). The human being is a profoundly cultural
animal. Although aggression is a basic human drive, the expression of that
drive depends on forces operating in a particular society at a particular time.
Muhammad and Malvo’s behavior was the product not only of their
biology but also of their social world, which included playing violent video
games and hanging around with a group that supported violence. Laws and social
and cultural norms serve as powerful factors that can inhibit or facilitate
aggressive behavior.
The
Frustration-Aggression Link
Imagine
for a moment that you are standing in front of a snack machine, You dig into
your pocket and come up with your last 75 cents. You breathe a sigh of relief.
You are very hungry and have just enough money to get a bag of chips. You put
your money into the machine and press the button. You watch and wait for the
mechanism to operate and drop your bag of chips. Instead, the mechanism grinds
away and your bag of chips gets hung up in the machine. You mutter a few choice
words, kick the machine, and walk away in a huff.
Analysis
of this incident gives us some insight into a factor that social psychologists
believe instigates aggression. In the example, a goal you wished to
obtain—satisfying your hunger—was blocked. This produced an emotional state
that led to aggression (kicking the vending machine). Your reaction to such a
situation illustrates the general principles of a classic formulation known as
the frustration-aggression hypothesis (Dollard, Doob, Miller, Mowrer, &
Sears, 1939).
In
its original form, the frustration-aggression hypothesis stated that
“aggression is always a consequence of frustration, the occurrence of
aggressive behavior always presupposes the existence of frustration and,
contrariwise . . . the existence of frustration leads to some form of
aggression” (Dollard et al., 1939, p. 1). In other words, according to the
frustration-aggression hypothesis, when we are frustrated, we behave
aggressively.
Components
of the Frustration-Aggression Sequence
What
are the components of the frustration-aggression sequence? An assumption of the
frustration-aggression hypothesis is that emotional arousal occurs when
goal-directed behavior is blocked. Frustration occurs, then, when two
conditions are met. First, we expect to perform certain behaviors, and second,
those behaviors are blocked (Dollard et al., 1939).
Frustration
can vary in strength, depending on three factors (Dollard et al., 1939). The
first is the strength of the original drive. If you are very hungry, for
example, and are deprived of a snack, your frustration will be greater than if
you are only slightly hungry. The second factor is the degree to which the
goal-directed behavior is thwarted. If your kicking of the machine dislodged a
smaller snack, for example, you would be less frustrated than if you received
no snack at all. The third factor is the number of frustrated responses. If
your thwarted attempt to get a snack came on the heels of another frustrating
event, your frustration would be greater.
Once
we are frustrated, what do we choose as a target? Our first choice is the
source of our frustration (Dollard et al., 1939)—the vending machine, in our
example. But sometimes aggression against the source of frustration is not
possible. The source may be a person in a position of power over us, such as
our boss. When direct aggression against the source of aggression is blocked,
we may choose to vent our frustration against another safer target —a son,
perhaps. If we have a bad day at work or school, we may take it out on an
innocent roommate or family member when we get home. This process is called
displaced aggression(Dollard et al., 1939). Displaced aggression is influenced
by the following factors (Marcus-Newhall, Pederson, Carlson, & Miller,
2000):
1. Intensity of the original provocation. The
higher the intensity, the less the displacement.
2. Similarity between the original and displaced
target. The higher the similarity, the greater the displacement.
3. The negativity of the interaction between the
individual and original target. The more negative the interaction, the greater
the displacement.
Although
the original frustration-aggression hypothesis stated categorically that
frustration always leads to aggression, acts of frustration-based aggression
can be inhibited (Dollard et al., 1939). If there is a strong possibility that
your aggressive behavior will be punished, you may not react aggressively to
frustration. If a campus security guard were standing beside the vending
machine, for example, you probably wouldn’t kick it for fear of
being arrested.
Factors
Mediating the Frustration-Aggression Link
The
frustration-aggression hypothesis stirred controversy from the moment it was
proposed. Some theorists questioned whether frustration inevitably led to
aggression (Miller, 1941). Others suggested that frustration leads to
aggression only under specific circumstances, such as when the blocked response
is important to the individual (Blanchard & Blanchard, 1984).
As
criticisms of the original theory mounted, modifications were made. For
example, Berkowitz (1989) proposed that frustration is connected to aggression
by negative affect, such as anger. If, as shown in Figure 10.2, the frustration
of goal-directed behavior leads to anger, then aggression will occur. If no
anger is aroused, no aggression will result. If anger mediates frustration, we
must specify which frustrating conditions lead to anger. Theoretically, if the
blocking of goal-directed behavior does not arouse anger, then the frustrated
individual should not behave aggressively. Let’s
consider other factors that mediate the frustration-aggression link.
Figure
10.2The relationship among frustration, anger, and aggression. Frustration
leads to aggression only if it arouses negative affect, such as anger.
Attributions
about Intent
Recall
from Chapter 4 that we are always interpreting people’s behavior, deciding that they did something because they meant it
(an internal attribution) or because of some outside situational factor (an
external attribution). The type of attribution made about a source of
frustration is one important factor contributing to aggression. If someone’s behavior
frustrates us and we make an internal attribution, we are more likely to
respond with aggression than if we make an external attribution.
Research
shows that the intent behind an aggressive act is more important in determining
the degree of retaliation than the actual harm done (Ohbuchi & Kambara,
1985). Individuals who infer negative intent on the part of another person are
most likely to retaliate. The actual harm done is no t so important as the
intent behind the aggressor’s act (Ohbuchi &
Kambara, 1985).
There
is additional evidence about the importance of attributions for aggression.
Research shows that if we are provided with a reasonable explanation for the
behavior of someone who is frustrating us, we will react less aggressively than
if no explanation is given (Johnson & Rule, 1986; Kremmer & Stephens,
1983). Moreover, if we believe that aggression directed against us is typical
for the situation in which it occurs, we are likely to attribute our attacker’s actions to external factors. Thus, we will retaliate less than if we
believe the attacker was choosing atypical levels of aggression (Dyck &
Rule, 1978). In this case, we would be more likely to attribute the attacker’s aggression
to internal forces and to retaliate in kind if given the opportunity.
Perceived
Injustice and Inequity
Another
factor that can contribute to anger and ultimately to aggression is the
perception that we have been treated unjustly. The following account of a
violent sports incident illustrates the power of perceived injustice to incite
aggression (Mark, Bryant, & Lehman, 1983, pp. 83–84):
In
November 1963, a riot occurred at Roosevelt Raceway, a harness racing track in
the New York metropolitan area. Several hundred fans swarmed onto the track.
The crowd attacked the judges’booth, smashed the
tote board, set fires in program booths, broke windows, and damaged cars parked in
an adjacent lot. Several hundred police officers were called to the scene.
Fifteen fans were arrested, 15 others hospitalized.
What
incited this riot? The sixth race was the first half of a daily double, in
which bettors attempt to select the winners of successive races, with potentially
high payoffs. During the sixth race, six of the eight horses were involved in
an accident and did not finish the race. In accordance with New York racing
rules, the race was declared official. All wagers placed on the six
nonfinishing horses were lost, including the daily double bets. Many fans
apparently felt that they were unjustly treated, that the race should have been
declared no contest.
This
incident is not unique. Frequently, we read about fans at a soccer match who
riot over a “bad call” or fans at a football game who pelt officials with
snowballs or beer cans following a call against a home team. In each case, the
fans are reacting to what they perceive to be an injustice done to the home
team.
Aggression
is often seen as a way of restoring justice and equity in a situation. The
perceived inequity in a frustrating situation, as opposed to the frustration
itself, leads to aggression (Sulthana, 1987). For example, a survey of female
prison inmates who had committed aggravated assault or murder suggested that an
important psychological cause for their aggression was a sense of having been
treated unjustly (Diaz, 1975). This perception, apparently rooted in an inmate’s childhood, persisted into adulthood and resulted in
aggressive acts.
Of
course, not all perceived injustice leads to aggression. Not everyone rioted at
the New York race track, and most sports fans do not assault referees for bad
calls. There may be more of a tendency to use aggression to restore equity when
the recipient of the inequity feels particularly powerless (Richardson,
Vandenbert, & Humphries, 1986). In one study, participants with lower
status than their opponents chose higher shock levels than did participants
with equal or higher status than their opponents (Richardson et al., 1986). We
can begin to understand from these findings why groups who believe themselves
to be unjustly treated, who have low status and feel powerless, resort to
aggressive tactics, especially when frustrated, to remedy their situation.
Riots and terrorism are often the weapons of choice among those with little
power.
The
Heat Effect
For
centuries it has been the belief that aggression is more likely to occur when
it is hot than when it is cool. The heat effect refers to the observation that
aggression is more likely when people are hot than when they are cool
(Anderson, 1989, 2001). For example, as shown in Table 10.1, most major riots
in the United States have occurred during months when the weather is hot.
Incidents of homicides, assaults, rapes, and family disturbances all peak
during summer months, especially during the month of July (Anderson, 1989).
Anderson (2001) has reviewed the research (field and laboratory) and has
concluded that the heat effect is real and is most likely due to the fact that
when it is hot, people get more cranky (Berkowitz, 1993). According to
Berkowitz, heat distorts assessments of social interactions so that what might
ordinarily be passed off as a minor incident gets blown out of proportion and
becomes a cause for aggression.
Table
10.1 Riots in the United States and Heat
Anderson
and his colleagues (2000) have proposed the General Affective Aggression Model
(GAAM)that draws on this idea to account for the effects of heat on aggression.
As shown in Figure 10.3, heat-induced negative affect (crankiness) primes
aggressive thoughts and perceptions, which then cause the escalation of a minor
incident.
The
Social Learning Explanation for Aggression
The
frustration-aggression hypothesis focuses on the responses of individuals in
particular, frustrating situations. But clearly, not all people respond in the
same ways to frustrating stimuli. Some respond with aggression, whereas others
respond with renewed determination to overcome their frustration. It appears
that some people are more predisposed to aggression than others. How can we
account for these differences?
Although
there are genetically based, biological differences in aggressiveness among
individuals, social psychologists are more interested in the role of socialization
in the development of aggressive behavior (Huesmann, 1988; Huesmann &
Malamuth, 1986). Socialization, as mentioned earlier, is the process by which
children learn the behaviors, attitudes, and values of their culture.
Socialization is the work of many agents, including parents, siblings, schools,
churches, and the media. Through the socialization process, children learn many
of the behavior patterns, both good and bad, that will stay with them into
adulthood.
Figure
10.3Figure 10.3. GAAM model explanation for heat effect
Aggression
is one behavior that is developed early in life via socialization and persists
into adulthood (Huesmann, Eron, Lefkowitz, & Walder, 1984). In fact, a
longterm study of aggressive behavior found that children who were rated by
their peers as aggressive at age 8 were likely to be aggressive as adults, as
measured by selfratings, ratings by participants’spouses, and citations
for criminal and traffic offenses (Huesmann et al., 1984).
The
stability of aggression over time applies to both males and females (Pulkkinen
& Pitkanen, 1993). However, the age at which early aggressiveness predicts
later aggressive behavior differs for males and females. In one study,
researchers investigated the relationship between Swedish children’s aggressiveness (measured by teacher ratings) at two ages (10 and
13) and crime rates through age 26 (Stattin & Magnusson, 1989). For males,
aggressiveness ratings at both age levels were significant predictors of
serious crimes committed later in life. However, for females, only
aggressiveness ratings at age 13 predicted later criminal behavior. For males
and females, early aggressiveness was most closely related to crimes of the
“acting out” type, such as violent crimes against property and other people, rather
than drug offenses, traffic offenses, or crimes committed for personal gain
(Stattin & Magnusson, 1989).
Taken
together, these studies show a clear pattern of early aggression being
significantly related to aggression later in life (as measured by crime
statistics). Although there is some difference between males and females (at
least in terms of the age at which the relationship between early aggression
and later aggression begins), it is clear that the relationship between
childhood aggression and adulthood aggression is true for both males and
females.
What
happens during these early years to increase aggression among some children? In
the sections that follow, we look at how socialization relates to the
development of aggressive behavior patterns.
The
Socialization of Aggression
Unlike
the biological approaches to aggression, Albert Bandura’s (1973) social learning theorymaintains that aggression is
learned, much like any other human behavior. Aggression can be learned through
two general processes: direct reinforcement and punishment, and observational
learningor learning by watching others. Often, individuals who commit violent
acts grew up in a neighborhood where violence was commonplace. These
individuals saw that aggression was a method of getting one’s way. They probably even tried it for themselves and obtained
some goal. If aggression pays off, one is then more likely to use aggressive
behavior again, learning through the process of direct reinforcement. If the
aggression fails, or one is punished for using aggression, aggression is less
likely to be used in the future.
Although
the processes of direct reinforcement and punishment are important, social
learning theory maintains that its primary channel is through observational
learning, or modeling. This occurs when, for example, a young man standing in a
playground sees a person get money by beating up another person. People quickly
learn that aggression can be effective. By watching others, they learn new
behaviors, or they have existing behaviors encouraged or inhibited.
Bandura
and his colleagues (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963) provided powerful evidence
in support of the transmission of aggression through observational learning.
They showed that children who watch an aggressive model can learn new patterns
of behavior and will display them when given the opportunity to do so. Bandura
and his colleagues designed an ingenious experiment to test this central
principle of social learning theory.
In
this experiment, children were exposed to a model who behaved aggressively
against a “Bobo doll,” a large, inflatable, plastic punching doll. The model
engaged in some specific behavior, such as kicking and punching the doll while
screaming, “Sock him in the nose” (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1961). After the
child observed the model engage in this behavior, he or she was taken to a room
with several toys. After a few minutes, the experimenter went in and told the
child that he or she could not play with the toys because they were being saved
for another child (this was to frustrate the child). The child was then taken
to another room with several other toys, including the Bobo doll.
Bandura
performed a number of variations on this basic situation. In one experiment,
for example, the children saw the model being rewarded, being punished, or
receiving no consequences for batting around the Bobo doll (Bandura, 1965). In
another, children observed a live model, a filmed model, or a cartoon model
(Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963). In all the variations, the dependent variable
was the same—the number of times the child imitated the aggressive behaviors
the model displayed.
Bandura
found that when the children saw aggression being rewarded, they showed more
imitative responses than when it was punished. Live models evoked the most
imitative responses, followed by film models and then cartoon models, but any
aggressive model increased imitative responses over the nonaggressive or
no-model conditions. Exposure to the aggressive model elicited other aggressive
responses that the child had not seen from the model (Bandura et al., 1963).
Apparently, an aggressive model can motivate a child to behave aggressively in
new, unmodeled ways.
Bandura
(1973) concluded that observational learning can have the following effects.
First, a child can learn totally new patterns of behavior. Second, a child’s behavior can be inhibited (if the model is punished) or
disinhibited (if the model is rewarded). Disinhibition in this context means
that a child already knows how to perform a socially unacceptable behavior
(such as hitting or kicking) but is not doing it for a reason. Seeing a model
rewarded removes inhibitions against performing the behavior. Bandura calls
this process vicarious reinforcement. And third, a socially desirable behavior
can be enhanced by observing models engaged in prosocial activities.
Bandura’s findings have been observed across cultures. McHan (1985)
replicated Bandura’s basic experiment in Lebanon. Children were exposed either to
a film showing a child playing aggressively with a bobo doll or to a film
showing a boy playing nonaggressively with some toys. McHan found that the
children who were exposed to the aggressive film were more aggressive in a
subsequent play situation. They also exhibited more novel aggressive behaviors
than children who had seen the nonaggressive film. These results exactly
replicate Bandura’s original findings and offer additional
support for
the social learning approach to aggression.
We
have established that exposing children to filmed aggressive models contributes
to increased physical aggression. Is there any evidence that exposure to
violence in naturalistic settings relates to levels of aggression? According to
a study by
Gorman-Smith
and Tolan (1998), the answer to this question is yes. Gorman-Smith and Tolan
investigated the relationship between exposure to community violence and
aggression in a sample of minority males growing up in high-crime
neighborhoods. Their results showed that exposure to violence in the community
was related to an increase in aggression and feelings of depression. They also
reported that the increase in aggression is specific to exposure to violence in
the neighborhood and not to general levels of stress. Finally, Gorman-Smith and
Tolan reported that the number of people who are exposed to community violence
does not relate significantly to parental discipline practices but may relate
more strongly to peer influences and other communityrelated factors.
Aggressive
Scripts: Why and How They Develop
One
mechanism believed to underlie the relationship between observation and
aggression is the formation of aggressive scripts during the socialization
process. Scripts are internalized representations of how an event should occur.
Another term for a script is event schema. You may, for example, have a script
about what goes on at a college basketball game: You go to the arena, sit in
your seat, and cheer for your team. Such scripts influence how people behave in
a given social situationExposing a child to aggressive models—parents, peers,
television characters, video games—during socialization contributes to the
development of aggressive scripts (Huesmann, 1986; Huesmann & Malamuth,
1986). These scripts, in turn, lead to increased aggression and a tendency to
interpret social interactions aggressively. And they can persist, greatly
influencing levels of aggression in adulthood.
Aggressive
scripts develop through three phases (Huesmann & Malamuth, 1986). During
the acquisition and encoding phase, the script is first learned and placed into
the child’s memory. Much like a camcorder, a child
who sees violence—or is reinforced directly for violence—records the violent
scenes into memory. A script will be most easily encoded into memory if the
child believes the script-related behavior is socially acceptable (Huesmann,
1988). When one grows up in a violent neighborhood, for example, one will
undoubtedly acquire and encode an aggressive script based on his or her
experiences.
The
stored script is strengthened and elaborated on during the maintenance phase.
Strengthening and elaboration occur each time a child thinks about an
aggressive event, watches an aggressive television show, plays aggressively, or
is exposed to violence from other sources (Huesmann, 1988; Huesmann &
Malamuth, 1986). Research shows, for example, that children who are exposed to
high levels of violence in their communities tend to develop aggressive
behaviors (Gorman-Smith & Tolan, 1998).
Initially,
during the retrieval and emission phase, the internalized script guides the
child’s behavior whenever a situation similar
to the one in the script occurs. If the child has watched too many Clint Eastwood
movies, for example, competition with another child for a toy may lead to a
“make my day” scenario. The script may suggest to young Clint that competition
is best resolved using aggression. Often aggressive behavior certainly fits
with this model. Those who are exposed to violence on a day-to-day basis and
feel threatened may turn to violence as a way to resolve conflicts. Aggressive
scripts are played out to their bloody conclusions.
The
Role of the Family in Developing Aggressive Behaviors
Although
children are exposed to many models, the family provides the most immediate
environment and is the most influential agent of socialization. It makes sense,
then, that aggressive behavior is closely linked with family dynamics.
One
developmental model proposed to explain the evolution of aggressive behavior is
the social-interactional model (Patterson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989). According
to this model, antisocial behavior (such as aggression) arises early in life as
a result of poor parenting, such as harsh, inconsistent discipline and poor
monitoring of children. Poor parenting leads to a child’s behavior problems, which in turn contribute to rejection by
peers and academic problems in school. Such children often become associated
with deviant peer groups in late childhood and adolescence. In many cases,
delinquency results.
Aggressive
Parenting
Key
to the social-interactional model is the disciplinary style adopted by parents
and the parent-child interaction style that results. Some parents have an
antisocial parenting style, according to the model. Several factors contribute
to such parental behavior. As shown in Figure 10.4, these factors include
antisocial behavior and poor family management by their own parents, family
demographics, and family stressors. Parents’antisocial behavior
contributes to disruptions in their family management practices and,
ultimately, to antisocial behavior from the child.
Figure
10.4The social-interaction model of antisocial behavior. According to this
model, antisocial parenting gives rise to disrupted family management and an
increase in a child’s antisocial behavior. Antisocial parenting relates to
three factors: family demographics, grandparental traits, and family stressors.
From
Patterson, DeBaryshe, and Ramsey (1989).
Parents
who fall into a harmful cycle of parenting generally rely heavily on the use of
power or harsh measures designed to control the child’s behavior. They also use physical and/or verbal punishment. Do these
techniques encourage children to act aggres sively themselves? The answer is a
firm yes! Although parents use power assertion and punishment with their
children to make them comply, research shows that it actually reduces children’s compliance (Crockenberg & Litman, 1986). This
noncompliance may, in turn, cause parents to adopt an even more coercive
disciplinary style.
Straus
conducted a series of correlational studies (summarized in Straus, 1991) on the
relationship between the use of physical punishment and aggressive behavior.
Straus obtained information from adolescents and adults about the frequency
with which they experienced physical punishment while they were children.
Straus reported, first, that almost 90% of U.S. parents of children aged 3 to 4
used some form of physical punishment. The rate of physical punishment declined
slowly after age 4 but remained at a relatively high level—60% or above—until
the child was 13 years old. Thus, physical punishment as a parenting technique
is widespread in our society.
Straus
also found that as the frequency of physical punishment used during
socialization increased, so did the rate of physical aggression used outside
the family later on in adulthood. More ominously, as the frequency of physical
punishment increased, so did homicide rates. The negative effects of punishment
apply to other cultures as well. One study conducted in Singapore found that
parental use of physical punishment (caning or slapping) was related to higher
levels of aggression among preschool-aged children (Sim & Ong, 2005). Other
results from this study showed that caning by fathers increased aggression
among both male and female children. However, there was a crosssex relationship
for fathers and mothers who slapped their children. Father slapping had the
greatest effect on female children, whereas mother slapping had the greatest
effect on male children. Finally, physical punishment is significantly associated
with a variety of negative outcomes, including aggressive behavior, lower
levels of moral internalization of behavior, degraded parent-child
relationships, and poorer mental health (Gershoff, 2002). The only positive
behavior associated with physical punishment is immediate compliance on the
part of the child (Gershoff, 2002).
Physical
punishment is not the only form of parental behavior associated with heightened
aggression. Parents also subject their children to verbal and symbolic
aggression, which can include these behaviors (Vissing, Straus, Gelles, &
Harrop, 1991, p. 228):
•
Insulting or swearing at the child.
•
Sulking or refusing to talk about a problem.
•
Stomping out of the room or house.
•
Doing or saying something to spite the child.
•
Threatening to throw something at or hit the child.
•
Throwing, smashing, hitting, or kicking something.
Like
physical aggression, verbal or symbolic aggression is commonly directed at
children and can contribute to “problems with aggression, delinquency, and interpersonal
relationships” on the part of the children (Vissing et al., 1991, p. 231). This
relationship holds even when the effects of other variables—such as physical
aggression, age and gender of the child, socioeconomic status, and psychosocial
problems of the child—are held constant. Moreover, parents’use of verbal or symbolic aggression as part of their parenting style is
more highly associated with aggression in children than is physical aggression.
One possible explanation for the pernicious effects of verbal aggression on
children is that name calling and similar parental behaviors have implications
for the child’s self-esteem, with children experiencing
verbal aggression showing lower levels of self-esteem (Ruth & Francoise,
1999).
Supporting
evidence comes from a 22-year study of the relationship between the parental
behaviors of rejection, punishment, and low identification with their children
and aggression in children (Eron, Huesmann, & Zelli, 1991). This study
suggests that parental rejection and punitiveness are significantly correlated
with aggression in childhood and later in adulthood. Children whose parents
rejected them at age 8, for example, showed a greater tendency toward
aggression as adults than nonrejected children, and harsh parental punishment,
particularly for girls, led to increased aggression. Generally, parental
rejection and punitiveness were found to have their most enduring relationship
with aggression if the rejection and punitiveness began before age 6. Similar
effects were reported with a sample of Dutch adolescents (Hale, Van Der Valk,
Engels, & Meeus, 2005). Hale et al. also found that parental rejection
operates through depression to produce aggression. That is, parental rejection
contributes to adolescent depression, which relates to elevated levels of
aggression. The picture, however, is quite complex. For example, rejected
children tend to behave in ways that lead parents to reject them (Eron et al.,
1991). So, parental rejection that is related to aggression later in life may
be partly caused by the child’s behavior—a vicious cycle.
Exposure
to high levels of family aggression also relates to aggression used in a wide
variety of relationships (Chermack & Walton, 1999; Murphy & Blumenthal,
2000). For example, Chermack and Walton (1999) studied the relationship between
family aggression (parent-to-parent aggression, parent-to-child aggression) and
the use of aggression in several types of relationships (dating, marital,
etc.). They found that if participants saw their parents behaving aggressively
toward each other and were the recipients of parental aggression themselves,
the participants were more likely to use aggression in their own dating
relationships. Interestingly, general aggression related positively only to being
the actual target of parental aggression. Additionally, seeing one’s parents behave aggressively also contributes to heightened
feelings of psychological stress among both men and women (Julian, McKenry,
Gavazzi, & Law, 1999). However, the psychological stress was most likely to
be transformed into verbal or physical aggression among men as opposed to women
(Julian et al., 1999). Thus, exposure to aggression in the family appears to
influence adult aggression through the arousal of negative psychological
symptoms. In any event, the evidence is clear: Exposure to family violence as a
child contributes significantly to aggression later in life.
Role
Modeling of Aggressive Behavior
What
is the link between parental aggression and child aggression? The most likely
explanation is role modeling. Whenever parents use physical or verbal
aggression, they are modeling that behavior for their children. This is a
special case of observational learning. Children observe their parents behaving
aggressively; they also see that the aggressive behavior works, because
ultimately the children are controlled by it. Because the behavior is
reinforced, both parents and children are more likely to use aggression again.
The message sent to the child is loud and clear: You can get your way by using
physical or verbal aggression. Through these processes of learning, children
develop aggressive scripts (Eron et al., 1991), which organize and direct their
aggressive behavior in childhood and in adulthood.
Child
Abuse and Neglect
Parental
discipline style is not the only family-related factor related to increases in
aggression. Child abuse has also been linked to aggressive behavior later in
life, especially among children who also have intrinsic vulnerabilities, such
as cognitive, psychiatric, and neurological impairments (Lewis, Lovely, Yeager,
& Della Femina, 1989). Research shows that being abused or witnessing abuse
is strongly related to highly violent behavior patterns. But physical abuse is
not the only kind of abuse that contributes to increased aggressive behavior.
Abused and neglected children are more likely to be arrested for juvenile (26%)
and adult (28.6%) violent criminal behavior compared to a nonabused,
nonneglected control group (16.8% and 21.1% arrest rates for juvenile and adult
violent crime, respectively; Widom, 1992). Children who were only neglected had
a higher arrest rate for violent crime (12.5%) than nonneglected children had
(7.9%).
Being
the victim of child abuse has another pernicious effect. Exposure to abusive
situations desensitizes one to the suffering of others. In one study (Main
& George, 1985), for example, abused and nonabused children were exposed to
a peer showing distress. Nonabused children showed concern and empathy for the
distressed peer. Abused children showed a very different pattern. These
children did not respond with concern or empathy but rather with anger,
including physical aggression. Thus, child abuse and neglect are major
contributors not only to aggressive behavior later in life but also to an
attitude of less caring for another person’s
suffering.
Family
Disruption
Yet
another family factor that contributes to aggressive behavior patterns is
family disruption—for example, disruption caused by an acrimonious divorce.
Research shows that disruption of the family is significantly related to higher
rates of crime (Mednick, Baker, & Carothers, 1990; Sampson, 1987). One
study investigated the relationship between several family variables, such as
family income, male employment, and family disruption (defined as a
female-headed household with children under age 18), and homicide and robbery
rates among blacks and whites (Sampson, 1987). The study found that the single
best predictor of African American homicide was family disruption. A similar
pattern emerged for robbery committed by blacks and whites. Family disruption,
which was strongly related to living under economically deprived conditions,
was found to have its greatest effect on juvenile crime, as opposed to adult
crime. It was found that, at least for robbery, the effects of family
disruption cut across racial boundaries. Family disruption was equally harmful
to blacks and whites.
Another
study looked at family disruption from a different perspective: the impact of
divorce on children’s criminal behavior (Mednick et al.,
1990). The study examined Danish families that had divorced but were stable
after the divorce (the divorce solved interpersonal problems between the parents);
divorced but unstable after the divorce (the divorce failed to resolve
interpersonal problems between the parents); and not divorced. The study showed
the highest crime rates among adolescents and young adults who came from a
disruptive family situation. The crime rate for those whose families divorced
but still had significant conflict was substantially higher (65%) than for
those whose families divorced but were stable afterward (42%) or for families
that did not divorce (28%).
Clearly,
an important contributor to aggression is the climate and structure of the family
in which a child grows up. Inept parenting, in the form of overreliance on
physical or verbal punishment, increases aggression. Child abuse and neglect,
as well as family disruption, also play a role in the development of aggressive
behavior patterns. Children learn their aggressive behavior patterns early as a
result of being in a family environment that supports aggression. And, as we
have seen, these early aggressive behavior patterns are likely to continue into
adolescence and adulthood.
The
Role of Culture in Violent Behavior
In
addition to the influence of the immediate family on the socialization of
aggression, social psychologists have also investigated the role that culture
plays. Cross-cultural research (Bergeron & Schneider, 2005) suggests that
aggression is less likely to be seen in cultures that show the following
characteristics:
1.
Collectivist values
2. High levels of moral discipline
3. Egalitarian values
4. Low levels of avoiding uncertainty
5. Confucian values
There
are also cultural differences with respect to the expression of verbal
aggression through the use of different invectives (De Raad, Van Oudenhoven,
& Hofstede, 2005). De Raad et al. found that invectives referring to social
relationships (e.g., “son of a whore,” “good for nothing”) were most common
among Spanish participants. Participants from the Netherlands seem to prefer
invectives relating to the genital region (e.g., “prick,” “scrotum cleaner”),
and participants from Germany prefer invectives targeting the anal region
(e.g., “asshole”) and social inadequacy (e.g., “spastic”). Participants from
all three countries used references to abnormality to insult others. Another
cultural difference can be seen among different segments of culture in the
United
States. Nisbett and his colleagues have been studying this issue by comparing
southern and northern regions of the United States. In a series of studies that
include examining homicide statistics (Nisbett, 1993), field experiments
(Nisbett, Polly, & Lang, 1995), and laboratory experiments (Cohen, 1998), a
clear trend toward greater violence among southern than northern Americans
emerges.
To
what can we attribute the regional differences in violence? Nisbett (1993)
suggested that there are a variety of explanations for regional differences.
These include traditional explanations suggesting that the South has more
poverty, higher temperatures, and a history of slavery as well as the
possibility that whites have imitated aggressive behavior seen among the black
population. Nisbett suggested that there is another more plausible explanation
for the regional differences observed. He hypothesized that in the South (and
to some extent in the frontier West) a culture of honorhas evolved in which
violence is both more widely accepted and practiced than in the North, where no
such culture exists. Nisbett suggested that this culture of honor arose because
of the different peoples who settled in the North and South in the 17th and
18th centuries.
The
South was largely settled by people who came from herding economies in Europe,
most notably from borderlands of Scotland and Ireland (Nisbett, 1993). The
North, in contrast, was settled by Puritans, Quakers, and Dutch farmers, who
developed a more agriculturally based economy (Nisbett, 1993). According to
Nisbett, violence is more endemic to herding cultures, because it is important
to be constantly vigilant for theft of one’s
livestock. It was important in these herding economies to respond to any threat to one’s herd or grazing lands with sufficient force to drive away
intruders or potential thieves. Nisbett maintains that from this herding
economy arose the culture of honor that persists in the South to this day. This
culture of honor primes southern individuals for greater violence than their
northern counterparts.
Is there any evidence to support the
supposition that individuals from a herding economy are more predisposed to
honor-related aggression than those from other economies? One study provides
some support for this relationship (Figueredo, Tal, McNeil, & Guillen,
2004). Figueredo et al. looked at whether herding and farming populations
differ in their adherence to a culture of honor, using participant samples from
Mexico and other Central American countries. Consistent with the hypothesis
stated by Nesbitt and his colleagues, individuals from herding populations were
more likely to adhere to the culture of honor (e.g., more likely to endorse
revenge) than those making up farming communities.
What evidence do we have that such a
culture of honor exists and that it affects violence levels in the South?
Nisbett (1993) reported that when southern and northern cities of equal size
and demographic makeup are compared, there is a higher homicide rate among
southern white males than among northern white males. This difference is only
true for argument-related homicides, not for homicides resulting from other
felonies (e.g., robbery; Cohen, Nisbett, Bowdle, & Schwartz, 1996).
Interestingly, this regional difference holds only for white males and not
African American males (Nisbett, Polly, & Lang, 1995). Additionally,
Nisbett found a greater acceptance of violence to solve interpersonal conflicts
and to respond to a perceived insult among southern than among northern white
males. The differences between southern and northern white males is most
pronounced for behaviors that receive moderate to low support from the general
public (Hayes & Lee, 2005). Hayes and Lee found that differences emerged
between northern and southern white males on the following behaviors (p. 613):
1.
If an adult male stranger hit a man’s child after accidentally damaging
the stranger’s car,
2.
If a drunk adult male stranger bumped into a man and his wife on the
street,
3.
If an adult male stranger was encountered by a man at a protest rally
showing opposition to the man’s views.
No difference was found between
northerners and southerners for behaviors receiving more widespread approval.
For example, no difference was found for a scenario involving an adult male
punching a woman.
Findings, based on homicide rates, were
verified by Nisbett and his colleagues in a series of experiments. In a field
experiment (Cohen & Nisbett, 1997), employers in various parts of the
United States were sent a letter from a potential job applicant who committed
either an honor-based homicide (killing someone who was having an affair with
his fiancé) or an auto theft. Each response was analyzed for whether an
application was sent to the potential employee and the tone of the return
letter. Cohen and Nisbett found that more southern-based companies sent a job
application to the employee convicted of manslaughter than did northern-based
companies. However, there was no difference between southern and northern
companies in the rate of compliance to the employee who stole a car.
Additionally, the tone of the letters coming from southern companies was warmer
and more understanding of the homicide than was the tone of the letters from
northern companies. Again, there was no difference in warmth or understanding
between northern and southern companies for the theft letter.
Regional differences in violence between
the North and South have been well documented. But is the culture of honor
responsible? Are southern males more likely to react negatively to insults than
northern males? In a series of interesting laboratory experiments (Cohen et
al., 1996), southern and northern white males were insulted or not insulted by
a male confederate of the experimenter. In one experiment, Cohen and colleagues
(1996) were interested in whether there was a difference between southerners
and northerners in their physiological responses to the insult. Participants
were told that they were going to take part in an experiment that required
monitoring of blood sugar levels. Saliva samples were obtained from
participants before and after the insult (or no insult). The saliva samples
were analyzed for cortisol and testosterone levels. (Cortisol is a
stress-related hormone that increases when one is aroused or under stress.)The
results from this experiment are shown in Figure 10.5 (testosterone levels) and
Figure 10.6 (cortisol levels). As you can see, there was no difference between
insulted and noninsulted northern participants for both cortisol and
testosterone levels. However, for southern participants, there was a
significant rise in both cortisol and testosterone levels for insulted southern
participants (compared to the noninsulted southerners). Thus, in response to an
insult, southern white males are more “primed” physiologically for aggression
than their northern counterparts (Cohen et al., 1996). In another experiment,
Cohen and colleagues (1996) found that after being publicly insulted (compared
to being privately insulted or not insulted), southern white males were more
likely to experience a drop in perceived masculinity. No such difference was
found for northern white males.
Figure 10.5Percentage testosterone change
as a function of culture and insult. Northerners did not show a significant
increase in testosterone levels after being insulted. Southerners, on the other
hand, showed substantial increases in testosterone levels after being insulted.
Figure 10.6Percentage cortisol change as a
function of culture and insult. Northerners did not show a significant increase
in cortisol levels after being insulted. In contrast, southerners showed an
increase in cortisol levels after being insulted.
Cohen (1998) investigated those aspects of
southern and western culture that relate most closely to the acceptance and use
of violence. Cohen looked at the role of community and family stability in
explaining honor-based violence. Cohen hypothesized that among more stable
communities, reputations and honor would have more meaning than in less stable
communities. As a consequence, more honor-based violence was expected in stable
than in unstable communities. Homicide rates among stable and unstable
communities in the North, South, and West were compared. Cohen found a higher
honor-based homicide rate among stable southern and western communities than
among unstable southern and western communities. No such difference existed for
stable and unstable northern communities. Cohen also found that the rate of
felonyrelated homicides (not related to honor) was lower among stable than
among unstable communities in the South and West, but not in the North.
Additionally, Cohen found that honor-related homicides were higher among
communities in the South and West in which traditional families (i.e., intact
nuclear families) were more common than less common. The opposite was true for
northern communities. Thus, the manner in which cultures evolve, with respect
to stability and adherence to traditional family structures, relates closely to
patterns of violence. In the South and West, evolution toward community
stability (in which honor and reputation in the South and West are important)
and adherence to more traditional family structures give rise to higher levels
of violence. Such is not the case for northerners, for whom honor and
reputation appear to be less important.
Further evidence for a unique southern
culture of honor is provided in another study by Cohen (1996). Cohen compared
northern and southern (and western) states with respect to gun-control laws,
self-defense laws, treatment of violence used in defense of one’s property,
laws concerning corporal punishment, capital punishment laws, and stances taken
by legislators on using military responses to threats to U.S. national
interests. Cohen found that compared to northern states, southern (and western)
states had more lax gun-control laws, more lenient laws concerning using
violence for self-defense and protection of property, more lenient laws for
domestic violence offenders (where disciplining one’s wife is used as a
justification for male perpetrators of domestic violence), and a greater
tolerance for the use of corporal punishment. Southern states were more likely
to execute condemned prisoners than northern or western states. Finally,
southern legislators were more likely to endorse the use of military force than
northern (or western) states. These findings support the conclusion that
cultural differences, embodied in regional laws, exist between the North and
South (and to a lesser extent between the West and the North). More lenient
laws in the South tend to sanction and support the use of violence.
Interestingly, the “culture of honor” may
not be unique to American culture. One study compared Polish and German young
adults’views concerning using aggression to defend one’s reputation (Szmajke &
Kubica, 2003). Szmajke and Kubica found that Polish young adults were more
favorably inclined toward using aggression in response to a social offense and
expected their children to react aggressively toward provocation from other
children.
The
Role of Television in Teaching Aggression
Although
parents play the major role in the socialization of children and probably
contribute most heavily to the development of aggressive scripts, children are
exposed to other models as well. Over the years, considerable attention has
focused on the role of television in socializing aggressive behaviors.
Generally, most research on this topic suggests that there is a link (though
not necessarily a causal link) between exposure to television violence and
aggressive behavior (Huesmann, 1988; Huesmann, Lagerspetz, & Eron, 1984;
Josephson, 1987). Evidence also suggests that the link between watching violent
programming and aggression persists from childhood through adolescence into
adulthood (Huesmann, Moise-Titus, Podolski, & Eron, 2003). A meta-analysis
conducted by Hogben (1998) revealed the following significant relationships:
1.
Viewing “justified” televised violence leads to more aggression.
2. Viewing violence with “inaccurate”
consequences leads to more violence.
3. Viewing “plausible” violence leads to more
aggression.
4. The effect of televised violence is stronger
for studies conducted outside the United States than those conducted in the
United States.
5. The size of the effect of television violence
on aggression is small.
Hogben
estimates that if violence were eliminated from television, the overall amount
of aggression we see in our culture would go down by around 10%.
We
should note at this point that research in this area has traditionally focused
on the effect of violent television content and direct, physical aggression.
However, research is now showing that there may also be an effect of depictions
of indirect aggression on indirect aggressive behavior. One study conducted in
England found that acts of indirect aggression are actually more frequent than
acts of physical or verbal aggression (Coyne & Archer, 2004). This study
also revealed that female characters on television were more likely to engage
in indirect aggression than male characters. Research is beginning to show a
link between viewing indirect aggression and the use of indirect aggression
(Coyne & Archer, 2005; Coyne, Archer, & Eslea, 2004). For example, a
study by Coyne and Archer (2005) found that girls who were exposed to media
portrayals of indirect aggression tended to show higher levels of that form of
aggression. Some early research in the area showed that males are more
influenced than females by violent television (Liebert & Baron, 1972). More
recent research suggests that gender may not be important in understanding the
relationship between exposure to televised violence and aggression (Huesmann et
al., 1984). The correlations between watching television violence and
aggression are about the same for male and female children. However, one
interesting gender difference exists. Children, especially males, who identify
with television characters (that is, want to be like them) are most influenced
by television violence.
Watching
television violence may also have some subtle effects. People who watch a lot
of violence on television tend to become desensitized to the suffering of
others, as we saw was the case with abused children (Rule & Ferguson,
1986). Furthermore, children who watch a lot of violent television generally
have a more favorable attitude toward aggressive behavior than do children who
watch less.
Even
sanctioned aggression can increase the incidence of aggressive behavior among
those who view it on television. The impact on aggression of well-publicized
heavyweight championship fights has been documented (Phillips, 1983). Among
adults, homicide rates were found to increase for 3 days after these boxing
matches (Miller, Heath, Molcan, & Dugoni, 1991). When a white person loses
the match, homicides of whites increase; when an African American loses the
match, homicides of African Americans increase. A similar effect can be seen
with suicide rates. The number of suicides increases during the month in which
a suicide is reported in the media compared to the month before the report appears
(Phillips, 1986). Interestingly, the rate remains high (again compared to the
month before the report) a month after the report.
Although
most studies support the general conclusion that there is a relationship
between watching media portrayals of violence and aggression, a few words of
caution are appropriate (Freedman, 1984):
1. The relationship may not be strong.
Correlational studies report relatively low correlations between watching media
violence and aggression, and experimental studies typically show weak effects.
2. Although watching violence on television is
associated with increased aggression, there is evidence that watching
television is also associated with socially appropriate behavior, such as
cooperative play or helping another child (Gadow & Sprafkin, 1987; Mares
& Woodard, 2005).
3. Other variables, such as parental
aggressiveness and socioeconomic status, also correlate significantly with
aggression (Huesmann et al., 1984). One 3-year study conducted in the
Netherlands found that the small correlation between violent television viewing
and aggression (r= .23 and .29 for boys and girls, respectively) virtually
disappeared when children’s preexisting levels of
aggression and intelligence were taken into account (Wiegman, Kuttschreuter,
& Baarda, 1992).
4. Many studies of media violence and aggression
are correlational and, as explained in Chapter 1, cannot be used to establish a
causal relationship between these two variables. Other variables, such as
parental aggressiveness, may contribute causally to both violent television
viewing and aggression in children.
Individual
personality characteristics and social conditions mediate the relationship
between exposure to violent content and aggressive behavior. For example,
Haridakis (2002) found that “disinhibition” (nonconformity to social norms) and
“locus of control” (perception of the degree to which one is controlled by
external events or internal motives) were significant predictors of
media-related aggression. Generally, individuals who are likely to conform and
have an external locus of control showed the most aggression. Children who
identify with TV characters and perceive TV violence to be realistic are most
affected by TV violence (Huesmann et al., 2003). Finally, violent media have a
greater effect on adolescents who feel alienated from school and victimized by
their peers (Slater, Henry, Swaim, & Cardador, 2004). With the connection
between exposure to televised violence and aggressive behavior established,
researchers have turned their attention to explaining why the relationship
exists. One explanation for this relationship is that exposure to violence on
television and movies contributes to the development of aggressive scripts (see
our previous discussion on this topic). Another possible explanation is that
exposure to aggressive media content may prime aggressive thoughts, making them
more accessible (Chory-Assad, 2004). There is some evidence for this.
Chory-Assad found that after watching sitcoms with high levels of verbal aggression,
participants produced high numbers of verbally aggressive thoughts
characterized by attacks on a person’s
character and competence. So, it appears that exposure to aggressive programming
increases aggressive thinking patterns.
Exposure
to Violent Video Games
Video
games have come a long way from the original “Pong” game (a rather crude tennis
game) to today’s highly realistic games. Many modern
video games involve elaborate
stories and scenarios designed to involve the player. These story lines are
quite successful in immersing the player in the game, maintaining interest and
arousal (Schneider, Lang, Shin, & Bradley, 2004). Additionally, many
popular games involve moderate to high levels of violence. The popularity of
video games containing highly realistic violent content has raised concerns
about the effects of such games on children’s
behavior. A major concern is that exposure to these realistic, violent games
can cause
children and adults to behave aggressively. In recent years social scientists
have addressed this concern. In this section we shall explore the relationship
between playing violent video games and overt aggression.
The
main question we need to address is whether exposure to violent video games
increases aggression. The answer to this question is that it can (Anderson
& Bushman, 2001). Anderson and Bushman conducted a meta-analysis of the
literature and concluded that playing violent video games increased aggression
among both males and females. This was the case regardless of whether the study
reported was experimental or correlational. Additionally, playing violent video
games increases physiological arousal and aggressive thoughts and emotions.
Violent video games were also associated with a short-term decrease in
prosocial behavior. Generally, research suggests that there is a link between
playing violent video games and aggression, and that link is quite strong
(Anderson & Bushman, 2001; Anderson & Dill, 2000). However, the effect
of playing violent video games on aggression is probably not as strong as the
effect of televised violence on aggression (Sherry, 2001). Playing violent
video games has also been found to increase an individual’s immediate level of “state hostility.” That is, playing a
violent video
game increases hostility while the person is playing the game (Arriaga,
Esteves, Carniero, & Montiero, 2006).
Interestingly,
playing a violent video game activates parts of the brain that are commonly
associated with aggressive thoughts and behavior. In a study conducted by Weber,
Ritterfield, and Mathiak (2006), participants played a video game that had
violent and nonviolent sequences while undergoing a functional MRI (fMRI) scan.
Weber et al. found that while playing the violent segments of the game, there
was activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (normally associated
with aggression) and suppression of the anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala.
Weber et al. suggest that this pattern of brain activity indicates that areas
of the brain associated with emotions such as empathy are suppressed, allowing
the game player to engage in the violent activities needed for the game.
How
about gender effects? Anderson and Bushman’s
(2001) meta-analysis showed that both males and females are affected by playing
violent video games. Research confirms that females are affected by violent
video games (Anderson & Murphy, 2003).
However,
one experiment suggests that the effect of violent video games is more
pronounced for men than for women (Bartholow & Anderson, 2002). These researchers
found that males delivered more intense punishment on another person after
playing a violent video game (compared to a nonviolent video game) than females
under the same conditions. Finally, research also suggests that females are
most affected by violent video games when they control a female character in
the game (Anderson & Murphy, 2003). At this time, we don’t know if a similar effect exists for males.
As
is the case with exposure to violent television, playing violent video games
does not affect everyone equally. Long-term playing of violent video games is
associated with increased aggression most strongly among people with aggressive
personalities (Anderson & Dill, 2000). Among these individuals, exposure to
high levels of video game violence produces high levels of aggression.
Individuals with less aggressive personalities are less affected by video game
violence. Based on their experimental and correlational studies, Anderson and
Dill suggest that playing violent video games increases real-life aggression
(delinquent behavior) and aggression under controlled conditions. They suggest
that playing violent video games primes a person for aggression by increasing
aggressive thoughts.
Media
Violence and Aggression: Summing Up
Exposure
to media violence is one among many factors that can contribute to aggression
(Huesmann et al., 1984). Available research shows a consistent but sometimes
small relationship between media violence and aggression. But interpersonal
aggression probably can best be explained with a multiprocess model, one that
includes media violence and a wide range of other influences (Huesmann et al.,
1984). In all likelihood, media violence interacts with other variables in
complex ways to produce aggression.
Viewing
Sexual Violence: The Impact on Aggression
Television
and video games are not the only media that has come under fire for depicting
violence. Many groups have protested the depiction of violence against women in
pornographic magazines, movies, and on the Internet. These groups claim that
such sexually explicit materials influence the expression of violence,
particularly sexual violence, against women in real life.
In
the debate about pornographic materials, researchers have made a distinction
between sexually explicit and sexually violent materials (Linz, Penrod, &
Donnerstein, 1987). Sexually explicit materials are those specifically created
to produce sexual arousal. A scene in a movie depicting two nude people
engaging in various forms of consensual sex is sexually explicit. Sexually
violent material includes scenes of violence within a sexual context that are
degrading to women. These scenes need not necessarily be sexually explicit
(e.g., showing nudity). A rape scene (with or without nudity) is sexually
violent. Of course, materials can be both sexually explicit and sexually
violent.
Although
the causes of rape are complex (Groth, 1979; Malamuth, 1986), some researchers
and observers have focused on pornography as a factor that contributes to the
social climate in which sexual violence against women is tolerated. However,
not all forms of pornography are associated with sexual violence. Exposure to
sexually violent materials does relate to increased sexual violence (Malamuth
& Check, 1983). However, mild, nonviolent forms of erotica, such as
pictures from Playboymagazine or scenes of sex between consenting couples, may
inhibit sexual violence against women (Donnerstein, Donnerstein, & Evans,
1975, p. 175).
In
a study reported by Donnelly and Fraser (1998), 320 college students responded
to a questionnaire concerning arousal to sadomasochistic fantasies and acts.
The results showed that males were significantly more likely to be aroused by
fantasizing about and engaging in sadomasochistic sexual acts. Specifically,
males scored higher than females on measures of being dominant during sex,
participating in bondage and discipline, being restrained, and being spanked.
In terms of arousal to behaviors, males scored higher than females on watching
bondage and discipline, being dominant during sex, and taking part in
discipline and bondage.
Of
course, sexual arousal does not usually lead to aggression. Most males can
easily control their sexual and aggressive impulses. A wide range of social
norms, personal ethics, and moral beliefs act to moderate the expression of
violence toward women, even when conditions exist that, according to research,
lead to increased violence.
The
Impact of Sexually Violent Material on Attitudes
Besides
increasing violence against women, exposure to sexually violent material has
another damaging effect. It fosters attitudes, especially among males, that
tacitly allow rape to continue. There is a pervasive rape myth in U.S. society,
which fosters such beliefs as “only bad girls get raped,” “if a woman gets raped,
she must have asked for it,” “women Ę»cry
rape’only when they’ve been jilted or have something to cover up,” and “when a woman
says no, she really means yes” (Burt, 1980, p. 217; Groth, 1979).
Men
are more likely than women to accept the rape myth (Muir, Lonsway, & Payne,
1996). Additionally, such beliefs are most common among men who believe in
stereotyped sex roles, hold adversarial sexual beliefs, and find interpersonal
aggression an acceptable form of behavior. Thus, the rape myth is integrally
tied to a whole set of related attitudes (Burt, 1980). Interestingly, research
shows that the rape myth may be stronger in U.S. culture than in other
cultures. Muir, Lonsway, and Payne (1996) compared U.S. and Scottish
individuals for acceptance of the rape myth. They found that the rape myth was more
pervasive among Americans that Scots.
Do
media portrayals of sexual violence contribute to rape myths and attitudes?
Research suggests that they do (Malamuth & Check, 1981, 1985). In these
studies, viewing sexually explicit, violent films increased male (but not
female) participants’ acceptance of violence against women. Such
portrayals also tended to reinforce rape myths. Media portrayals of a woman
enjoying sexual violence had their strongest impact on males who were already
predisposed to violence against women (Malamuth & Check, 1985). Men who are
likely to commit rape also have beliefs that support the rape myth, such as a
belief that rape is justified and the perception that the victim enjoyed the
rape (Linz, Penrod, & Donnerstein, 1987; Malamuth & Check, 1981).
Malamuth
and Check, for example, had some participants watch films widely distributed in
mainstream movie theaters that depicted sexual violence against women (e.g.,
The Getaway). In these films, the sexual violence was portrayed as justified
and having positive consequences. Other participants watched films with no
sexual violence (e.g., Hooper). After viewing the films, participants (both
male and female) completed measures of rape-myth acceptance and acceptance of
interpersonal violence. The results showed that for male participants, exposure
to the films with sexual violence against women increased acceptance of the
rape myth and acceptance of interpersonal violence against women. Female
participants showed no such increase in acceptance of the rape myth or in
violence against women. In fact, there was a slight trend in the opposite
direction for female participants.
These
“softer” portrayals of sexual violence with unrealistic outcomes in films and
on television (e.g., the raped woman marrying her rapist) may have a more
pernicious effect than hard-core pornography. Because they are widely
available, many individuals see these materials and may be affected by them.
The appetite for such films has not subsided since Malamuth and Check’s 1981 experiment, and films depicting violence against women are
still made and widely distributed.
Finally,
one need not view sexually explicit or violent materials in order for one’s attitudes
toward women and sexual violence to be altered. McKay and Covell (1997)
reported that male students who looked at magazine advertisements with sexual
images (compared to those who saw more “progressive” images) expressed
attitudes that showed greater acceptance of interpersonal violence and the rape
myth. They were also more likely to express adversarial sexual attitudes and
less acceptance of the women’s movement.
Men
Prone to Sexual Aggression: Psychological Characteristics
We
have seen that male college students are aroused by depictions of rape and can
be instigated to aggression against women through exposure to sexually
explicit, violent materials. Does this mean that all, or at least most, males
have a great potential for sexual aggression, given the appropriate
circumstances? No, apparently not. Psychological characteristics play a part in
a man’s inclination to express sexual
aggression against women
(Malamuth, 1986).
In
one study, six variables were investigated to see how they related to
self-reported sexual aggression. The six predictor variables were:
1. Dominance as a motive for sexual behavior
2. Hostility toward women
3. Accepting attitudes toward sexual aggression
4. Antisocial characteristics or psychoticism
5. Sexual experience
6. Physiological arousal to depictions of rape
Participants’sexual aggression was assessed by a test that measured whether
pressure, coercion, force, and so on were used in sexual relationships.
Positive
correlations were found between five of the six predictor variables and sexual
aggression directed against women. Psychoticism was the only variable that did
not correlate significantly with aggression. However, the presence of any one
predictor alone was not likely to result in sexual aggression. Instead, the
predictor variables tended to interact to influence sexual aggression. For
example, arousal to depictions of rape is not likely to translate into sexual
aggression unless other variables are present. So, just because a man is
aroused by depictions of rape, he will not necessarily be sexually violent with
women. In other words, several variables interact to predispose a man toward
sexual aggression.
Lackie
and de Man (1997) investigated the relationship between several variables,
including sex-role attitudes, physical aggression, hostility toward women,
alcohol use, and fraternity affiliation, and sexual aggression. Their findings
showed that sexually aggressive males tended to be physically aggressive in
general. Furthermore, they found that stereotyped sex-role beliefs, acceptance
of interpersonal violence, masculinity, and fraternity membership were
positively related to self-reported sexual aggression. They also found that the
most important predictors of sexual aggression were the use of physical
aggression, stereotyped sex-role beliefs, and fraternity membership. In another
study, Carr and VanDeusen (2004) found a similar pattern of results. Carr and
VanDeusen found that four variables significantly related to sexual violence.
These were alcohol use, exposure to pornography, sexual conservatism, and
acceptance of interpersonal violence. Those prone to sexual violence used
alcohol and pornography to a greater extent, were more sexually conservative,
and were more accepting of interpersonal violence than those less prone to
sexual violence.
So,
whether an individual will be sexually aggressive is mediated by other factors.
For example, Dean and Malamuth (1997) found that males who are at risk for
sexual violence against women were most likely to behave in a sexually
aggressive way if they were also self-centered. A high-risk male who is not
self-centered but rather is sensitive to the needs of others is not likely to
behave in a sexually aggressive way. However, regardless of whether a high-risk
male is self-centered, he is likely to fantasize about sexual violence (Dean
& Malamuth, 1997). Additionally, feelings of empathy also appear to mediate
sexual aggression. Malamuth, Heavey, and Linz found that males who are high in
empathy are less likely to show arousal to scenes of sexual violence than males
who are low in empathy (cited in Dean & Malamuth, 1997).
What
do we know, then, about the effects of exposure to sexual violence on
aggression? The research suggests the following conclusions:
1. Exposure to mild forms of nonviolent erotica
tends to decrease sexual aggression against women.
2. Exposure to explicit or sexually violent
erotica tends to increase sexual aggression against women but not against men.
3. Individuals who are angry are more likely to
be more aggressive after viewing sexually explicit or violent materials than
are individuals who are not angry.
4. Male college students are aroused by
depictions of rape. However, men who show a greater predisposition to rape are
more aroused, especially if the woman is portrayed as being aroused.
5. Exposure to media portrayals of sexual
aggression against women increases acceptance of such acts and contributes to
the rape myth. Thus, sexually explicit, violent materials contribute to a
social climate that tolerates rape.
6. No single psychological characteristic
predisposes a man to sexual aggression. Instead, several characteristics
interact to increase the likelihood that a man will be sexually aggressive
toward women.
Reducing
Aggression
We
have seen that interpersonal aggression comes in many different forms,
including murder, rioting, and sexual violence. We also have seen that many
different factors can contribute to aggression, including innate biological
impulses, situational factors such as frustration, situational cues such as the
presence of weapons, and aggressive scripts internalized through the process of
socialization. We turn now to a more practical question: What can be done to
reduce aggression? Although aggression can be addressed on a societal level,
such as through laws regulating violent television programming and pornography,
the best approach is to undermine aggression in childhood, before it becomes a
life script.
Reducing
Aggression in the Family
According
to the social-interactional model described earlier in this chapter, antisocial
behavior begins early in life and results from poor parenting. The time to
target aggression, then, is during early childhood, when the socialization
process is just under way.
Teachers,
health workers, and police need to look for signs of abuse and neglect and
intervene as soon as possible (Widom, 1992). Waiting until an aggressive child
is older is not the best course of action (Patterson et al., 1989).
Intervention attempts with adolescents produce only temporary reductions in
aggression, at best.
One
way to counter the development of aggression is to give parents guidance with
their parenting. Parents who show tendencies toward inept parenting can be
identified, perhaps through child-welfare agencies or schools, and offered
training programs in productive parenting skills. Such training programs have
been shown to be effective in reducing noncompliant and aggressive behavior in
children (Forehand & Long, 1991). Children whose parents received training
in productive parenting skills were also less likely to show aggressive
behavior as adolescents.
What
types of parenting techniques are most effective in minimizing aggression?
Parents should avoid techniques that provide children with aggressive role
models. Recommended techniques include positive reinforcement of desired behaviors
and time-outs (separating a child from activities for a time) for undesired
behaviors. Also, parenting that involves inductive techniques, or giving
age-relevant explanations for discipline, is related to lowered levels of
juvenile crime (Shaw & Scott, 1991). Parents can also encourage prosocial
behaviors that involve helping, cooperating, and sharing. It is a simple fact
that prosocial behavior is incompatible with aggression. If a child learns to
be empathic and altruistic in his or her social interactions, aggression is
less likely to occur. To support the development of prosocial behaviors,
parents can take four specific steps (Bee, 1992, pp. 331–443):
1. Set clear rules and explain to children why
certain behaviors are unacceptable. For example, tell a child that if he or she
hits another child, that other child will be hurt.
2. Provide children with age-appropriate
opportunities to help others, such as setting the table, cooking dinner, and
teaching younger siblings.
3. Attribute prosocial behavior to the child’s internal characteristics; for example, tell the child
how helpful he or she is.
4. Provide children with prosocial role models
who demonstrate caring, empathy, helping, and other positive traits.
Reducing
Aggression with Cognitive Intervention and Therapy
Reducing
aggression through better parenting is a long-term, global solution to the
problem. Another more direct approach to aggression in specific individuals
makes use of cognitive intervention. We have seen that children who are exposed
to violence develop aggressive scripts. These scripts increase the likelihood
that a child will interpret social situations in an aggressive way. Dodge
(1986) suggested that aggression is mediated by the way we process information
about our social world. According to this social information-processing view of
aggression, there are five important steps involved in instigating aggression
(as well as other forms of social interaction). These are (as cited in Kendall,
Ronan, & Epps, 1991):
1. We perceive and decode cues from our social
environment.
2. We develop expectations of others’behavior based on our attribution of intent.
3. We look for possible responses.
4. We decide which response is most appropriate.
5. We carry out the chosen response.
Individuals
with aggressive tendencies see their own feelings reflected in the world.
They
are likely to interpret and make attributions about the behaviors of others
that center on aggressive intent. This leads them to respond aggressively to
the perceived threat. Generally, aggressive individuals interpret the world as
a hostile place, choose aggression as a desired way to solve conflict, and
enact those aggressive behaviors to solve problems (Kendall et al., 1991).
Programs
to assess and treat aggressive children have been developed using cognitive
intervention techniques. Some programs use behavior management strategies
(teaching individuals to effectively manage their social behavior) to establish
and enforce rules in a nonconfrontational way (Kendall et al., 1991).
Aggressive children (and adults) can be exposed to positive role models and
taught to consider nonaggressive solutions to problems.
Other
programs focus more specifically on teaching aggressive individuals new
information-processing and social skills that they can use to solve
interpersonal problems (Pepler, King, & Byrd, 1991; Sukhodolsky, Golub,
Stone, & Orban, 2005). Individuals are taught to listen to what others say
and, more important, think about what they are saying. They are also taught how
to correctly interpret others’behaviors,
thoughts, and
feelings, and how to select nonaggressive behaviors to solve interpersonal
problems. These skills are practiced in role-playing sessions where various
scenarios that could lead to aggression are acted out and analyzed. In essence,
the aggressive child (or adult) is taught to reinterpret social situations in a
less-threatening, less-hostile way. Cognitively-based interventions may also be
effective with high-risk individuals.
LeSure-Lester
(2002) contrasted a cognitive intervention program that included anger
recognition, self-talk, and alternatives to aggression with a more traditional
intervention with a sample of abused African American adolescents.
LeSure-Lester found that the cognitive intervention resulted in greater
reductions in aggressive behavior than the more traditional intervention.
As
you can see, cognitively based therapy techniques have produced some
encouraging results. It appears that they can be effective in changing an
individual’s perceptions of social events and in
reducing aggression. However, the jury is still out on these programs. It may be
best to view them as just one technique among many to help reduce aggression.
Other
therapeutic techniques might also be effective in reducing aggression. In one
study conducted in Israel, group-based “bibliotherapy” involving both the
mother and child was most successful in reducing children’s aggression (Schectman & BiraniNasaraladen, 2006). Among
schoolchildren,
using a system that reinforced nonaggressive behavior on the playground (a
straight behavioral intervention) also is effective in reducing aggression
(Roderick, Pitchford, & Miller, 1997).
The
Beltway Sniper Case Revisited
The
fate that befell the victims of the Beltway Snipers was the result of naked
aggression directed against them. We would classify the type of aggression
displayed by the Beltway Snipers as instrumental aggression. The fact that
Muhammad and Malvo planned to extort money and/or use the random victims to set
up a final murder of Muhammad’s ex-wife suggests
that they were using the killings as a means to an end.
Although
it would be difficult to pinpoint an exact cause for the Beltway Snipers’ shooting
spree, it is fairly clear that there were no physiological causes for the
aggression (e.g., no damage to the hypothalamus). The best explanations for the
shooting spree might lie in the frustration-aggression and social learning
perspectives. It seems evident that Muhammad was deeply frustrated and angry
over the custody dispute with his ex-wife. We have seen how frustration,
mediated by anger, can provoke aggressive behavior. Further, Muhammad learned
skills in the military that lent themselves to the sniper-type method he used
to kill his victims. Lee Malvo’s motives are more
difficult
to determine. Was there something in his childhood that could explain his
behavior? Malvo came from a poor, single-parent family. He was raised by his
mother (who was not married to Malvo’s father). Malvo’s father left the scene when Lee was an infant and Lee
rarely saw his father. Recall from the social-interactional model of aggression
how family experiences can shape a person’s
tendencies toward aggressive behavior. It may well be that Lee Malvo’s childhood experiences shaped his behavior later in his life.
Chapter
Review
1.
How do social psychologists define aggression?
For
social psychologists, the term aggressioncarries a very specific meaning, which
differs from a layperson’s definition. For social psychologists,
aggression is
any behavior intended to inflict harm (whether psychological or physical) on
another organism or object. Key to this definition are the notions of intent
and the fact that harm need not be limited to physical harm but can also
include psychological harm.
2.
What are the different types of aggression?
Social
psychologists distinguish different types of aggression, including hostile
aggression (aggression stemming from emotions such as anger or hatred) and
instrumental aggression (aggression used to achieve a goal). Direct aggression
refers to overt forms of aggression such as physical aggression and verbal
aggression. Indirect aggression is aggression that is social in nature. Another
type of aggression called relational aggression (using social ostracism,
rejection, and direct confrontation) has elements of both direct and indirect
aggression. Symbolic aggression involves doing things that block another person’s goals.
Sanctioned aggression is aggression that society approves, such as a soldier
killing in war or a police officer shooting a suspect in the line of duty.
3.
What are the gender differences in aggression?
Research has established that there are, in
fact, differences in aggression between males and females. One of the most
reliable differences between males and females is the male’s greater predisposition toward direct, physical aggression, most
evident among children. However, the role of gender in the use of indirect,
relational aggression is still an open question. Males tend to favor physical
aggression as a way to settle a dispute and are more likely than females to be
the target of aggression. Females, however, tend to use verbal aggression more
than males. Males and females also think differently about aggression. Females
tend to feel guiltier than males about using aggression and show more concern
for the harm done by aggression. The observed gender differences are most
likely a result of the interaction between biological and social forces.
Laboratory
research on gender differences in aggression suggests that the difference
between males and females is reliable but quite small. However, crime
statistics bear out the commonly held belief that males are more aggressive
than females. Across three major categories of violent crime (murder, robbery,
and assault), males commit far more violent crimes than females.
4.
How can we explain aggression?
As
is typical of most complex behaviors, aggression has multiple causes. Several
explanations for aggression can be offered, including both biological and
social factors.
5.
What are the ethological, sociobiological, and genetic explanations for
aggression?
Biological
explanations include attempts by ethologists and sociobiologists to explain
aggression as a behavior with survival value for individuals and for groups of
organisms. Ethology theory suggests that aggression is related to the
biological survival and evolution of an organism. This theory emphasizes the
roles of instincts and genetics. Sociobiology, like ethology, looks at
aggression as having survival value and resulting from competition among
members of a species. Aggression is seen as one behavior biologically
programmed into an organism. There is also a genetic component for aggression,
especially for males. Research has found that genetics and the common
environment combine to influence aggression. Most likely, genetics operates by
resulting in characteristics that predispose a person to behave aggressively.
However, just because a person has a genetic predisposition for aggression does
not guarantee that the person will behave aggressively.
6.
What role do brain mechanisms play in aggression?
The
roles of brain mechanisms and hormonal influences in aggression have also been
studied. Stimulation of certain parts of the brain elicits aggressive behavior.
The hypothalamus is one part of the brain that has been implicated in
aggression. Stimulation of one part of the hypothalamus in a cat leads to
emotional aggression, whereas stimulation of another elicits predatory
aggression. Interacting with social factors, these neurological factors
increase or decrease the likelihood of aggression. The male hormone
testosterone has also been linked to aggressive behavior. Higher concentrations
of testosterone are associated with more aggression. Like brain mechanisms,
hormonal influences interact with the social environment to influence
aggression.
7.
How does alcohol consumption relate to aggression?
Although
alcohol is considered a sedative, it tends to increase aggression. Research
shows that individuals who are intoxicated behave more aggressively than those
who are not. Furthermore, it is not only the pharmacological effects of alcohol
that increase aggression. An individual’s
expectations about the effects of alcohol also can increase aggression after consuming
a beverage
believed to be alcoholic. Alcohol appears
to operate on the brain to reduce
levels of the neurotransmitter serotonin.
This reduction is serotonin is related to increased aggression. Furthermore,
alcohol tends to suppress the executive cognitive functions that normally
operate to mediate aggressive responses. The alcohol-aggression link is
mediated by individual characteristics and the social situation. Individuals,
especially men, who are high on a characteristic known as dispositional empathy
are less likely to behave aggressively. It appears that alcohol interacts with
individual characteristics and the social situation to influence aggression.
8. What is the frustration-aggression
hypothesis?
The frustration-aggression hypothesis suggests
that aggression is caused by frustration resulting from blocked goals. This
hypothesis has raised much controversy. Once frustrated, we choose a target for
aggression. Our first choice is the source of the frustration, but if the
source is an inappropriate target, we may vent our frustration against another
target. This is called displaced aggression. Whether aggression is displaced
depends on three factors: the intensity of the original frustration, the
similarity between the original and displaced target, and the negativity of the
interaction between the individual and original target.
9. How does anger relate to frustration
and aggression, and what factors contribute to anger?
A modified version of the
frustration-aggression hypothesis suggests that frustration does not lead to
aggression unless a negative affect such as anger is aroused. Anger may be
aroused under several conditions. Cognitive mediators, such as attributions
about intent, have been found to play a role in the frustration-aggression link
as well. If we believe that another person intends to harm us, we are more
likely to react aggressively. If we are given a good reason for why we are
frustrated, we are less likely to react aggressively.
Another social psychological mechanism
operating to cause aggression is perceived injustice. Aggression can be used to
restore a sense of justice and equity in such situations. Research suggests
that a perceived inequity in a frustrating situation is a stronger cause for
aggression than frustration itself.High temperature also relates to
frustration-related aggression. Research shows that under conditions of high
temperature, aggression is likely to occur. One explanation for this is that
heat makes people cranky and more likely to interpret situations as aggressive,
calling for an aggressive response.
10. How does social learning theory
explain aggression?
According to social learning theory,
aggression is learned, much like any other human behavior. The primary means of
learning for social learning theorists is observational learning, or modeling.
By watching others we learn new behaviors or have preexisting behaviors
inhibited or disinhibited. Research confirms the role of early experience in
the development of aggressive behavior. Additionally, there is continuity
between childhood aggression and adult aggression.
11.
What are aggressive scripts, and how do they relate to aggression?
One
mechanism believed to underlie the relationship between observation and
aggression is the formation of an aggressive script during the socialization
process. These aggressive scripts lead a person to behave more aggressively and
to interpret social situations in aggressive terms. During the socialization
process, children develop aggressive scripts and behavior patterns because they
are exposed to acts of aggression, both within the family and in the media.
12.
How does the family socialize a child into aggression?
Research shows that aggressive behavior
patterns develop early in life. Research also shows that there is continuity
between childhood aggression and aggression later in life; that is, an
aggressive child is likely to grow into an aggressive adult.
According
to the social-interactional model, antisocial behavior such as aggression
results from inept parenting. Parental use of physical or verbal aggression is
related to heightened aggressiveness among children, a finding that extends
across cultures. Physical punishment is significantly associated with a variety
of negative outcomes, including aggressive behavior, lower levels of moral
internalization of behavior, degraded parent-child relationships, and poorer
mental health. Other research shows that verbal aggression directed at children
by parents is particularly problematic. Verbal aggression may signal parental
rejection, which has been associated with a host of negative outcomes,
including aggression.
Child
abuse and neglect also have been found to lead to increases in aggression (as
measured by violent crime). In addition, child abuse leads to a desensitization
to the suffering of others. An abused child is likely to respond to an agemate
in distress with anger and physical abuse, rather than concern or empathy (as
would a nonabused child). Child abuse, then, leads to a callous attitude toward
others as well as to increases in aggression.
Finally,
family disruption also relates to increases in aggression. Children from
disrupted homes have been found to engage in more criminal behavior as adults
than children from nondisrupted homes.
13.
What is the role of culture in aggression?
An
individual’s level of aggressiveness relates to the
cultural environment within
which he or she is reared. Cross-cultural research shows that aggression is
less likely to occur in cultures that have collectivist values, high levels of
moral discipline, egalitarian values, low levels of avoiding uncertainty, and
Confucian values.
Research
comparing individuals from the American South with the American North has shown
differences in attitudes toward using aggression. Generally, individuals from
the South are more favorable toward using aggression than individuals from the
North. One explanation for this is that a culture of honor has developed in the
South (and the West) because different people settled these regions during the
17th and 18th centuries. The South was settled by people from herding
economies, and these people were predisposed to be constantly vigilant for
theft on one’s stock and react with force to drive
intruders away to protect one’s property. From
this the culture of honor emerged.
14.
What role do the media play in aggression?
One important application of social learning
theory to the problem of aggression is the relationship between media
portrayals of aggression and aggressive behavior. Research suggests that children
who watch aggressive television programs tend to be more aggressive. Although
some early research suggested that males were more affected by television
violence than were females, more recent research suggests that there is no
reliable, general difference between males and females. One gender difference
that does emerge is that children, especially males, who identify with
television characters are most affected by television violence. Additionally,
heavy doses of television violence desensitize individuals to violence. A
meta-analysis has shown that televised violence is most likely to lead to overt
aggression when the violence shown on television is justified, is shown having
inaccurate consequences, and is plausible.
Although
many studies have established a link between watching media violence and
aggression, the observed effects are small. Additionally, televised violence
does not affect everyone in the same way. Some individuals are more prone to be
affected by televised violence than others.
15.
What are the effects of playing violent video games on aggressive behavior?
Research
shows that playing violent video games increases aggression among both males
and females. Additionally, playing violent video games increases physiological
arousal, aggressive thoughts and emotions, and state hostility.
Violent
video games are also associated with a short-term decrease in prosocial
behavior. Playing a violent video game activates parts of the brain that are
commonly associated with aggressive thoughts and behavior, while suppressing
parts of the brain associated with empathy. Finally, playing violent video
games does not affect everyone equally. Long-term playing of violent video
games is associated with increased aggression most strongly among people with
aggressive personalities.
16.
What is the link between sexual violence portrayed in the media and sexual
aggression directed toward women?
The
research on the link between violent sexual media portrayals and violence
directed at women leads to six conclusions: (1) Exposure to mild forms of
erotica tends to decrease sexual violence against women. (2) Exposure to
explicit or sexually violent erotica increases aggression against women but not
against men. (3) Individuals who are angry are more likely to be more
aggressive after viewing sexually explicit or violent materials than
individuals who are not angry. (4) Male college students are aroused by
depictions of rape. However, individuals who show a greater predisposition to
rape are more aroused, especially if the victim is shown being aroused by
sexual violence. (5) Exposure to media portrayals of sexual violence increases
acceptance of violence against women and contributes to the rape myth. Thus,
sexually explicit, violent pornography contributes to a social climate that
tolerates rape. (6) There is no single psychological characteristic that
predisposes a man to sexual violence. Instead, several characteristics interact
to increase the likelihood that a man will be sexually violent.
17.
How can aggression be reduced?
Many factors contribute to aggression,
including biological predispositions, frustration, the presence of aggressive
cues, the media, and family factors. The most fruitful approach to reducing
aggression is to target family factors that contribute to aggression.
Aggression can be reduced if parents change inept parenting styles, do not
abuse or neglect their children, and minimize family disruption. Parents should
reduce or eliminate their use of physical and verbal aggression directed at children.
Positive reinforcement for desired behavior and time-out techniques should be
used more often. Socializing children to be altruistic and caring can also help
reduce aggression.
According
to the cognitive approach, children are encouraged to reinterpret situations as
nonaggressive. The social information-processing view of aggression maintains
that there are five important steps involved in the instigation to aggression:
We perceive and decode cues from our social environment, we develop
expectations of others’behavior based on our attribution of intent, we
look for possible responses, we decide which response is most appropriate, and
we carry out the chosen response. The cognitive approach suggests that
aggressive individuals need to change their view of the world as a hostile
place, to manage their aggressive impulses, and to learn new social skills for
managing their interpersonal problems.
*********************************************
Social Psychology
Third Edition
Kenneth S. Bordens Indiana University—Purdue University Fort Wayne
Irwin A. Horowitz - Oregon State University
Social Psychology, 3rd Edition
Copyright ©2008 by Freeload Press
Illustration used on cover © 2008 JupiterImages Corporation
ISBN 1-930789-04-1
No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or
by any means, electronic, mechanical, recording, photocopying, or otherwise, without the prior written
permission of the publisher.
Printed in the United States of America by Freeload Press.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Komentar
Posting Komentar