11 Prosocial Behavior and Altruism 401
Prosocial Behavior and Altruism
Whoever destroys a single life is as guilty as though
he had destroyed the entire world; and whoever rescues a single life earns as
much merit as though he had rescued the entire world.
—The Talmud
When
Irene Gut Opdyke was growing up in Poland during the 1930s, she could never
have imagined the fate that the future had in store for her. Irene was born in
a small village in Poland on May 5, 1922. Early in her life she decided to
enter a profession that involved helping others, so she enrolled in nursing
school. However, Irene had to flee her home when the Nazis invaded Poland in
1939. Irene eventually joined a Polish underground unit but was beaten and
raped by a group of Russian soldiers who found her group in the woods.
Next,
Irene decided to try to fi nd her family and began making her way back home.
She was captured in a church by the Germans and forced to work in a munitions
plant. The work was physically demanding, and one day Irene collapsed under the
burden of her work. Because of her youth, Aryan appearance, and good looks,
Irene caught the eye of a German major named Eduard Rugemer. Rugemer arranged
for Irene to work in a local hotel that catered to German army and SS offi
cers. Her primary duties involved serving the offi cers their meals. It was
during her period of employment at the hotel that she fi rst noticed what was
happening to Jews. She saw fi rsthand the treatment the Jews endured in the
ghetto behind the hotel. She saw a baby flung into the air and shot by a Nazi.
She then decided that she had to do something. One of her fi rst helping acts
was to save table scraps and leave them for the starving dwellers of the
ghetto. As the war progressed, the Germans were forced to move their munitions
plant to Ternopol, Poland. Here Irene resumed her duties serving meals. Major
Rugemer also put Irene in charge of the laundry where she met a family of Jews
and befriended them. Irene started helping them by giving them food and
blankets. Around this time Major Rugemer also made Irene his personal
housekeeper. One day while serving a meal to the German officers she overheard
a conversation indicating that more and more Jews were to be rounded up and
killed. Her friends in the laundry were clearly in danger. So, Irene made a
momentous decision. She decided to hide the Jews to save them from
extermination.
At
first she hid the group behind a false wall in the laundry area. Then she hid
them in a heating duct in Major Rugemer’s apartment. When Major Rugemer moved
to a large villa with servant’s quarters in the cellar and a bunker beneath the
house, Irene took her charges and hid them in the cellar of Major Rugemer’s
villa.
One
day Irene was at the marketplace in town when the Gestapo herded everyone into
the town center. There a Polish family was hanged along with the Jewish family
they were hiding. Usually when Irene returned home, she locked the door and
left the key turned in the lock so nobody could come in unexpectedly. Irene was
so shaken by what she had witnessed that she locked the door, but pulled the
key out of the lock. Two members of the Jewish family, Fanka Silberman and Ida
Bauer, came out of the cellar to help Irene with her chores. The three were in
the kitchen when Major Rugemer came home unexpectedly and found them. Irene had
been caught and the Jews were in danger. Major Rugemer, visibly angry,
retreated to his study. Irene followed him and made a plea for her Jewish
friends. Major Rugemer agreed to let the Jews stay, but at a cost. Irene would
have to become his mistress.
Eventually,
Ternopol was liberated by the advancing Russian army. Irene and her charges fled
into the woods to await liberation. Irene Opdyke’s courageous acts were
directly responsible for saving Fanka Silberman, Henry Weinbaum, Moses Steiner,
Marian Wilner, Joseph Weiss, Alex Rosen, David Rosen, Lazar Haller, Clara
Bauer, Thomas Bauer, Abram Klinger, Miriam Morris, Hermann Morris, Herschel
Morris, and Pola Morris. Without Irene’s help they all surely would have ended
up in labor and/or death camps. After the war Irene’s story was verifi ed and
she was designated a righteous rescuer by the state of Israel.
What
motivated Irene Opdyke? Why did she risk her relatively secure position with
Major Rugemer for people she had only recently befriended? And, what about
Major Rugemer’s decision to allow Irene to continue hiding the Jews at his
villa? Was his action altruistic, or did he have another reason for his
behavior? Why do we care about the fate of other people? Indeed, do we care at
all? These are fundamental questions about human nature. Theologians,
philosophers, evolutionary biologists, and novelists all have suggested answers.
Social psychologists have suggested answers, too, contributing their empirical
findings to the discussion.
Irene
Opdyke’s behavior was clearly out of the ordinary. Very few Poles were willing
to risk their lives to save Jews. A notable aspect of Irene’s behavior was that
she expected nothing in return, neither material nor psychological rewards. In
fact, rescuers such as Irene Opdyke typically shy away from the hero status
awarded them. In her mind, she did what had to be done—end of story. Regardless,
her actions were purely altruistic. So Irene was an unusual human being—but not
unique. Others, albeit few, have performed equally selfless acts.
In
this chapter we consider why people help others, when they help, and what kinds
of people help. We ask, what lies behind behavior such as Irene Opdyke’s? Does
it spring from compassion for her fellow human beings? Does it come from a need
to be able to sleep at night, to live with ourselves? Or is there some other
motivation? What circumstances led Opdyke to offer the help she did, and what
process
did she go through to arrive at this decision? Or was her decision more a
function of her character, her personal traits? Was she perhaps an example of
an altruistic personality? And what about the people Irene Opdyke saved? How
did receiving her help affect them? What factors determined how they responded
to that help? These are some of the questions addressed in this chapter.
Why
Do People Help?
There
are two types of motives for behaviors such as Irene Opdyke’s. Sometimes we help because we want to relieve a person’s suffering. Behavior motivated by the desire to relieve a victim’s suffering is called altruism. Other times we help because we hope to gain
something from it for ourselves. We may give to a charity to get a tax
deduction, for example, or we may give because we think it makes us look good.
Often, we experience personal satisfaction and increased self-esteem after
helping. When we give help with an eye on the reward we will get, our behavior
is not really altruistic. It falls into the category of behaviors known simply
as helping behavior.
Notice
that the distinction between altruism and helping behavior lies in the
motivation for performing the behavior, not the outcome. A person who is
motivated purely by the need to relieve the suffering of the victim may receive
a reward for his or her actions. However, he or she didn’t perform the actions with the expectation of receiving that
reward. This marks the behavior as altruistic.
The
distinction between altruism and helping behavior may seem artificial because
the outcome in both cases is that someone in need receives help. Does it matter
what motivates the behavior? Yes, it does. The quality of the help given may
vary according to the motivation behind the behavior. For example, there were
others besides Irene Opdyke who helped rescue Jews, but some of them were paid
for their efforts. The Jews who paid their helpers were not necessarily treated
very well. In fact, Christians in Nazi-occupied Europe who helped hide Jews for
pay did not extend the same level of care as those who were not paid. Jews
hidden by “paid helpers” were more likely to be mistreated, abused, and turned
in than were those hidden by the more altruistic “rescuers” (Tec, 1986).
The
question posed by social psychologists about all of these acts is, What
motivates people to help? Is there really such a thing as altruism, or are
people always hoping for some personal reward when they help others?
Researchers have proposed a number of hypotheses to answer this question.
Empathy:
Helping in Order to Relieve Another’s Suffering
Social
psychologist C. Daniel Batson (1987, 1990a, 1990b) suggested that we may help
others because we truly care about them and their suffering. This caring occurs
because humans have strong feelings of empathy—compassionate understanding of
how the person in need feels. Feelings of empathy encompass sympathy, pity, and
sorrow (Eisenberg & Miller, 1987).
What
cognitive and/or emotional experience underlies empathy? Batson, Early, and
Salvarani (1997) suggested that perspective taking is at the heart of helping
acts. According to Batson and colleagues, there are two perspectives that are
relevant to helping situations: imagine other and imagine self. An
imagine-other perspective operates when you think about how the person in need
of help perceives the helping situation and the feelings that are aroused in
that situation. An imagine-self perspective operates when you imagine how you
would think and feel if you were in the victim’s
situation.
Batson and colleagues predicted that the perspective taken affects the arousal
of empathy or personal distress.
Batson
and colleagues (1997) conducted an experiment in which participants were told
to adopt one of three perspectives while listening to a story about a person in
need (Katie). In the objective-perspective condition, participants were
instructed to be as objective as possible and not to imagine what the person
had been through. In the imagine-other condition, participants were instructed
to try to imagine how the person in need felt about what had happened. In the
imagine-self condition, participants were told to imagine how they themselves
would feel in the situation. Batson and colleagues measured the extent to which
the manipulation produced feelings of empathy or personal distress.
Batson
and colleagues (1997) found that participants in both imagine conditions felt
more empathy for Katie than did those in the objective condition. Furthermore,
they found that participants in the imagine-other condition felt more empathy
than did those in the imagine-self condition. Participants in the imagine-self
condition were more likely to experience personal distress than empathy. Thus,
two emotional experiences were produced depending on which perspective a person
took.
How
does empathy relate to altruism? Although attempts to answer this question have
been somewhat controversial, it appears that empathy, once aroused, increases
the likelihood of an altruistic act. This is exactly what is predicted from
Batson and colleagues’(1997) empathy-altruism hypothesis.
Psychologists, however, have never been comfortable with the idea that people
may do selfless acts. The idea of a truly altruistic act runs contrary to the
behaviorist tradition in psychology. According to this view, behavior is under
control of overt reinforcers and punishers. Behavior develops and is maintained
if it is reinforced. Thus, the very idea of a selfless, nonrewarded act seems
farfetched.
Empathy
and Egoism: Two Paths to Helping
When
we see or hear about someone in need, we often experience personal distress.
Now, distress is an unpleasant emotion, and we try to avoid it. After all, most
of us do not like to see others suffer. Therefore, we may give help not out of
feelings of empathy for victims but in order to relieve our own personal
distress. This motive for helping is called egoism. For example, if you saw the
suffering after Hurricane Katrina and thought, “If I don’t do something, I’ll feel terrible all day,” you would be
focused on your own distress rather than on the distress of the victims.
Generally, egoistic motives are more self-centered and selfish than empathic
motives (Batson, Fultz, & Schoenrade, 1987). Thus, there are different
paths to helping, one involving empathy and the other personal distress. These
two competing explanations of helping are shown in Figure 11.1.
How
can we know which of these two paths better explains helping behavior? Note
that when the motivation is to reduce personal distress, helping is only one
solution. Another is to remove ourselves from the situation. But when the
motivation is altruistic, only one solution is effective: helping the victim.
The egoist, motivated by reducing personal distress, is more likely to respond
to someone in need by escaping the situation if possible. The altruist,
motivated by empathy for the victim, is not.
Figure
11.1Helping as a function of ease of escape and empathy. Participants high in
empathy are likely to help a person in need, even if escape is easy.
Participants low in empathy help only if escape is difficult.
From.
Batson, Fultz, and Schoenrade (1987
Batson
designed some experiments to test the relative merits of the personal distress
versus the empathy-altruism explanations by varying the ease with which
subjects could avoid contact with the person in need. In one study, subjects
watched someone (apparently) experiencing pain in response to a series of
electric shocks (Batson, 1990a). Some subjects were told that they would see
more of the shock series—the difficultescape condition. Others were told that
they would see no more of the shock series, although the victim would still get
shocked—the easy-escape condition.
The
personal distress reduction explanation predicts that everyone will behave the
same in this situation. When escape is easy, everyone will avoid helping—we all
want to relieve our feelings of personal distress. When escape is difficult,
everyone will help—again, we all want to relieve our feelings of personal
distress. The empathy-altruism explanation, on the other hand, predicts that
people will behave differently, depending on their motivation. This will be
particularly apparent when it is easy to escape. Under these conditions, those
motivated by egoistic concerns will escape. Those motivated by empathy will
help even though they easily could have escaped. Batson’s research confirmed the empathy hypothesis, which predicts
that empathic feelings
matter very much. Some people chose to help even when escape was easy,
indicating that it was their caring about the victim, not their own discomfort,
that drove their behavior (Figure 11.2). In a recent replication of Batson’s original experiment employing all female participants the same
pattern of results was found (Bierhoff & Rohmann, 2004). When escape was
easy empathic individuals were more likely to help than egoistic individuals.
No such difference emerged for the difficult escape condition. Other research
shows that it is the helper’s empathic
feelings for the person in need that are the prime motivators for helping
(Dovidio, Allen, & Schroeder, 1990).
In
a different test of the empathy-altruism hypothesis, Batson and Weeks (1996)
reasoned that if a person aroused to empathy tries to help a person in distress
and fails, there should be a substantial change in the helper’s state of mind to a negative mood. They reasoned further that less
negative mood change would result when little or no empathy was aroused. The
results of their experiment confirmed this. Participants in the high-empathy
condition experienced greater negative mood shifts after failed help than
participants in the low-empathy condition.
Figure
11.2.The relationship between the emotion experienced, ease of escape and
helping.
Based
on data from Batson, et al. (1988)
Interestingly,
empathy does not always lead to an increase in altruism. Batson and colleagues
(1999) demonstrated that both egoism and empathy can lead to reduced helping
or, what they called a “threat to the common good.” Batson and colleagues gave
participants the opportunity to divide resources among a group or keep them for
themselves (egoism). In one group-allocation condition, one of the group
members aroused the empathy of the participants. In a second group-allocation
condition, there was no group member who aroused empathy. In both group
conditions, participants could choose to allocate resources to the group as a
whole or to an individual member of the group. Batson and colleagues found that
when a participant’s allocation scheme was private, he or
she allocated fewer resources to the group than the self. This was true
regardless of whether the empathy-arousing victim was present. Conversely, when
allocation strategies were public, participants allocated fewer resources to
the group as a whole only when the empathy-arousing victim was present. The
research from Batson and colleagues suggests that both egoism and empathy can
threaten the common good. However, potential evaluation by others (the public
condition) strongly inhibits those motivated by egoism but not empathy.
Empathy appears to be a powerful emotion
that can lead to helping even when the altruistic individual has been treated
badly by another. In an imaginative experiment by Batson and Ahmad (2001),
female participants took part in a game involving an exchange of raffle
tickets. The participant was given three tickets worth +5, +5 and –5. The
participant was told that her partner in the game (there was no partner; the
partner’s behavior was determined by the experimenter) had the same tickets (+5,
+5, and –5). Batson and Ahmad aroused high empathy for the partner for some
participants and low empathy for others. On the first exchange the “partner”
gave the participant the –5 raffle ticket, meaning that the partner was in
effect trying to keep as many tickets as possible. The measure of altruism was
the number of participants who would give a +5 ticket to the partner. The
empathy-altruism hypothesis predicts that participants experiencing high
empathy for the partner should be willing to give the partner positive raffle
tickets, despite the defection by the partner. The results were consistent with
this prediction: 45% of the high-empathy participants gave the defecting
partner the +5 ticket, whereas only 10% of the low-empathy participants gave the
+5 card. Finally, empathy is an emotion that is not directed equally to all
individuals in need. Empathy has been found to be a stronger predictor of
helping when an in-group member needs help than if an out-group member needs
help (Sturmer, Snyder, & Omoto, 2005).
Challenging
the Empathy-Altruism Hypothesis
Everett
Sanderson was standing on a subway platform one day when a woman fell onto the
tracks. Sanderson leapt down onto the tracks and pulled the woman to safety
just moments before a train rushed into the station. When asked why he went to
a stranger’s aid, he replied that he would not have been
able to live with himself had he not helped.
Perhaps
people help because not helping would violate their view of themselves as moral
and altruistic and would make them feel guilty. Or, perhaps they are concerned
with what others may think if they do not help, and they would experience
shame. The notion that people may help because of the shame and guilt they will
feel if they do not help—known as the empathy-punishment hypothesis—presents a
challenge to the empathy-altruism hypothesis.
Batson
accepted the challenge of this hypothesis. He thought that people who help to
avoid guilt or shame should help less when provided with a good justification
for not helping. After all, if you can plausibly justify not helping to other
people (avoid shame) and to yourself (avoid guilt), then no punishment occurs.
If, however, your motive for helping is purely altruistic, then reduction of
the victim’s distress is the issue, not good rationalizations
for not helping.
Batson
and his colleagues (1988) designed research to pit the empathy-altruism
hypothesis against the empathy-punishment explanation. There were two variables
in this experiment: the subject’s level of empathy
for the victim (high or low) and the strength of the justification for not helping
(strong or weak). Subjects listened to a simulated news interview in which a
college senior (Katie) was interviewed about her parents’and sister’s recent deaths in an automobile accident and her current
role as sole supporter of her younger brother and sister. Empathy was
manipulated by instructing subjects either to pay attention to the “technical
aspects” of the news program (low empathy) or to “try to imagine how the person
who is being interviewed feels” (Batson et al., 1988, p. 61).
After
hearing the news program, the subjects read two letters left by the professor
in charge of the experiment. The first letter thanked the subjects for
participating and indicated that it occurred to him that some subjects might
want to help Katie. The second letter was from Katie herself, outlining ways
that the subjects could help her (e.g., babysitting, helping around the house,
helping with fundraising projects). Subjects indicated their willingness to
help on a response form that was used for the justification manipulation. The
response form had eight spaces for individuals to indicate whether they would
help Katie. In all cases, seven of the eight spaces were already filled in with
fictitious names. In the low justification for not helping condition, five of
the seven individuals on the list had agreed to help Katie. In the high
justification for not helping condition, only two of the seven agreed to help.
The
empathy-punishment explanation predicts that when there is a strong
justification for not helping, the amount of empathy aroused won’t matter. The empathy-altruism hypothesis predicts that empathic
motivation matters most when justification for not helping and empathy are
high. Only when people fail to empathize with the person in need does high
justification for not helping have an effect on helping. The results of the
research support the empathy-altruism hypothesis (Batson, 1990a; Batson et al.,
1988). If a person has empathic feelings and truly cares about the person in
need, rationalizations, however strong, do not stop him or her from helping.
Yet
another challenge to the empathy-altruism hypothesis comes from research by
Cialdini and his colleagues. Cialdini suggested that the data supporting the
empathy-altruism hypothesis can be reinterpreted with changes in one’s sense of self that occur in empathy situations. Cialdini
and colleagues argued that in addition to arousing empathic concern about a
person in distress, helping situations also arouse a greater sense of
self-other overlap. Specifically, the helper sees more of himself or herself in
the person in need (Cialdini, Brown, Lewis, Luce, & Neuberg, 1997). When
this occurs, the helper may engage in helping because of a greater sense of
closeness with the victim than with the arousal of empathic concern alone.
Cialdini
and colleagues (1997) conducted three experiments to test the self-other
oneness hypothesis. They found that when the self-other-oneness dimension was
considered along with empathy arousal, the relationship between empathy and
altruism was weakened substantially. Furthermore, they found that empathy
increases altruism only if it results in an increase in self-other oneness.
According to Cialdini and colleagues, empathic concern for a victim serves as
an emotional cue for the increase in self-other oneness. Additionally, as
suggested by Neuberg and colleagues, because empathy is an emotion, it may only
be important in deciding between not helping or providing minimal or
superficial help (Newberg, Cialdini, Brown, Luce, & Sagarin, 1997).
However,
the matter was not resolved, because Batson (1997) pointed out that the methods
used by Cialdini and colleagues were questionable. In fact, Batson and
colleagues (1997) found that when more careful procedures were used, there was
little evidence that self-other oneness was critical in mediating the
empathy-altruism link. As to whether empathy arousal leads only to superficial
helping, Batson pointed out that the empathy-altruism hypothesis only states
that empathy arousal is often associated with an altruistic act and does not
specify the depth of the act. Batson, however, does acknowledge that there may
be limits to the empathy-altruism relationship.
Where
do we stand currently on these hypotheses about helping? Although the research
of Batson and others supports the empathy-altruism hypothesis (Batson et al.,
1988; Dovidio et al., 1990), other research does not. For example, a strong
relationship has been found between feeling and giving help, a finding that
does not support the empathy-altruism hypothesis (Cialdini & Fultz, 1990).
If we give help when we feel sad, it seems more likely that we are helping to
relieve personal distress than out of pure altruism.
It
is apparent that the empathy-altruism hypothesis remains a point of controversy
in social psychology. Batson (1997) suggested that the controversy exists
mainly over whether there is enough clear evidence to justify acceptance of the
empathy-altruism hypothesis. There is agreement, according to Batson, that
empathy can be a factor in altruistic behavior. At this point, it is probably
best to adopt a position between the competing hypotheses. People may be
motivated by empathic altruism, but they seem to need to know that the victim
benefited from their help (Smith, Keating, & Stotland, 1989). This allows
them to experience empathic joy for helping the victim. Empathic joy simply
means that helpers feel good about the fact that their efforts helped someone
and that there was a positive outcome for that person. Helpers get a reward:
the knowledge that someone they helped benefited. Additionally, helping
situations may arouse a greater sense of closeness or oneness with the helper
and the victim. In any event, empathy does appear to be an important emotion
involved in altruism.
Biological
Explanations: Helping in Order to Preserve Our
Own
Genes
As
mentioned earlier, some psychologists have been skeptical about the existence
of purely altruistic behavior, because they believe behavior is shaped and
regulated by rewards and punishments. But there is another reason psychologists
have been skeptical about the existence of pure altruism, and that reason is
biological: People or animals that carry altruism involving personal danger to its
logical conclusion will sometimes die. Because self-preservation, or at least
the preservation of one’s genes (i.e., one’s children or
relatives), is a fundamental rule of evolutionary biology, pure altruism stands
on some shaky grounds (Wilson, 1978). Self-sacrificing behavior is very rare.
When it occurs, we reward it extravagantly. The Medal of Honor, for example, is
given for extraordinary bravery, behavior that goes beyond the call of duty.
Evolutionary
biologists find altruistic behavior fascinating, because it presents a
biological paradox: In light of the principle of survival of the fittest, how
can a behavior have evolved that puts the individual at risk and makes survival
less likely (Wilson, 1975)? The principle of natural selection favors selfish
behavior. Those animals that take care of themselves and do not expend energy
on helping others are more likely to survive and reproduce their genes. The
basic measure of biological fitness is the relative number of an individual’s offspring that survive and reproduce (Wilson, 1975).
The
evolutionary biologist’s answer to the paradox is to suggest
that there are no examples
of purely altruistic, totally selfless behavior in nature. Instead, there is
behavior that may have the effect of helping others but also serves some
selfish purpose. For example, consider the white-fronted bee eater, a bird
living in eastern and southern Africa (Goleman, 1991b). These birds live in
complex colonies consisting of 15 to 25 extended families. Family units consist
of about four overlapping generations. When breeding time arrives, some family
members do not breed. Instead, they serve as helpers who devote themselves to
constructing nests, feeding females, and defending the young. This helping is
called alloparenting, or cooperative breeding.
How
could such behavior have evolved? The bee eaters who do not breed lose the
opportunity to pass on their genes to offspring. However, their behavior does
help to ensure the survival of the whole colony and, specifically, the family
members with whom they share genes. This conclusion is supported by the fact
that the bee eater helpers provide cooperative help only to their closest
relatives. Birds that could have provided help but do not turn out to be
“in-laws”—birds that have no genetic connection with the mating pairs. Although
the helping behavior does not further the survival of the individual’s genes, it serves to preserve the individual’s gene pool.
Do
humans differ significantly from animals when it comes to altruism? According
to sociobiologists, human social behavior is governed by the same rules that
order all animal behavior. A central problem of sociobiology is to explain how
altruism can exist even though such behavior endangers individual fitness and
survival (Wilson, 1975, 1978). However, there is ample evidence that altruism
among humans flourishes and endures.
One
possible resolution to this apparent paradox lies in the idea that human
survival, dating to the beginnings of human society, depends on cooperation.
Human beings, smaller, slower, and weaker than many other animal species,
needed to form cooperative groups to survive. In such groups reciprocal
altruism may be more important than kin altruism. In reciprocal altruism, the
costs of behaving altruistically are weighed against the benefits. If there is
greater benefit than cost, an altruistic response will occur. Also, reciprocal
altruism involves a kind of tit-for-tat mentality: You help me, and I’ll help you.
Cooperation
and reciprocal altruism (helping one another) would have been selected for,
genetically, because they increase the survival of human beings (Hoffman,
1981). Unlike animals, humans do not restrict their helping to close genetic
relatives. Instead, humans can maintain the gene pool by helping those who
share common characteristics, even if they are not close kin (Glassman, Packel,
& Brown, 1986). Helping nonkin may help one preserve one’s distinguishing characteristics in the gene pool in a manner analogous to
helping kin.
Social
psychologists acknowledge that biology plays a role in altruistic behavior.
Altruism does not occur as often or as naturally as aggression, but it does
occur. However, social psychology also points out that altruistic behavior in
humans is determined by more than the biological dimension of our nature.
Helping
in Emergencies: A Five-Stage
Decision
Model
Irene
Opdyke’s decision to help the Jews in Ternopol
is an example of helping involving a long-term commitment to a course of
action. We refer to this as long-term helping. Opdyke’s
help involved a commitment that was extended over a period of months and required a
great investment of effort and resources. However, there are many other
situations that require quick action involving a short-term commitment to
helping. For example, if you saw a child fall into a pond, you probably would
rescue that child. We refer to this type of helping as situation-specific
helping.This helping, most likely in response to an emergency, does not require
a long-term investment of effort and resources.
Emergency
situations in which bystanders give help occur quite often. But there are also
many instances in which bystanders remain passive and do not intervene. This is
true even when a victim is in clear need of help. One such incident captured
the attention not only of the public but also of social psychologists: the
tragic death of Kitty Genovese on March 13, 1964.
Genovese,
a 24-year-old waitress, was coming home from work in Queens, New York, late one
night. As she walked to her apartment building, a man wielding a knife attacked
her. She screamed for help; 38 of her neighbors took notice from their
apartments. One yelled for the man to stop. The attacker ran off, only to
return when it was obvious that nobody was coming to her aid. He stabbed Genovese
repeatedly, eventually killing her. The attack lasted 40 minutes. When the
police were called, they responded within 2 minutes. More than 40 years later,
this tragedy continues to raise questions about why her neighbors did not
respond to her cries for help.
The
Genovese tragedy and similar incidents that occur all too frequently have
raised many questions among the public and among social scientists.
Dissatisfied with explanations that blamed life in the big city (“urban
apathy”), social psychologists Darley and LatanĂ© began to devise some
explanations about why the witnesses to Genovese’s
murder
did nothing to intervene. Darley and Latané sketched out a social psychological
model to explain the bystanders’behavior.
The
model proposed that there are five stages a bystander must pass through, each
representing an important decision, before he or she will help a person in need
(Latané, & Darley, 1968). In their original formulation of the model,
Latané and Darley (1968) suggested that a bystander must notice the situation,
label the situation correctly as an emergency, and assume responsibility for
helping. Darley and Latané proposed that there is a factor even beyond assuming
responsibility: The individual must decide how to help. Help, according to these
researchers, could take the form of direct intervention (Irene Opdyke’s behavior) or indirect intervention (calling the police). The
general model
proposed by Latané and Darley (1968; Darley & Latané, 1968), along with an
additional stage, is shown in Figure 11.3.
Figure 11.3 The fivestage model of helping. The path to
helping begins with noticing an emergency situation. Next, a potential helper
must label the situation correctly as an emergency and then assume
responsibility for helping. A negative decision at any point will lead to
nonhelping. Based on Darley and Latané (1968) and Latané and Darley (1968).
At
each stage of the model, the individual must assess the situation and make a
“yes” or “no” decision. At any point in the decision process, a “no” decision
will lead to failure to help. A “yes” decision itself does not guarantee
intervention; it simply allows the person to move to the next stage of the
model. According to the model, help will be given only if a “yes” decision is
made at each stage. Let’s consider each of the five stages.
Stage
1: Noticing the Situation
Before
we can expect a person to intervene in a situation, that person must have
noticed that an emergency exists. If, for example, an accident occurred 10
miles from where you are presently sitting, you would not be expected to help
because you are unaware of the accident. Before you can act, you must be aware
that something has occurred. For example, Kitty Genovese’s neighbors were aware of what was happening to Kitty. Noticing was not a
problem for the witnesses.
Noticing
is purely a sensory/perceptual phenomenon. If the emergency situation catches
our attention, we will notice the situation. As such, noticing involves the
basic laws of perception, such as the figure-ground relationship. This
fundamental relationship is manifested when a stimulus stands out against a
background. For example, when you go to a museum and look at a painting hanging
on the gallery wall, the painting is the figure and the gallery wall is the
background. We pay most attention to the figure (so when you tell a friend
about your trip to the museum, you will describe the painting and not the
gallery wall). In general, we are particularly likely to notice a stimulus that
is brightly colored, noisy, or somehow stands out against a background. This is
also true when noticing an emergency. Our chances of noticing an emergency
increase if it stands out against the background of everyday life. For example,
we are more likely to notice an automobile accident if there is a loud crash
than if there is little or no sound. Anything that makes the emergency more
conspicuous will increase the probability that we will attend to it.
Stage
2: Labeling the Situation as an Emergency
If
a person notices the situation, the next step is to correctly label it as one
that requires intervention. One very important factor at this stage is whether
there is ambiguity or uncertainty about what has happened. For example, imagine
that you look out the window of your second-floor apartment one day and notice
immediately below the window a car with its driver’s side door open and a person laying half in and half out of
the car. Has the
person collapsed, perhaps of a heart attack or a stroke? Or is the person
changing a fuse under the dashboard or fixing the radio? If you decide on the
latter explanation, you will turn away and not give it another thought. You
have made a “no” decision in the labeling stage of the model.
Recognizing
an emergency situation can be highly ambiguous because there is often more than
one interpretation for a situation. Is the woman upstairs beating her child or
merely disciplining her? Is the man staggering down the street sick or drunk?
Is that person slumped in the doorway injured or a drunken derelict? These
questions must be resolved if we are to correctly label a situation as an
emergency requiring our intervention.
When
two 10-year-old boys abducted a 2-year-old from a shopping center in Liverpool,
England, in 1993 and subsequently killed him, they walked together for 2 miles
along a busy road congested with traffic. Thirty-eight people remembered seeing
the three children, and some said later that the toddler was being dragged or
appeared to be crying. Apparently, the situation was ambiguous enough—were they
his older brothers, trying to get him home for dinner?—that no one stopped. A
driver of a dry-cleaning van said he saw one of the older boys aim a kick at
the toddler, but it looked like a “persuading” kind of kick such as one might
use on a 2-year-old (Morrison, 1994). The driver failed to label the situation
correctly.
The
Ambiguity of the Situation
Research
confirms that situational ambiguity is an important factor in whether people
help. In one study, subjects were seated in a room and asked to fill out a questionnaire
(Yakimovich & Salz, 1971). Outside the room, a confederate of the
experimenter was washing windows. When the experimenter signaled, the
confederate knocked over his ladder and pail, fell to the pavement, and grabbed
his ankle. In one condition (the verbalization condition), the confederate
screamed and cried for help. In the other condition (the no-verbalization
condition), the confederate moaned but didn’t cry
for help.
In
both conditions, subjects jumped up and went to the window when they heard the
sound of the crash. Therefore, all subjects noticed the emergency. In the
verbalization condition, 81% (13 of 16) tried to help the victim. In the
no-verbalization condition, however, only 29% (5 of 17) tried to help. The
clear cry for help, then, increased the probability that people would help.
Without it, it wasn’t clear that the man needed help.
Note
also that the potential helpers had all seen the victim before his accident. He
was a real person to them. Recall in the Genovese case that the witnesses had
not seen her before she was stabbed. Given this fact and that the murder took
place in the fog of the early morning hours, ambiguity must have existed, at
least for some witnesses.
The
Presence of Others
The
presence of other bystanders also may affect the labeling process. Reactions of
other bystanders often determine the response to the situation. If bystanders
show little concern over the emergency, individuals will be less likely to
help. When we are placed in a social situation (especially an ambiguous one),
we look around us to see what others are doing (the process of social
comparison). If others are not concerned, we may not define the situation as an
emergency, and we probably will not offer to help.
In
one study, increasing or decreasing the availability of cues from another
bystander affected helping (Darley, Teger, & Lewis, 1973). Subjects were
tested either alone or in groups of two. Those participating in groups were
either facing each other across a table (face-to-face condition) or seated
back-to-back (not-facing condition). An emergency was staged (a fall) while the
subjects worked on their tasks. More subjects who were alone helped (90%) than
subjects who were in groups. However, whether subjects were facing each other
made a big difference. Subjects who were facing each other were significantly
more likely to help (80%) than subjects not facing each other (20%). Consider
what happens when you sit across from someone and you both hear a cry for help.
You look at her, she looks at you. If she then goes back to her work, you
probably will not define the situation as an emergency. If she says, “Did you
hear that?” you are more likely to go investigate.
Generally,
we rely on cues from other bystanders more and more as the ambiguity of the
situation increases. Thus, in highly ambiguous emergency situations, we might
expect the presence of others who are passive to suppress helping. The fact
that the witnesses to Genovese’s murder were in
their separate apartments and did not know what others were doing and thinking
operated to suppress intervention.
Stage
3: Assuming Responsibility to Help: The Bystander Effect
Noticing
and correctly labeling a situation as an emergency are not enough to guarantee
that a bystander will intervene. It is certain that the 38 witnesses to
Genovese’s murder noticed, to one degree or another,
the incident and probably labeled it as an emergency. What they did not do is
conclude that they had a responsibility to help. Darley and Latané (1968),
puzzled by the lack of intervention on the part of the witnesses, thought that
the presence of others might inhibit rather than increase helping. They
designed a simple yet elegant experiment to test for the effects of multiple
bystanders on helping. Their experiment demonstrated the power of the bystander
effect, in which a person in need of help is less likely to receive help as the
number of bystanders increases.
Subjects
in this experiment were told it was a study of interpersonal communication.
They were asked to participate in a group discussion of their current problems.
To ensure anonymity, the discussion took place over intercoms. In reality,
there was no group. The experimenter played a tape of a discussion to lead the
subject to believe that other group members existed.
Darley
and Latané (1968) varied the size of the group. In one condition, the subject
was told that there was one other person in the group (so the group consisted
of the subject and the victim); in a second condition, there were two other
people (subject, victim, and four others). The discussion went along
uneventfully until it was the victim’s turn
to speak. The actor who played the role of the victim on the tape simulated a seizure. Darley
and Latané noted the number of subjects who tried to help and how long it took
them to try to help.
The
study produced two major findings. First, the size of the group had an effect
on the percentage of subjects helping. When the subject believed that he or she
was alone in the experiment with the victim, 85% of the subjects helped. The
percentage of subjects offering help declined when the subject believed there
was one other bystander (62%) or four other bystanders (31%). In other words,
as the number of bystanders increased, the likelihood of the subject helping the
victim decreased.
The
second major finding was that the size of the group had an effect on time
between the onset of the seizure and the offering of help. When the subject
believed he or she was alone, help occurred more quickly than when the subject
believed other bystanders were present. In essence, the subjects who believed
they were members of a larger group became “frozen in time” by the presence of
others. They had not decided to help or not to help. They were distressed but
could not act.
Interestingly,
the “other bystanders” need not be physically present in order for the
bystander effect to occur. In one experiment conducted by Garcia, Weaver,
Moskowitz, and Darley (2002), participants were asked to imagine that they had
won a dinner for either themselves and 30 friends, 10 friends, or just for
themselves (alone condition). Later participants were asked to indicate how
much money they would be willing to donate to charity after they graduated
college. Garcia et al. found that participants indicated the lowest level of
donations in the 30 friends condition, and the most in the alone condition (the
10-friends condition fell between these two groups). This effect extends to
computer chat rooms (Markey, 2000). Markey found that as the number of participants
in a chat room increased, the time it took to receive requested help also
increased. Interestingly, the chat room bystander effect was eliminated when
the person making the request personalized the request by singling someone out
by name.
Why
Does the Bystander Effect Occur?
The
best explanation offered for the bystander effect is diffusion of
responsibility (Darley & Latané, 1968). According to this explanation, each
bystander assumes that another bystander will take action. If all the
bystanders think that way, no help will be offered. This explanation fits quite
well with Darley and LatanĂ©’s findings in
which the bystanders
could not see each other, as was the case in the Genovese killing. Under these
conditions, it is easy to see how a bystander (unaware of how other bystanders
are acting) might assume that someone else has already taken or will take
action.
What
about emergency situations in which bystanders can see one another? In this
case, the bystanders could actually see that others were not helping. Diffusion
of responsibility under these conditions may not explain bystander inaction
(Latané & Darley, 1968). Another explanation has been offered for the
bystander effect that centers on pluralistic ignorance, which occurs when a
group of individuals acts in the same manner despite the fact that each person
has different perceptions of an event (Miller & McFarland, 1987). In the
bystander effect, pluralistic ignorance operates when the bystanders in an
ambiguous emergency situation look around and see each other doing nothing;
they assume that the others are thinking that the situation is not an emergency
(Miller & McFarland, 1987). In essence, the collective inaction of the
bystanders leads to a redefinition of the situation as a nonemergency.
Latané
and Darley (1968) provided evidence for this explanation. Subjects filled out a
questionnaire alone in a room, with two passive bystanders (confederates of the
experimenter) or with two other actual subjects. While the subjects were
filling out the questionnaire, smoke was introduced into the room through a
vent. The results showed that when subjects were alone in the room, 75% of the
subjects reported the smoke, many within 2 minutes of first noticing it. In the
condition in which the subject was in the room with two passive bystanders,
only 10% reported the smoke. In the last condition, in which the subject was
with two other subjects, 38% reported the smoke. Thus, the presence of
bystanders once again suppressed helping. This occurred despite the fact that
subjects in the bystander conditions denied that the other people in the room
had any effect on them.
In
post-experimental interviews, Latané and Darley (1968) searched for the
underlying cause for the observed results. They found that subjects who reported
the smoke felt that the smoke was unusual enough to report, although they didn’t feel that the smoke was dangerous. Subjects who failed to
report the smoke, which was most likely to occur in the two-bystander
condition, developed a set of creative reasons why the smoke should not be
reported. For example, some subjects believed that the smoke was smog piped
into the room to simulate an urban environment, or that the smoke was truth gas
designed to make them answer the questionnaire truthfully. Whatever reasons
these subjects came up with, the situation was redefined as a nonemergency.
Is
diffusion of responsibility, dependent on the number of bystanders present,
alwaysthe underlying cause for the bystander effect? Although diffusion of
responsibility is the most widely accepted explanation, it is not the only
explanation. Levine (1999) suggests that there are situations in which
diffusion of responsibility based on the presence of bystanders cannot explain
nonintervention. Instead, Levine suggests that if a bystander assumes that a
social category relationshipexists between parties in a potential helping
situation, intervention is unlikely. A social category relationship is one in
which bystanders assume that the parties involved belong together in some way.
For example, a spousal relationship would fit this definition because the two
individuals are seen as belonging together in the relationship. Levine argues
that when we are confronted with a situation in which a social category
relationship exists or is assumed, a social norm of nonintervention is
activated. In short, we are socialized to keep our noses out of family matters.
In fact, there is research that shows that bystanders are less willing to
intervene in an emergency situation when a social category relationship exists
(Shotland & Straw, 1976). Shotland and Straw, for example, found that 65%
of participants were willing to intervene in an argument between a male and
female who were strangers, but only 19% were willing to intervene when the male
and female were said to be married.
Levine
(1999) provides further evidence for this effect. He analyzed the trial
transcript of the trial of two 10-year-old boys who murdered a 2-year-old child
in London in 1993 (we briefly described this crime earlier in this chapter).
The two older boys, Jon Thompson and Robert Venables, abducted James Bulger and
walked Bulger around London for over 2 hours. During this time, the trio of
boys encountered 38 witnesses. Some witnesses were alone, whereas others were
with other bystanders. In a situation reminiscent of the Kitty Genovese murder,
none of the 38 witnesses intervened. Based on his analysis of the trial
transcript, Levine concluded that the nonintervention had little or nothing to
do with the number of bystanders present, or diffusion of responsibility.
Instead, statements of witnesses during trial testimony indicated that the
witnesses assumed (or were told by the older boys) that the older boys were
Bulger’s brothers taking him home. According to Levine, the assumption
that a social category relationship existed among the boys was the best
explanation for why the 38 witnesses did not intervene.
As
a final note, we need to understand that category relationships can extend
beyond social categories. We may assume that a relationship exists between
people and objects. For example, imagine you are going to your car after work
and see another car parked next to yours. You see that the hood is open and
there is someone tinkering with something under the hood. What would you think
is going on? Most likely you would assume that the person tinkering under the
hood owns the car and is fixing something. You would then be surprised to learn
the next day that the car was stolen and the man tinkering under the hood was a
thief! Assuming that such relationships exist can be a powerful suppressant to
intervention.
Limits
to the Bystander Effect
Increasing
the number of bystanders does not always suppress helping; there are exceptions
to the bystander effect. The bystander effect does not hold when intervention
is required in a potentially dangerous situation (Fischer, Greitemeir,
Pollozek, & Frey, 2006). In this experiment participants watched what they
believed was a live interaction between a male and female (actually the participants
viewed a prerecorded videotape). In the high-potential-danger condition, the
male was shown to be a large, “thug-like” individual who made progressively
more aggressive sexual advances toward the female, culminating in sexually
aggressive touching of the female and the female crying for help. At that point
the tape went blank. In the low-potential-danger condition, the male was shown
as a thin, short male who engaged in the same sexually aggressive behavior with
the same victim reactions. Half of the participants watched the interaction
alone (no bystander) and the other half watched it in the presence of a
confederate of the experimenter (bystander). The experimenters measured whether
the participant tried to help the female in distress. As shown in Figure 11.4,
the bystander effect was replicated in the low-danger situation: Fewer
participants attempt to help when a bystander is present than when the
participant is alone. In the high-danger situation, however, the bystander
effect was not evident.
Figure
11.4Bystanders who are alone are likely to help in high and low danger
situations. The presence of another bystander increased helping in the high
danger but not low danger situation; a clear reversal of the usual bystander
effect.
Based
on data from Fischer, et al. (2006).
In
another experiment, a reversal of the typical bystander effect was shown with a
potentially dangerous helping situation. One group of researchers staged a rape
on a college campus and measured how many subjects intervened (Harari, Harari,
& White, 1985). The subjects had three options in the experimental
situation: fleeing without helping, giving indirect help (alerting a police
officer who is out of view of the rape), or giving direct help (intervening
directly in the rape).
Male
subjects were tested as they walked either alone or in groups. (The groups in
this experiment were simply subjects who happened to be walking together and
not interacting with one another.) As the subjects approached a certain point,
two actors staged the rape. The woman screamed, “Help! Help! Please help me!
You bastard! Rape! Rape!” (Harari et al., 1985, p. 656). The results of this
experiment did not support the bystander effect. Subjects walking in groups
were more likely to help (85%) than subjects walking alone (65%). In this
situation—a victim is clearly in need and the helping situation is dangerous—it
seems that bystanders in groups are more likely to help than solitary
bystanders (Clark & Word, 1974; Harari et al., 1985).
The
bystander effect also seems to be influenced by the roles people take. In
another study, some subjects were assigned to be the leaders of a group
discussion and others to be assistants (Baumeister, Chesner, Senders, &
Tice, 1988). When a seizure was staged, subjects assigned the role of leader
were more likely to intervene (80%) than those assigned the role of assistant
(35%). It appears that the responsibility inherent in the leadership role on a
specific task generalizes to emergencies as well.
Finally,
the bystander effect is less likely to occur when the helping situation
confronting us involves a clear violation of a social norm that we personally
care about. Imagine, for example, you see a person throw an empty bottle into
the bushes at a public park. In such a situation you may engage in social
control behaviors (e.g., confront the offender, complain to your partner).
Contrast this with a situation where private property is involved (e.g.,
painting graffiti in an elevator in a building owned by a large corporation).
You may be less likely to engage in social control behaviors. Chekroun and
Brauer (2001) wondered if the bystander effect would operate differently in
these two situations. They hypothesized that the bystander effect would hold
for situations involving low personal implications (e.g., graffiti in the
elevator), but not in situations involving high personal implications (e.g.,
littering in a public park). In the low-personal-implication condition a
confederate of the experimenters entered an elevator in a shopping center
parking lot. As soon as the door closed, the confederate began scrawling
graffiti on the wall with a magic marker. This was done under two conditions: a
participant alone in the elevator with the confederate (no bystanders) or two
or three naĂŻve individuals in the elevator with the confederate. In the
high-personal-implications condition a confederate of the experimenters threw
an empty plastic bottle into some bushes in a public park in front of one
participant or a group of two or three participants. In both situations the
reaction of the participant(s) was (were) recorded on a scale ranging from no
social control to an audible negative comment. As you can see in Figure 11.5,
social control was most likely to occur when other bystanders were present in
the park-littering situation (high personal implications). Less social control
was shown by the groups of participants in the graffiti situation (low personal
implications).
Figure 11.5 Social control behaviors are more likely if
a behavior has high personal implications (littering in a public park) than if
the behavior has low personal implications (graffi ti in a privatelyowned
elevator). Based on data from Checkroun and Brauer (2002).
Stage
4: Deciding How to Help
The
fourth stage of the five-stage model of helping is deciding how to help. In the
staged rape study, for example, subjects had a choice of directly intervening
to stop the rape or aiding the victim by notifying the police (Harari, Harari,
& White, 1985). What influences decisions like this?
There
is considerable support for the notion that people who feel competent, who have
the necessary skills, are more likely to help than those who feel they lack
such competence. In a study in which subjects were exposed to a staged arterial
bleeding emergency, the likelihood of providing effective help was determined
only by the expertise of the subjects (some had Red Cross training; Shotland
& Heinhold, 1985).
There
are two reasons why greater competence may lead to more helping. First,
feelings of competence increase confidence in one’s
ability to help and to know what ought to be done (Cramer, McMaster, Bartell,
& Dragna, 1988). Second, feelings of competence increase sensitivity to the
needs of others and empathy toward victims (Barnett, Thompson, & Pfiefer,
1985). People who feel like leaders are probably also more likely to help
because they feel more confident about being able to help successfully.
Many
emergencies, however, do not require any special training or competence. Irene
Opdyke had no more competence in rescuing Jews than anyone else in Ternopol. In
the Genovese case, a simple telephone call to the police was all that was
needed. Clearly, no special competence was required.
Stage
5: Implementing the Decision to Help
Having
passed through these four stages, a person may still choose not to intervene.
To understand why, imagine that as you drive to campus, you see a fellow
student standing next to his obviously disabled car. Do you stop and offer to
help? Perhaps you are late for your next class and feel that you do not have
the time. Perhaps you are not sure it is safe to stop on the side of the
highway. Or perhaps the student strikes you as somehow undeserving of help
(Bickman & Kamzan, 1973). Or perhaps the place where the help is needed is
noisy (Moser, 1988). These and other considerations influence your decision
whether to help.
Assessing
Rewards and Costs for HelpingSocial psychologists have found that people’s evaluation of the rewards and costs involved in helping
affect their decision to help or not to help. There are potential rewards for
helping (gratitude from the victim, monetary reward, recognition by peers) and
for not helping (avoiding potential danger, arriving for an appointment on
time). Similarly, there are costs for helping (possible injury, embarrassment,
inconvenience) and for not helping (loss of self-esteem). Generally, research
indicates that the greater the cost of helping, the less likely people are to
help (Batson, O’Quin, Fultz, & Vanderplas, 1983; Darley & Batson,
1973; Piliavin & Piliavin, 1972; Piliavin, Piliavin, & Rodin, 1975).
In
a study of this relationship, Darley and Batson (1973) told seminarians taking
part in an experiment at Princeton University that a high school group was
visiting the campus and had requested a seminarian speaker. Half the subjects
were told they had little time to get across campus to speak to the high school
group, and the other half were told they had plenty of time. Additionally, some
subjects were asked to speak about the meaning of the parable of the Good
Samaritan. The seminarians then left the building to give their talk, and lo
and behold, while walking down a narrow lane, they saw a young man collapse in
front of them. What did they do?
Now,
do you recall the story of the Good Samaritan? A traveler is set on by robbers
and left by the side of the road. A priest and a Levite, people holding
important positions in the clergy of the time, walked by swiftly without
helping. But a Samaritan, passing along the same road, stopped and helped. We
might say that, for whatever reasons, helping was too costly for the priest and
the Levite but not too costly for the Samaritan.
What
about the seminarians? The “costly” condition in this experiment was the tight
schedule: Stopping to help would make them late for their talk. Was helping too
costly for them? Yes, it was. Subjects who were in a hurry, even if they were
thinking about the story of the Good Samaritan, were less likely to stop and
help than were subjects who were not in a hurry.
In
an attempt to “capture” the effects of various costs for helping and
nonhelping, Fritzsche, Finkelstein, and Penner (2000) had participants evaluate
scenarios containing three costs for helping (time required to help, the
discomfort involved in helping, and the urgency of the help) and three costs
for not helping (victim responsibility, ability to diffuse responsibility, and
victim deservingness). Participants read the scenarios in which these six
variables were manipulated and were instructed to play the role of the
individual receiving the request for help. For each scenario, the participant
indicated his or her likelihood of helping the person making the request for
help. Fritzsche et al. (2000) found confirmation for the effects of cost on
helping. In the scenarios where costs for helping were high, participants
expressed lower willingness to help. Fritzsche et al. evaluated the importance
of each of the six variables in determining willingness to give help. They
found that the cues varied in importance with respect to helping. There was no
significant gender difference in how the variables affected willingness to
help. The following list shows the importance of the six variables (in order
starting with the most important one):
1.
Victim responsibility
2. Urgency of the help
3. Time required for help
4. Diffusion of responsibility
5. Discomfort involved in helping
6. Victim’s
deservingness
As
is the case in decision-making research, there was a discrepancy between what
participants believed would be important in determining helping and what
actually turned out to be important. Participants believed that victim
deservingness, time required to render help, and ability to diffuse
responsibility would be the most important factors driving willingness to help.
However, as you can see from the previous list, only one of those factors was
near the top of the list (time required for help). Finally, there was a gender
difference in this finding. Males were more accurate than females in
identifying the importance of the variables.
The
Effect of Mood on Helping
Likelihood
of helping can even be affected by the bystander’s
mood. The research of Isen
(1987) and her coworkers has shown that adults and children who are in a
positive mood are more likely to help others than people who are not. People
who had found a dime in a phone booth in a shopping mall were more likely to
pick up papers dropped by a stranger than people who had not found a coin.
Students who had gotten free cookies in the library were more likely to
volunteer to help someone and were less likely to volunteer to annoy somebody
else when asked to do so as part of an experiment.
Although
positive mood is related to an increase in helping, it does not lead to more
helping if the person thinks that helping will destroy the good mood (Isen
& Simmonds, 1978). Good moods seem to generate good thoughts about people,
and this increases helping. People in good moods also are less concerned with
themselves and more likely to be sensitive to other people, making them more
aware of other people’s needs and therefore more
likely to help (Isen, 1987).
Music,
it is said, can soothe the wild beast. Can it also make you more likely to
help? North, Tarrent, and Hargreaves (2004) investigated this question.
Participants in a gym were exposed to either soothing or annoying music during
their workout periods. After the workout, participants were asked to help in a
low-cost (sign a petition) or high-cost (help distribute leaflets) situation.
North et al. found that when the soothing music had been played during the
workout, participants were more likely to help in the high-cost situation than
if the annoying music had been played. There was no difference between the two
types of music for the low-cost helping situation.
Gratitude
and Helping
Another
factor that can affect helping is whether an individual received help when he
or she needed help. Gratitude is an emotional state that has three functions
relating to prosocial behavior (McCullough, Kilpatrick, Emmons, & Larson,
2001). First, gratitude acts as a sort of “moral barometer,” indicating a
change in one’s state of mind after receiving help.
Second, gratitude can function as a “moral motivator,” impelling the recipient
of help to reciprocate to his or her benefactor or strangers. Third, gratitude
can serve as a “moral reinforcer.” When someone expresses gratitude after
receiving help, it increases the likelihood that the recipient of the gratitude
will engage in prosocial behavior in the future. Taken together, these three functions
suggest that gratitude will increase helping. But does it?
The
answer to this question is yes. A feeling of gratitude tends to enhance helping
(Bartlett & DeSteno, 2006; Tsang, 2006). In Bartlett and DeSteno’s experiment, participants were led to believe that they would
be performing a group task with another participant. Actually, the “other
participant” was a confederate of the experimenter. The real participant and
confederate performed tasks on separate computers. In the “gratitude”
condition, after completing a task and while waiting for scores to be
displayed, the confederate surreptitiously kicked the real participant’s monitor plug out of a power strip. The confederate then “helped”
the participant by finding and fixing the problem. In the “amusement”
condition, participants watched a brief, amusing video clip (to induce positive
affect unrelated to gratitude) after completing the task (the confederate did
not kick out the plug nor offer help). In the “neutral” condition the
confederate did not kick the plug out and only carried on a brief conversation
with the real participant. Sometime later the confederate approached the
participant and asked the participant to complete a long and tedious
problem-solving survey. As shown in Figure 11.6, Bartlett and DeSteno found
that participants were more willing to help in the gratitude condition than in
either the amusement or neutral conditions. Thus, it was the gratitude itself
and not just positive feelings that might be generated by receiving help that
increased helping. Bartlett and DeSteno conducted some follow-up studies to
determine if gratitude merely activates the norm of reciprocity (you should
help those who help you), thus leading to an increase in helping. Based on
their results, Bartlett and DeSteno concluded that it was, in fact, the feeling
of gratitude experienced by the real participants that increased helping, and
not the norm of reciprocity.
Figure
11.6Gratitude and not just positive emotions increase helping. Gratitude seems
to have special qualities that increase helping. Based on data from Bartlett
& DeSteno (2006)
Characteristics
of the Victim
A
decision to help (or not to help) also is affected by the victim’s characteristics. For example, males are more likely to help
females than to help other males (Eagly & Crowley, 1986; West, Whitney,
& Schnedler, 1975). Females, on the other hand, are equally likely to help
male and female victims (Early & Crowley, 1986). Physically attractive
people are more likely to receive help than unattractive people (Benson,
Karabenick, & Lerner, 1976). In one study, a pregnant woman, whether alone
or with another woman, received more help than a nonpregnant woman or a
facially disfigured woman (Walton et al., 1988).
Potential
helpers also make judgments about whether a victim deserves help. If we
perceive that a person got into a situation through his or her own negligence
and is therefore responsible for his or her own fate, we tend to generate
“just-world” thinking (Lerner & Simmons, 1966). According to the just-world
hypothesis, people get what they deserve and deserve what they get. This type
of thinking often leads us to devalue a person whom we think caused his or her
own misfortune (Lerner & Simmons, 1966). Generally, we give less help to victims
we perceive to have contributed to their own fate than to those we perceive as
needy through no fault of their own (Berkowitz, 1969; Schopler & Matthews,
1965).
However,
we may relax this exacting standard if we perceive that the person in need is
highly dependent on our help. In one experiment, subjects received telephone
calls at home in which the caller mistook them for the owner of “Ralph’s Garage” and told them that her car had broken down (Gruder,
Romer, & Korth, 1978). The caller says either that she meant to have the
car serviced but forgot (help needed due to victim’s negligence)
or that the car was just serviced (no negligence). In one condition, after the
subject informs the caller that she has not reached Ralph’s Garage, the caller says that she has no more change to make
another call (high dependency). In another condition, no mention is made of
being out of change. In all conditions the caller asks the subject to call
Ralph’s Garage for her. The researchers found
that subjects were more likely to help the negligent victim who had no more
change than the negligent victim who presumably had other ways to get help
(Figure 11.7). It seems that high dependence mediates just-world thinking.
Regardless of whether the victim deserves what she gets, we can’t help but take
pity on her.
Figure 11.7The effect of dependency and
victim fault on helping. In Gruder’s “Ralph’s Garage” experiment, participants
were more likely to help a victim high in dependency who was at fault for his
predicament. Based on data from Gruder, Romer, and Kroth (1974)
Just-world thinking also comes into play
when we consider the degree to which a victim contributed to his or her own
predicament. If you, as a helper, attribute a victim’s suffering to his or her
own actions (i.e., make an internal attribution), you will be less likely to
help than if you attribute the suffering to some external cause (Schmidt &
Weiner, 1988). When making judgments about individuals in need of help, we take
into account the degree to which the victim had control over his or her fate
(Schmidt & Weiner, 1988). For example, Greg Schmidt and Bernard Weiner
(1988) found that subjects expressed less willingness to help a student in need
of class notes if he needed the notes because he went to the beach instead of
class (a controllable situation) than if he had medically related vision
problems that prevented him from taking notes (uncontrollable situation).
Why do perceptions of controllability
matter? Schmidt and Weiner (1988) reported that the emotions aroused are
important factors in one’s reaction to a person in need. If a victim’s
situation arouses anger, as in the controllable situation, we are less likely
to give help than if the victim’s situation arouses sympathy (as in the
uncontrollable situation). Apparently, we are quite harsh when it comes to a
victim whom we perceive as having contributed to his or her own plight. We
reserve our sympathy for those victims who had little or no control over their
own fates.
In an interesting application of this effect,
Weiner and his colleagues (Graham, Weiner, Giuliano, & Williams, 1993;
Weiner, 1993; Weiner, Perry, & Magnusson, 1988) applied this analysis to
victims of various illnesses. Subjects tended to react with pity (and less
anger) toward victims of conditions over which the victims had little control
(Alzheimer’s disease, cancer). Conversely, subjects tended to react with anger
(and less pity) for victims of supposedly controllable conditions (AIDS,
obesity; Weiner, 1993; Weiner et al., 1988). The emotion tied to the victim’s
situation (pity versus anger) mediated willingness to help. Subjects indicated
less willingness to help victims with controllable problems than those with
uncontrollable problems (Weiner et al., 1988). Additionally, subjects assigned
greater responsibility to a person with a disease (AIDS) if the victim’s
behavior was perceived to have contributed to his or her disease than if the
victim’s behavior was not perceived to have contributed. For example, if a
person with AIDS contracted the disease via a blood transfusion, less
responsibility is assigned to the victim than if the person contracted the
disease via a sexual route (Graham et al., 1993).
Does this concept of the deserving versus
the nondeserving victim hold across cultures? In an interesting study conducted
by Mullen and Stitka (2000), U.S. and Ukranian participants were compared.
Participants read profiles about individuals who needed organ transplants. Half
the individuals were portrayed as having contributed to their own problems
(practicing poor health behaviors), whereas the other half were said to have
their condition because of a genetic disorder. Two other variables were
manipulated. One was the degree to which the individual needing the transplant
contributed to society (high or low), and the other was the degree of need for
the new organ (i.e., 95% versus 80% chance of dying if a transplant was not
performed). Mullen and Stitka found clear evidence for a cultural difference in
the variables that mediate helping. U.S. participants mainly based their
helping decisions on the degree to which an individual contributed to his or
her own problems. That is, less help is likely to be given to the person who
practiced poor health habits than to the person who suffers from a genetic disorder.
Ukranian participants, on the other hand, placed more weight on one’s
contributions to society than on the other factors. However, both American and
Ukranian participants were influenced by the other variables. U.S. participants
were influenced by contribution to society and need, in that order, following
personal responsibility. Ukranian participants also were influenced by personal
responsibility and need, in that order, after contributions to society.
There is evidence that characteristics of
the helper may interact with perceived controllability in determining affective
responses to victims and helping behavior. In an analysis of reactions to
individuals living in poverty, Zucker and Weiner (1993) found that politically
conservative individuals were likely to blame the victim for being in poverty,
attributing poverty to characteristics of the victim. Consequently, these
individuals tend to react with anger and are less willing to help. On the other
hand, more liberal individuals see poverty as driven by societal forces, not
under control of the victim, and react with pity and are more willing to help.
Finally, social categorization also
affects one’s decision to help (Levine & Thompson, 2004; Levine, Cassidy,
Brazier, & Reicher, 2002; Levine, Prosser, Evans, & Reicher, 2005;
Sturmer, Snyder, & Omoto, 2005). That is, we are more likely to help
someone in need who is from our “in-group” as opposed to someone from an
“out-group.” In one study that demonstrated this effect, Levine and Thompson
(2004) had participants read two scenarios depicting natural disasters (a flood
and an earthquake). The scenarios depicted disasters of equal severity and
elicited similar helping responses. Each disaster was said to have occurred
either in Europe or South America. Participants were British students enrolled
at Lancaster University in England. Levine and Thompson manipulated the “social
identity” of the participants. Some participants were induced into adopting a
“British social identity” and others a more general “European social identity.”
After reading the scenarios, participants were asked the extent to which they
would be willing to help the victims of the natural disasters.
Consistent with the notion that we are
more likely to help members of an in-group, participants who were induced into
a European social identity expressed a greater willingness to help European
victims of either disaster than those who adopted the British social identity.
Less help was extended to victims of a South American disaster, regardless of
the identity induced. Thus, members of an out-group were least likely to be
helped. In another experiment Levine et al. (2005) found that soccer fans were
more likely to help someone in need who was wearing their team’s jersey than
someone wearing a rival team’s jersey.
Race and Helping Behavior
Another characteristic of the victim
investigated by social psychologists is race. Are blacks more or less likely
than whites to receive help when they need it? If you base your answer on
stories on television and in the newspapers, you might think that blacks and
whites in our society never help each other. But this is simply not true. Many
blacks risked their lives to save whites during the Los Angeles riots in 1992.
A group of African American residents of South Central Los Angeles helped get
Reginald Denny to the hospital, saving his life. Interracial helping does
occur. What does the social psychological research say about this issue?
A meta-analysis of the literature in this
area (Saucier, Miller, & Doucet, 2005) found that race and helping present
a rather complex picture. According to Saucier et al., the meta-analysis did
not show any overall, universal bias against black victims in need of help.
Black and white victims, given the same helping situation, are equally likely
to receive help. However, racial bias did emerge when specific variables were
examined. Most specifically, variables relating to aversive racism (see Chapter
4) did show bias. Saucier et al. found that blacks are less likely to receive
help than whites under the following conditions:
1.
When the help required longer commitments of time
2.
When the help was more risky
3.
When the help was more difficult
4.
When the distance between the helper and victim increased
5.
When a white helper could rationalize away nonhelp
In terms of specific studies, there have
been numerous studies conducted to investigate aspects of interracial helping
(Benson, Karabenick, & Lerner, 1976; Dovidio & Gaertner, 1981;
Gaertner, Dovidio, & Johnson, 1982). In one, for example, white subjects,
assessed as either high or low in prejudice, were given an opportunity to help
either a black or a white victim (Gaertner et al., 1982). The subjects were
either alone (subject and victim) or with four others (three bystanders and the
victim). The researchers recorded the amount of time subjects took to give the
victim aid. Their results showed that white victims were helped more quickly
than black victims, especially by prejudiced subjects, when bystanders were
present. Blacks and whites were helped equally quickly when no bystanders were
present. Thus, the bystander effect is stronger for black than for white
victims (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1977; Gaertner et al., 1982).
Given the opportunity to diffuse
responsibility, bystanders will avail themselves of the opportunity more with
black than with white victims (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1977) This may occur
because when multiple bystanders are present, a black victim is seen as less
severely injured than a white victim (Gaertner, 1975). When there is a single
bystander, there is no such differential assessment of injury severity
(Gaertner, 1975).
Other factors also influence the help
given to black versus white victims. In another study, white subjects were
given an opportunity to help either a black or white male (Dovidio &
Gaertner, 1981). This person was introduced as the subject’s “supervisor” or
“subordinate” and was said to be of either higher or lower cognitive ability
than the subject. When given an opportunity to help, white subjects helped the
black subordinate (lower status) more than the black supervisor (higher
status), regardless of the ability level. However, African American subjects
gave help based more on ability than on status. According to this study, status
is relevant in whites’decision to help blacks, with more help given to
lower-status blacks (Dovidio & Gaertner, 1981). Ability is more relevant in
blacks’decision to help whites, with more help given to high-ability than
low-ability whites.
The relationship between race and helping
behavior is complex and involves numerous situational factors as well as racial
attitudes. A review of the literature by Crosby, Bromley, and Saxe (1980) found
mixed results. These researchers drew three conclusions:
1.
Bias exists against African American victims, but the bias is not
extreme. Clear discrimination against African American victims was reported in
44% of the studies reviewed; 56% showed no discrimination or reverse
discrimination.
2.
Whites and blacks discriminate against the opposite race at about the
same level.
3.
Whites discriminate against black victims more under remote conditions
(over the telephone) than in face-to-face situations.
In another study, researchers investigated
race differences in the level of help given to elderly individuals who lived at
home (Morrow-Howell, Lott, & Ozawa, 1990). They analyzed a program in which
volunteers were assigned to help elderly clients shop and provide them with
transportation, counseling, and telephone social support. This study found very
few differences between black and white volunteers. For example, both black and
white volunteers attended training sessions at equal rates and were evaluated
equally by their supervisors.
There was, however, one interesting
difference between black and white volunteers when the race of the client was
considered. According to client reports, volunteers who were of a different
race than the client spent less time with clients than did volunteers of the
same race. Additionally, when the volunteer and client were of the same race,
the client reported that there were more home visits and that the volunteer was
more helpful than if the volunteer and client differed in race.
A few cautions are in order here, however.
There was no independent measure of the amount of time volunteers spent with
clients or the quality of service rendered. The data on the volunteers’performance
were based on client reports. It could be that same-race clients were simply
more inclined to rate their volunteers positively than were different-race
clients. Nevertheless, the study documented a program of helping in which
altruistic tendencies transcended racial barriers.
Sexual Orientation and Helping
The sexual orientation of a person in need
influences willingness to help (Gore, Tobiasen, & Kayson, 1997; Shaw,
Bourough, & Fink, 1994). For example, Gore and colleagues (1997) had either
a male or female victim make a telephone call to participants. When the
participant answered, the victim made it clear that he or she had dialed the
wrong number. Implied sexual orientation was manipulated by having the victim
tell the participant that he or she was trying to reach his or her boyfriend or
girlfriend. They also told the participant that they had either used their last
quarter (high urgency) or had no more change (low urgency). Participants were
asked to call a number to report the emergency (which was actually the
experimenter’s number). The proportion of participants who returned the victim’s
call to the experimenter within 60 seconds was the measure of helping. The
results showed that heterosexuals were more likely to get help (80%) than
homosexuals (48%). Additionally, even when homosexuals were helped, it took
longer for the participants to call back than when the victim was heterosexual.
Increasing the Chances of Receiving Help
We have been looking at helping behavior
from the point of view of the potential helper. But what about the person in
need of help? Is there anything a victim can do to increase the chances of
being helped? Given all the obstacles along the path of helping, it may seem a
small miracle that anyone ever receives any help. If you are in a position of
needing help, however, there are some things you can do.
First, make your plea for help as loud as
possible. Yelling and waving your arms increase the likelihood that others will
notice your plight. Make your plea as clear as possible. You do not want to
leave any room for doubt that you need help. This will help bystanders
correctly label the situation as an emergency.
Next, you want to increase the chances
that a bystander will assume responsibility for helping you. Don’t count on
this happening by itself. Anything you can do to increase a bystander’s
personal responsibility for helping will increase your chances of getting help.
Making eye contact is one way to do this; making a direct request is another.
The effectiveness of the direct-request
approach was graphically illustrated in a field experiment in which a
confederate of the experimenter approached subjects on a beach (Moriarty,
1975). In one condition, the confederate asked the subject to watch his things
(a blanket and a radio) while the confederate went to the boardwalk for a
minute (the subject is given responsibility for helping). In another condition,
the confederate simply asked the subject for a match (social contact, but no
responsibility). A short time after the confederate left, a second confederate
came along and took the radio and ran off. More subjects helped in the
personal-responsibility condition (some actually ran the second confederate
down) than in the nonresponsibility condition. Thus, making someone personally
responsible for helping increases helping.
Courageous Resistance and Heroism
A vast majority of research on altruism in
social psychology has focused on helping in emergency situations. Typically,
this type of help requires an immediate decision to a specific situation.
However, not all helping falls into this category. There are helping situations
that may involve nonemergencies (e.g., volunteering in a hospital) and may
require a more deliberative decision than is required in an emergency
situation. For example, if you are trying to decide whether to volunteer your
time for a certain cause, you may take time to consider all aspects of your
decision. One category of such helping is called courageous resistance(Shepela
et al., 1999). According to Shepela et al., courageous resistance is “selfless
behavior in which there is a high risk/cost to the actor, and possibly to the
actor’s family or associates, where the behavior must be sustained over time,
is most often deliberative, and often where the actor is responding to a moral
calling” (p. 789).
Courageous resistors can be found in a
wide range of situations. For example, William Lawless was put in charge of
waste disposal at the Savannah River reactor, even though he had little
experience in radioactive waste disposal. He became aware that liquid
radioactive wastes were being dumped into shallow trenches. When he started
asking questions, he was told to keep quiet about it. Instead, Lawless went
public, and as a result, massive cleanup efforts were undertaken to remove
radioactive waste disposed of improperly. From the political world is Nelson
Mandela, founder of the African National Congress in South Africa. Mandela took
a stand against apartheid (the system in South Africa that separated whites and
blacks socially, economically, and linguistically). For his efforts he spent 28
years in prison. Eventually, he was released and went on to become the leader
of that country.
Sometimes individuals arise as courageous
resistors that surprise us. Two examples are John Rabe and Albert Goering. Rabe
was a Nazi businessman in Nanking, China. After the Japanese invaded Nanking
and began murdering Chinese civilians, Rabe used his Nazi credentials and
connections to save nearly 250,000 Chinese by protecting them in a German
compound, often facing down armed Japanese soldiers only with his Nazi
credentials. Albert Goering, the half-brother of Hermann Goering (the second
highest official in Nazi Germany), is credited with saving hundreds of
persecuted Jews during World War II. He would forge his brother’s name on
transit documents and use his brother’s influence if he got caught. Despite
having grown up in the same house as his brother Hermann, Albert emerged as a
much different person, dedicated to helping persecuted Jews escape those his
brother sent to persecute them.
A concept closely related to courageous
resistance is heroism. Heroismis any helping act that involves significant risk
above what is normally expected and serves some socially valued goal (Becker
& Eagly, 2004). The two elements of this definition require some
elaboration. There are many jobs that require considerable risk such as police
officer and firefighter. We expect individuals in these roles to accept a
degree of risk. So, for example, we expect a firefighter to enter a burning
building to save victims. Such behavior is not necessarily heroic because it is
expected of firefighters. However, if a firefighter goes back several times
into a building on the verge of collapse to rescue victims, that would qualify
as heroic. The second requirement of a heroic act is that it serves some valued
goal. Saving lives is certainly a valued goal, as is putting one’s job on the
line to expose a wrong.
As you can see, heroism and courageous
resistance have common elements. They have one important difference: A heroic
act need not involve an extended commitment. A heroic act can be a one-shot occurrence
involving a quick decision made on the spot. For example, Rick Rescorla (head
of security for a firm at the World Trade Center), who reentered the World
Trade Center to help stragglers get out and died when one of the towers
collapsed, would be considered heroic. His behavior clearly involved risk and
served the higher goal. It did not, however, involve the deliberative process
over time and the long-term commitment to a course of action. So, one can be
heroic without being a courageous resistor.
Finally, a heroic act need not always be
motivated by empathy for a victim or altruism. There can be a number of motives
for a heroic act. For example, a firefighter might act in a heroic way to gain
recognition and secure a promotion. His or her egoistic motivations do not
diminish the heroic nature of any act he or she performs.
In this section of the chapter we shall
focus on one particular example of courageous resistance and heroism: Ordinary
people who, under extraordinary circumstances, helped rescue Jews from the
Nazis during World War II. You should keep in mind that what these individuals
did was exceedingly dangerous. Anyone caught helping Jews was dealt with
harshly, including being sent to death camps or summarily hanged. Because of
prevailing anti-Jewish attitudes and the threat of punishment, engaging in
rescue activity was relatively rare, especially in Eastern Europe. However,
there were those who risked their lives to help others, in some cases for
years.
Before we begin our discussion of rescuers,
it is important to note that the relationship between altruism and courageous
resistance may, at times, be tenuous. Not all altruistic individuals are
courageous. For example, undoubtedly there were many Christians who deplored
what the Nazis were doing to Jews and felt empathy for the Jews. However,
because of fear of being caught and executed, many of these individuals did not
translate their empathic concern into tangible action to help. Likewise, not
all courageous people are altruistic. For example, Tec (1986) reports that some
people who helped the Jews were “paid helpers” who helped Jews primarily for
the money. These individuals were not motivated by empathy or altruism. As a
result, the quality of care received by Jews helped by paid helpers was far
lower than those helped by rescuers (Tec, 1986).
Explaining Courageous Resistance and
Heroism: The Role
of Personality
Much of the research on helping behavior
that we have discussed suggests that whether people help depends on situational
factors. For example, research shows that the costs of helping, the degree of
responsibility for helping, the assumed characteristics of the victim, and the
dangerousness of the situation all affect helping behavior. None of these
factors are under the control of the potential helper; they are part of the
situation.
Situational factors seem to be crucial in
situations that require spontaneous helping (Clary & Orenstein, 1991). The
situations created in the laboratory, or for that matter in the field, are
analogous to looking at a single frame in a motion picture. Recall the
seminarians. They were in a hurry, and although thinking of the parable of the
Good Samaritan, they practically leapt over the slumped body of a person in
need of their help. Is this unexpected event a fair and representative sample
of their behavior? It was for that particular situation. But, unless we look at
what comes before and after, we cannot make judgments about how they would
behave in other situations. Looking at these single-frame glimpses of helping
can lead us to overlook personality variables.
Although personality factors come into
play in all forms of altruism, they may be more likely to come to the fore in
long-term helping situations. Helping on a long-term basis, whether it involves
volunteering at a hospital or Albert Goering helping Jews, requires a degree of
planning. This planning might take place before the help begins. Or it may
occur after help begins. For example, rescuers of Jews in Nazi-occupied Europe
often did not plan their initial helping acts (Tec, 1986). However, their
continued helping required thought and planning. During planning, helpers
assess risks, costs, and priorities, and they match personal morals and
abilities with victims’needs. History teaches us that in times of great need, a
select few individuals emerge to offer long-term help. What is it about these
people that sets them apart from others who remain on the sidelines? Midlarsky,
Fagin Jones, and Corley (2005) compared rescuers and nonrescuers on a number of
personality dimensions. They found that the rescuers possessed a cluster of
personality characteristics that distinguished them from nonrescuers. These
characteristics were: “locus of control, autonomy, risk taking, social
responsibility, empathic, concern, and altruistic moral reasoning” (p. 918).
Rescuers, compared to nonrescuers, were more internally motivated, were more
independent, were more likely to take risks, showed higher levels of social
responsibility, had more empathic concern for others, and were more likely to
be driven by internal moral/altruistic values. Further, they found that
altruistic moral reasoning was the strongest correlate of rescue activity. So,
there is evidence for an altruistic personality, or a cluster of personality
traits, including empathy, that predisposes them to great acts of altruism.
However, we also must remain mindful that situational forces still may be
important, even in long-term helping situations. In the sections that follow,
we explore how situational factors and personality factors combine to influence
altruism. We begin by considering the factors that influenced a relatively
small number of individuals to help rescue Jews from the Nazis during their
World War II occupation of Europe.
Righteous Rescuers in Nazi-Occupied Europe
As Hitler’s final solution (the systematic
extermination of European Jews) progressed, life for Jews in Europe became
harder and more dangerous. Although most of Eastern Europe’s and many of
Western Europe’s Jews were murdered, some did survive. Some survived on their
own by passing as Christians or leaving their homes ahead of the Nazis. Many,
however, survived with the help of non-Jews who risked their lives to help
them. The state of Israel recognizes a select group of those who helped Jews
for their heroism and designates them as righteous rescuers (Tec, 1986).
Sadly, not as many individuals emerged as
rescuers as one might wish. The number of rescuers is estimated to have been
between 50,000 and 500,000, a small percentage of those living under Nazi rule
(Oliner & Oliner, 1988). In short, only a minority of people were willing
to risk their lives to help others.
It should not be too surprising that the
majority did not help the Jews. Those caught helping Jews, even in the smallest
way, were subjected to punishment, death in an extermination camp, or summary
execution. In other cases, especially in Poland, rescuing Jews amounted to
flying in the face of centuries of anti-Semitic attitudes and religious
doctrine that identified Jews as the killers of Jesus Christ (Oliner &
Oliner, 1988; Tec, 1986). The special problems facing Polish rescuers are
illustrated in the following quotation from one: “My husband hated Jews. . . .
Anti-Semitism was ingrained in him. Not only was he willing to burn every Jew
but even the earth on which they stood. Many Poles feel the way he did. I had
to be careful of the Poles” (Tec, 1986, p. 54).
Because Polish rescuers violated such
powerful social norms, some social psychologists have suggested that their
behavior is an example of autonomous altruism, selfless help that society does
not reinforce (Tec, 1986). In fact, such altruism may be discouraged by
society. Rescuers in countries outside Poland may have been operating from a
different motive. Most rescuers in Western Europe, although acting out of
empathy for the Jews, may have had a normocentric motivation for their first
act of helping (Oliner & Oliner, 1988). A normocentric motivation for
helping is oriented more toward a group (perhaps society) with whom an
individual identifies than toward the individual in need. In small towns in
southern France, for example, rescuing Jews became normative, the accepted and
expected thing to do. This type of altruism is known as normative altruism,
altruism that society supports and encourages (Tec, 1986).
Finally, it is important to understand
that not only were general attitudes throughout Europe related to the frequency
and type of rescue activity, but so were specific cultural and social forces
within specific regions of Europe. For example, Buckser (2001) points out that
the large-scale rescue of Danish Jews is best understood within the cultural
context of Denmark and its relationship to its Jewish population. Buckser
points out that in many areas the Danish population did not resist German
occupation. However, when it came to the Jewish population, Danes came together
to save all but a few Danish Jews. Buckser believes that Danes rose up to help
the Jews because of Grundtvigian Nationalism, which essentially placed Danish
national and cultural identity above differences among people. In Denmark, Jews
had successfully assimilated into the larger Danish culture. So, when the
Germans invaded and tried to portray the Jews as threatening outsiders, it didn’t
work well. Instead, the German characterization of the Jews activated the
unique Danish Nationalism, and Danes who otherwise acquiesced to the Germans
actively took part in the large-scale evacuation of Danish Jews to Sweden.
The Oliners and the Altruistic Personality
Project
One family victimized by the Nazis in
Poland was that of Samuel Oliner. One day in 1942, when Samuel was 12 years old
and living in the village of Bobawa, he was roused by the sound of soldiers’boots
cracking the predawn silence. He escaped to the roof and hid there in his
pajamas until they left. When he dared to come down from his rooftop perch, the
Jews of Bobawa lay buried in a mass grave. The village was empty.
Two years earlier, Samuel’s entire family
had been killed by the Nazis. Now he gathered some clothes and walked for 48
hours until he reached the farm of Balwina Piecuch, a peasant woman who had
been friendly to his family in the past. The 12-year-old orphan knocked at her
door. When Piecuch saw Samuel, she gathered him into her house. There she
harbored him against the Nazis, teaching him what he needed to know of the
Christian religion to pass as a Polish stable boy.
Oliner survived the war, immigrated to the
United States, and went on to teach at Humboldt State University in Arcata,
California. One of his courses was on the Holocaust. In it, he examined the
fate of the millions of Jews, Gypsies, and other Europeans who were
systematically murdered by the Nazis between 1939 and 1945. In 1978, one of his
students, a German woman, became distraught, saying she couldn’t bear the guilt
over what her people had done.
At this point, Oliner realized that the
history of the war, a story of murder, mayhem, and sadism, had left out a small
but important aspect: the accomplishments of the many altruistic people who
acted to help Jews and did so without expectation of external rewards (Goldman,
1988; Oliner & Oliner, 1988). Oliner and his wife, Pearl, established the
Altruistic Personality Project to study the character and motivations of those altruists,
whom the Oliners rightly call heroes.
Situational Factors Involved in Becoming a
Rescuer
Oliner and Oliner (1988) and Tec (1986)
investigated the situational forces that influence individuals to become
rescuers. These situational factors can be captured in the five questions for
which the Oliners wanted to find answers:
1.
Did rescuers know more about the difficulties the Jews faced than
nonrescuers?
2.
Were rescuers better off financially and therefore better able to help?
3.
Did rescuers have social support for their efforts?
4.
Did rescuers adequately evaluate the risks, the costs of helping?
5.
Were rescuers asked to help, or did they initiate helping on their own?
The Oliners interviewed rescuers and a
matched sample of nonrescuers over the course of a 5-year study and compared
the two groups. The Oliners used a 66-page questionnaire, translated into
Polish, German, French, Dutch, Italian, and Norwegian and used 28 bilingual
interviews. Results indicate that the situational differences between
rescuers and nonrescuers were not as
significant as expected. For example, rescuers were not wealthier than
nonrescuers. Tec (1986) reported that the greatest number of Polish helpers
came from the peasant class, not the upper class of Poles. Additionally,
rescuers and nonrescuers alike knew about the persecution of the Jews and knew
the risks involved in going to their aid (Oliner & Oliner, 1988).
Only two situational variables were
relevant to the decision to rescue. First, family support was important for the
rescue effort (Tec, 1986). Sixty percent of the rescuers in Tec’s sample
reported that their families supported the rescue effort, compared to only 12%
who said that their families opposed rescue efforts, a finding mirrored in
Oliner and Oliner’s study. Evidence suggests that rescue was made more likely
by the rescuers’being affiliated with a group that supported the rescue effort
(Baron, 1986). We can conclude that support from some outside agency, be it the
family or another support group, made rescue more likely.
The second situational factor was how the
rescuer first began his or her efforts. In most cases (68%), rescuers helped in
response to a specific request to help; only 32% initiated help on their own
(Oliner & Oliner, 1988). Tec reported a similar result. For most rescuers
the first act of help was unplanned. But once a rescuer agreed to help that
first time, he or she was likely to help again. Help was refused in a minority
of instances (about 15%), but such refusal was related to specific risks
involved in giving help. Most rescuers (61%) helped for 6 months or more (Tec,
1986). And 90% of the people rescuers helped were strangers (Goldman, 1988).
These situational factors—the costs of
helping, a request for help, and the support of other bystanders in a group of
which the rescuer was a member—also have been identified in research as
important in influencing the decision to help.
Personality Factors Involved in Becoming a
Rescuer
The results of the work by Oliner and
Oliner (1988) suggest that rescuers and nonrescuers differed from each other
less by circumstances than by their upbringing and personalities. The Oliners
found that rescuers exhibited a strong feeling of personal responsibility for
the welfare of other people and a compelling need to act on that felt
responsibility. They were moved by the pain of the innocent victims, by their
sadness, helplessness, and desperation. Empathy for the victim was an important
factor driving this form of altruism. Interestingly, rescuers and nonrescuers
did not differ significantly on general measures of empathy. However, they did
differ on a particular type of empathy called emotional empathy, which centers
on one’s sensitivity to the pain and suffering of others (Oliner & Oliner,
1988). According to the Oliners, this empathy, coupled with a sense of social
responsibility, increased the likelihood that an individual would make and keep
a commitment to help.
Beyond empathy, rescuers shared several
other characteristics (Tec, 1986). First, they showed an inability to blend in
with others in the environment. That is, they tended to be socially marginal,
not fitting in very well with others. Second, rescuers exhibited a high level
of independence and self-reliance. They were likely to pursue their personal
goals even if those goals conflicted with social norms. Third, rescuers had an
enduring commitment to helping those in need long before the war began. The war
did not make these people altruists; rather, it allowed these individuals to
remain altruists in a new situation.
Fourth, rescuers had (and still have) a
matter-of-fact attitude about their rescue efforts. During and after the war,
rescuers denied that they were heroes, instead saying that they did the only
thing they could do. Finally, rescuers had a universalistic view of the needy.
That is, rescuers were able to put aside the religion or other characteris-tics
of those they helped. Interestingly, some rescuers harbored anti-Semitic
attitudes (Tec, 1986). But they were able to put those prejudices aside and
help a person in need. These characteristics, along with high levels of
empathy, contributed to the rescuers’ decision to help the Jews.
The research on rescuers clearly shows
that they differed in significant ways from those who were nonrescuers (Oliner
& Oliner, 1988) or paid helpers (Tec, 1986). How can we account for these
differences? To answer this question, we must look at the family environments
in which rescuers were socialized.
Altruism as a Function of Childrearing
Style
In Chapter 10, we established that inept
parenting contributes to the development of antisocial behaviors such as
aggression. Oliner and Oliner (1988) found that the childrearing styles used by
parents of rescuers contributed to the development of prosocial attitudes and
behaviors. The techniques used by parents of rescuers fostered empathy in the
rescuers.
Research shows that a parental or adult
model who behaves altruistically is more likely to influence children to help
than are verbal exhortations to be generous (Bryan & Walbek, 1970).
Additionally, verbal reinforcement has a different effect on children’s
helping, depending on whether a model behaves in a charitable or selfish manner
(Midlarsky, Bryan, & Brickman, 1973). Ver bal social approval from a
selfish model does not increase children’s donations. However, social approval
from a charitable model does.
Models obviously have a powerful effect on
both aggressive and prosocial behaviors. Why, however, do you think that a
prosocial model has more effect on younger children than older children? What
factors can you think of to explain the fact that a model’s behavior is more
important than what the model says? Based on what you know about the effect of
prosocial models on children’s altruism, if you were given the opportunity to
design a television character to communicate prosocial ideals, what would that
character be like? What would the character say and do to foster prosocial
behavior in children? Similarly, what types of models should we be exposing
adults to in order to increase helping? Parents of rescuers provided role
models for their children that allowed them to develop the positive qualities
needed to become rescuers later in life. For example, rescuers (more than
nonrescuers) came from families that stressed the universal similarity of all
people, despite superficial differences among them (Oliner & Oliner, 1988).
Families stressed the aspect of religion that encouraged caring for those in
need. Additionally, families of rescuers did not discuss negative stereotypes
of Jews, which was more common among families of nonrescuers. As children,
then, rescuers were exposed to role models that instilled in them many positive
qualities.
It is not enough for parents simply to
embrace altruistic values and provide positive role models (Staub, 1985); they
must also exert firm control over their children.
Parents who raise altruistic children
coach them to be helpful and firmly teach them how to be helpful (Goleman,
1991a; Stab, 1985). Parents who are warm and nurturing and use reasoning with
the child as a discipline technique are more likely to produce an altruistic
child than cold, uncaring, punitive parents (Eisenberg & Mussen, 1989).
This was certainly true of families of rescuers. Parents of rescuers tended to
avoid using physical punishment, using an inductive style that focused on
verbal reasoning and explanation.
As important as the family is in the
socialization of altruism, it cannot alone account for a child growing up to be
an altruistic individual. Recall that Albert and Hermann Goering grew up in the
same household yet went down very different paths in adulthood. The child’s
cognitive development, or his or her capacity to understand the world, also
plays a role.
Altruism as a Function of Cognitive
Development
As children grow, their ability to think
about and understand other people and the world changes. The cognitive
perspective focuses on how altruistic behavior develops as a result of changes
in the child’s thinking skills. To study altruism from this perspective, Nancy
Eisenberg presented children with several moral dilemmas that pit one person’s
welfare against another person’s welfare. Here is one example: Bob, a young man
who was very good at swimming, was asked to help young crippled children who could
not walk to learn to swim so that they could strengthen their legs for walking.
Bob was the only one in his town who could do this job well, because only he
had both life-saving and teaching experiences. But helping crippled children
took much of Bob’s free time left after work and school, and Bob wanted to
practice hard as often as possible for an upcoming series of important swimming
contests. If Bob did not practice swimming in all his free time, his chances of
winning the contests and receiving a paid college education or sum of money
would be greatly lessened (Eisenberg & Mussen, 1989, p. 124).
The dilemma pits Bob’s needs against those
of other people. The children in Eisenberg’s study were asked several questions
about what Bob should do. For example, “Should Bob agree to teach the crippled
children? Why?” Based on their responses, children were classified according to
Eisenberg’s levels of prosocial reasoning. Eisenberg’s findings show that as
children get older, they are more likely to understand the needs of other
people and are less focused on their own selfish concerns. The research
suggests that this is a continual process and that people’s altruistic thinking
and behavior can change throughout life.
The idea that the development of altruism is
a lifelong process is supported by the fact that rescuers did not magically
become caring and empathic at the outset of the war. Instead, the ethic of
caring grew out of their personalities and interpersonal styles, which had
developed over the course of their lives. Rescuers were altruistic long before
the war (Huneke, 1986; Oliner & Oliner, 1988; Tec, 1986) and tended to
remain more altruistic than nonrescuers after the war (Oliner & Oliner,
1988).
Becoming an Altruistic Person
Altruism requires something more than
empathy and compassionate values (Staub, 1985). It requires the psychological
and practical competence to carry those intentions into action (Goleman, 1991).
Goodness, like evil, begins slowly, in small steps. Recall from the Chapter 7
discussion on social influence that we are often eased into behaviors in small
steps (i.e., through the foot-in-the-door technique). In a similar manner, many
rescuers gradually eased themselves into their roles as rescuers. People
responded to a first request for help and hid someone for a day or two. Once
they took that first step, they began to see themselves differently, as the
kind of people who rescued the desperate. Altruistic actions changed their
self-concept: Because I helped, I must be an altruistic person. As we saw in
Chapter 2, one way we gain self-knowledge is through observation of our own
behavior. We then apply that knowledge to our self-concept.
This is how Swedish diplomat Raoul
Wallenberg got involved in rescuing Hungarian Jews during World War II (Staub,
1985). The first person he rescued was a business partner who happened to be a
Hungarian Jew. Wallenberg then became more involved and more daring. He began
to manufacture passes for Jews, saying that they were citizens of Sweden. He
even handed out passes to Jews who were being put in the cattle cars that would
take them to the death camps. Wallenberg disappeared soon after, and his fate
is still unknown. Apparently, there is a unique type of person who is likely to
take that very first step to help and to continue helping until the end
(Goleman, 1991). Wallenberg and the other rescuers were such people.
Gender and Rescue
Research suggests that a small majority of
the rescuers were women (Becker & Eagly, 2004). For example, in Poland 57%
of rescuers were women. In France 55.6% were women. And in the Netherlands,
52.5% were women (Becker & Eagly, 2004). Becker and Eagly report that women
rescuers who were not part of a couple (e.g., husband-wife team) significantly
exceeded the number of such women in the general population. Further, the
motivation underlying male and female rescue differed. Women were more likely
to be motivated by interpersonal caring and a relationship orientation than men
(Anderson, 1993).
Anderson (1993) content analyzed the
questionnaire and interview data collected by Sam and Pearl Oliner (1988).
Anderson evaluated information on socialization experiences, the family
histories, and self-concepts of male and female rescuers. Anderson found very
different socialization experiences for male and female rescuers. She found
that men tended to be socialized toward civic life, had at least a high school
education, and were socialized to be autonomous. Women were more likely to be
socialized to be family oriented, were less likely to have had an education,
and were socialized for altruism. Anderson points out that these different
socialization experiences related to different forms of rescue activity for men
and women. Men, reflecting their socialization toward autonomy, were more likely
to work alone, rescuing large numbers of people, one at a time. Male rescue was
also more likely to be brief and repetitive (e.g., smuggling people out of
dangerous areas). Female rescuers, on the other hand, were more likely to work
with others in helping networks and help the same people over a longer period
of time. Anderson also found that women tended to be motivated by guilt and
expressed depression and doubts about their ability to help. Men were more
motivated to protect the innocent and were less socially connected than women.
A Synthesis: Situational and Personality
Factors in Altruism
We have seen that both situational and
personality factors influence the development and course of altruism. How do
these factors work together to produce altruistic behavior? Two approaches
provide some answers: the interactionist view and the application of the
five-stage decision model to long-term helping situations.
The Interactionist View
The interactionist view of altruism argues
that an individual’s internal motives (whether altruistic or selfish) interact
with situational factors to determine if a person will help (Callero, 1986).
Romer and his colleagues (Romer, Gruder, & Lizzadro, 1986) identified four
altruistic orientations based on the individual’s degree of nurturance (the
need to give help) and of succorance (the need to receive help):
1.
Altruistic—Those who are motivated to help others but not to receive
help
in return
2.
Receptive giving—Those who help to obtain something in return
3.
Selfish—Those who are primarily motivated to receive help but not give
it
4.
Inner-sustaining—Those who are not motivated to give or receive help
In their study, Romer and colleagues
(1986) led people to believe that they either would or would not be compensated
for their help. On the basis of the four orientations just described, these
researchers predicted that individuals with an altruistic orientation would
help even if compensation was not expected; receptive givers would be willing
to help only if they stood to gain something in return; selfish people would
not be oriented toward helping, regardless of compensation; and those described
as inner-sustaining would neither give nor receive, no matter what the
compensation.
Romer’s (1986) results confirmed this hypothesis.
Figure 11.8 shows the results on two indexes of helping: the percentage of
subjects who agree to help and the number of hours volunteered. Notice that
altruistic people were less likely to help when compensation was offered. This
is in keeping with the reverse-incentive effect described in Chapter 6. When
people are internally motivated to do something, giving them an external reward
decreases their motivation and their liking for the activity. There is also
evidence that personality and the situation interact in a way that can reduce
the bystander effect. In one study, researchers categorized subjects as either
“esteem oriented” or “safety oriented” (Wilson, 1976). Esteem-oriented
individuals are motivated by a strong sense of personal competency rather than
by what others do. Safety-oriented individuals are more dependent on what
others do. Subjects were exposed to a staged emergency (a simulated explosion
that supposedly hurt the experimenter), either while alone, in the presence of
a passive bystander (who makes no effort to help), or in the presence of a
helping bystander (who goes to the aid of the experimenter).
Figure 11.8 Helping
behavior and hours volunteered as a function of helping orientation and
compensation. Participants whose orientation was receptive giving were more
likely to help when they received compensation. Altruistic participants were
willing to help regardless of whether they were compensated.
From Romer, Gruder,
and Lizzadro (1986)
The study showed that esteem-oriented subjects
were more likely to help than safety-oriented subjects in all cases (Figure
11.9). Of most interest, however, is the fact that the esteem-oriented subjects
were more likely to help when a passive bystander was present than were the
safety-oriented subjects. Thus, subjects who are motivated internally (esteem
oriented) are not just more likely to help than those who are externally
motivated (safety oriented); they are also less likely to fall prey to the
influence of a passive bystander. This suggests that individuals who helped in
the classic experiments on the bystander effect may possess personality
characteristics that allow them to overcome the help-depressing effects of
bystanders.
Figure 11.9The relationship between
personality characteristics, presence, and type of bystander on the likelihood
of helping. Esteem-oriented participants were most likely to help, regardless
of bystander condition. Safety-oriented participants were most likely to help
if they were alone or if there was a helping bystander present. Based on data
from Wilson (1976).
We might also expect that the individual’s
personality will interact with the costs of giving help. Some individuals help
even though the cost of helping is high. For example, some subjects in Batson’s
(1990a) research described earlier in this chapter helped by offering to change
places with someone receiving electric shocks even though they could have
escaped the situation easily. And rescuers helped despite the fact that getting
caught helping Jews meant death. In contrast, there are those who will not help
even if helping requires minimal effort.
The degree to which the personality of the
helper affects helping may depend on the perceived costs involved in giving
aid. In relatively low-cost situations, personality will be less important than
the situation. However, in high-cost situations, personality will be more
important than the situation. As the perceived cost of helping increases,
personality exerts a stronger effect on the decision to help. This is
represented in Figure 11.10. The base of the triangle represents very low-cost
behaviors. As you move up the triangle, the cost of helping increases. The
relative size of each division of the triangle represents the number of people
who would be willing to help another in distress.
Figure 11.10The relationship between
personality and likeliness of helping in different helping situations. Nearly
everyone would help if cost were very low. As the cost of the helping act
increases, fewer and fewer individuals are expected to help. Only the most
altruistic individuals are expected to help in very high cost situations.
An extremely low-cost request (e.g.,
giving a stranger directions to the campus library) would result in most people’s
helping. People’s personalities matter little when it costs almost nothing to
help. In fact, probably more effort is spent on saying no than on directing the
passerby to the library. When the cost of helping becomes high, even
prohibitive, as in the case of rescuing Jews from the Nazis, fewer people help.
However, there are those who successfully overcome the situational forces
working against helping, perhaps due to their altruistic personalities, and
offer help.
Applying the Five-Stage Decision Model to
Long-Term Helping
Earlier in this chapter we described a
five-stage decision model of helping. That model has been applied exclusively
to the description and explanation of helping in spontaneous emergencies. Now
that we have explored some other aspects of helping, we can consider whether
that model may be applied to long-term and situation-specific spontaneous
helping. Let’s consider how each stage applies to the actions of those who
rescued Jews from the Nazis.
Noticing the Situation
For many rescuers, seeing the Nazis taking
Jews away provoked awareness. One rescuer, Irene Opdyke, first became aware of
the plight of the Jews when she happened to look through a hotel window and saw
Jews being rounded up and taken away (Opdyke & Elliot, 1992). Oliner and
Oliner (1988) reported that rescuers were motivated to action when they
witnessed some external event such as the one Opdyke witnessed. Of course,
however, many nonrescuers also saw the same events yet did not help.
Labeling the Situation as an Emergency
A critical factor in the decision to
rescue Jews was to label the situation as one serious enough to require
intervention. Here, the differences between rescuers and nonrescuers became
important. Apparently, rescuers were more likely to see the persecution of the
Jews as something serious that required intervention. The persecutions appeared
to insult the sensibilities of the rescuers. Nonrescuers often decided that
Jews must truly have done something to deserve their awful fate. They tended to
blame the victim and by so doing relieved themselves of any responsibility for
helping.
Rescuers also had social support to help
because they belonged to groups that valued such action. This is consistent
with the notion that encouragement from others may make it easier to label a
situation as one requiring intervention (Dozier & Miceli, 1985).
Assuming Responsibility to Help
The next step in the process is for the
rescuer to assume responsibility to help. For rescuers, the universalistic view
of the needy, ethics of justice and caring, and generally high levels of
empathy made assuming responsibility probable. In fact, many rescuers suggested
that after they noticed the persecution of Jews, they had to do something.
Their upbringing and view of the world made assumption of responsibility almost
a given rather than a decision. The main difference between the rescuers and
the nonrescuers who witnessed the same events was that the rescuers interpreted
the events as a call to action (Oliner & Oliner, 1988). For the rescuers,
the witnessed event connected with their principles of caring (Oliner &
Oliner, 1988) and led them to assume responsibility.
Another factor may have come into play
when the rescuers (or a bystander to an emergency situation) assumed
responsibility. Witnessing maltreatment of the Jews may have activated the norm
of social responsibility in these individuals. This norm involves the notion
that we should help others without regard to receiving help or a reward in
exchange (Berkowitz, 1972; Schwartz, 1975).
Deciding How to Help
Rescuers helped in a variety of ways
(Oliner & Oliner, 1988). They had to assess the alternatives available and
decide which was most appropriate. Alternatives included donating money to help
Jews, providing false papers, and hiding Jews. It appears that, at least
sometimes, perceived costs were not an issue. For example, Opdyke hid several
Jews in the basement of a German major’s house in which she was the
housekeeper, even after she witnessed a Polish family and the family of Jews
they were hiding hanged by the Nazis in the town marketplace.
Implementing the Decision to Help
The final stage, implementing the decision
to help, includes assessing rewards and costs for helping and potential
outcomes of helping versus not helping. When Everett Sanderson rescued someone
who had fallen onto the subway tracks, he said he could not have lived with
himself if he had not helped. This is an assessment of outcomes. For Sanderson,
the cost for not helping outweighed the cost for helping, despite the risks.
It is quite probable that the altruistic
personalities we have been studying made similar assessments. Because of their
upbringing and the events of their lives that defined them as altruistic
people, they decided that helping was less costly to them than not helping.
Most of them engaged in long-term helping. This suggests that they assessed the
outcome of their initial decision to help and decided that it was correct. This
was certainly true of Balwina Piecuch. It was also true of the Polish woman in
the following example, which illustrates the interactionist nature of
helping—the interplay of situational and personality factors and the
combination of spontaneous and long-term events:
A woman and her child were being led
through Cracow, Poland, with other Jews to a concentration station. The woman
ran up to a bystander and pleaded, “Please, please save my child.” A Polish
woman took the young boy to her apartment, where neighbors became suspicious of
this sudden appearance of a child and called the police. The captain of the police
department asked the woman if she knew the penalty for harboring a Jewish
child. The young woman said, with some heat, “You call yourself a Pole, a
gentleman, a man of the human race?” She continued her persuasive act, claiming
that one of the police in the room had actually fathered the child “and stooped
so low as to be willing to have the child killed” (Goldman, 1988, p. 8). Both
the woman and the young boy survived the war.
Altruistic Behavior from the Perspective
of the Recipient
Our discussion of altruism to this point
has centered on the helper. But helping situations, of course, involve another
person: the recipient. Social psychologists have asked two broad questions that
relate to the recipient of helping behavior: What influences an individual’s
decision to seek help? What reactions do individuals have to receiving help?
Seeking Help from Others
The earlier discussion of helping in
emergencies may have suggested that helping behavior occurs when someone
happens to stumble across a situation in which help is needed. Although this
does happen, there are also many situations in which an individual actively
seeks out help from another. Many Jews in Nazi-occupied Europe approached
potential helpers and asked for help. And today, we see many examples of people
seeking help: refugees seeking entrance to other countries, the homeless
seeking shelter, the uninsured seeking health care.
Seeking help has both positive and
negative aspects. On the positive side, the help a person needs will often be
forthcoming. For example, medical care may be given for a life-threatening
condition. On the negative side, a person may feel threatened or suffer loss of
self-esteem by asking for help (Fisher, Nadler, & Whitcher-Algana, 1982).
In Western society, a great premium is placed on being self-sufficient and
taking care of oneself. There is a social stigma attached to seeking help,
along with potential feelings of failure. Generally, seeking help generates
costs, as does helping (DePaulo & Fisher, 1980).
A Decision Model for Seeking Help
Researchers have suggested that a person
deciding whether to seek help may go through a series of decisions, much like
the helper does in Darley and LatanĂ©’s five-stage decision model. According to
Gross and McMullen (1982, p. 308), a person asks three questions before seeking
help:
1.
Do I have a problem that help will alleviate?
2.
Should I seek help?
3.
Who is most capable of providing the kind of help I need?
Gross and McMullen (1982) developed a
model to describe the process of help seeking. The model works in the following
way: Imagine that you have begun to have trouble falling asleep at night.
Before you will seek help, you must first become aware that there is a problem.
If you had trouble falling asleep only a few times, you probably will not
identify it as a problem, and you will not seek help. But if you have trouble
falling asleep for a few weeks, you may identify it as a problem and move to
the next stage of help seeking.
Now you must decide if the situation is
one that requires help. If you decide that it is not (the problem will go away
by itself), you will not seek help. If you decide that it is, you move on to
the next stage, deciding on the best way to alleviate the problem. Here you can
opt for self-help (go to the drugstore and buy some over-the-counter drug) or
help from an outside party (a physician or psychologist). If you choose
self-help and it is successful, the problem is solved and no further help is
sought. If the self-help is unsuccessful, you could then seek help from others
or resign yourself to the problem and seek no further help.
The likelihood that you may ask for and
receive help may also depend on the nature of the groups (and society) to which
you belong. Members of groups often behave altruistically toward one another
(Clark, Mills, & Powell, 1986) and are often governed by communal
relationships. Members benefit one another in response to each other’s needs
(Williamson & Clark, 1989). These relationships are in contrast to exchange
relationships, in which people benefit one another in response to, or with the
expectation of, receiving a benefit in return. Communal relationships are
characterized by helping even when people cannot reciprocate each other’s help
(Clark, Mills, & Powell, 1986).
Factors Influencing the Decision to Seek
Help
Clearly, the decision to seek help is just
as complex as the decision to give help. What factors come into play when a
person is deciding whether to seek help?
For one, individuals may be more likely to
ask for help when their need is low than when it is high (Krishan, 1988). This
could be related to the perceived “power” relationship between the helper and
the recipient. When need is low, people may perceive themselves to be on more
common footing with the helper. Additionally, when need is low, there is less
cost to the helper. People may be less likely to seek help if the cost to the
helper is high (DePaulo & Fisher, 1980).
Another variable in this decision-making
process is the person from whom the help is sought. Are people more willing to
seek help from a friend or from a stranger? In one study, the relationship
between the helper and the recipient (friends or strangers) and the cost to the
helper (high or low) were manipulated (Shapiro, 1980). Generally, subjects were
more likely to seek help from a friend than from a stranger (Figure 11.11).
When help was sought from a friend, the potential cost to the helper was not
important. When the helper was a stranger, subjects were reluctant to ask when
the cost was high.
Figure 11.11Help
seeking as a function of the cost of help and the nature of the potential
helper. Participants were likely to seek help from a friend in both low-cost
and high-cost helping situations. However, help was more likely to be sought
from a stranger if the cost of help were low.
Based on data from
Shapiro (1980)
There are several possible reasons for
this. First, people may feel more comfortable and less threatened asking a
friend rather than a stranger for costly help. Second, the norm of reciprocity
(see Chapter 7) may come into play in a more meaningful way with friends
(Gouldner, 1960). People may reason that they would do it for their friends if
they needed it. Thus, the expectation of reciprocity may make it easier to ask
for high-cost help from a friend. Third, people may perceive that they will
have more opportunities to reciprocate a friend’s help. They may never see a
stranger again.
A final variable that comes into play in
deciding to seek help is the type of task on which the help is needed. If
someone is doing something easy (but needs help), the person is less likely to
seek help than if the task is hard (DePaulo & Fisher, 1980). And if the
task is something in which the person has ego involvement, he or she is also
less likely to seek help. So, for example, accountants would be unlikely to
seek help preparing their own taxes, even if they needed the help.
Reacting to Help When It Is Given
When we help someone, or we see someone
receiving help, it is natural to expect that the person receiving the help will
show gratitude. However, there are times when received help is not appreciated
or when victims complain about the help that was received. After Hurricane
Katrina, for example, many displaced New Orleans residents complained about the
living accommodations and other support provided weeks after the hurricane
struck. Why do people who receive help not always react positively toward that
help? We shall explore this topic in this section.
Receiving help is a double-edged sword. On
the one hand, people are grateful for receiving help. On the other hand, they
may experience negative feelings when they are helped, feelings of guilt,
lowered self-esteem, and indebtedness. Jews who were hidden by rescuers, for
example, probably were concerned about the safety of their benefactors; they
also may have been disturbed by the thought that they could never reciprocate
the help they received.
Generally, there are four potentially
negative outcomes of receiving help. First, an inequitable relationship may be
created. Second, those who are helped may experience psychological reactance;
that is, they may feel their freedom is threatened by receiving help. Third,
those who receive help may make negative attributions about the intent of those
who have helped them. Fourth, those who receive help may suffer a loss of
selfesteem (Fisher et al., 1982). Let’s look at two of these outcomes: inequity
and threats to self-esteem.
The Creation of an Inequitable
Relationship
Recall from Chapter 9 that we strive to
maintain equity in our relationships with others. When inequity occurs, we feel
distress and are motivated to restore equity. Helping someone creates inequity
in a relationship (Fisher et al., 1982), because the recipient feels indebted
to the helper (Leventhal, Allen, & Kemelgor, 1969). The higher the cost to
the helper, the greater the inequity and the greater the negative feelings
(Gergen, 1974).
Inequity can be reversed when the help is
reciprocated. Generally, a recipient reacts more negatively to that help and
likes the helper less if he or she does not have the ability to reciprocate
(Castro, 1974). Recipients are also less likely to seek help in the future when
they have not been able to reciprocate, especially if the cost to the helper
was high.
The relationship between degree of
indebtedness and need to reciprocate is a complex one. For example, if someone
helps you voluntarily, you will reciprocate more than if someone is obliged to
help you as part of a job (Goranson & Berkowitz, 1966). You also are likely
to reciprocate when the cost to the donor is high (Pruitt, 1968).
Interestingly, the absolute amount of help given is less important than the
cost incurred by the helper (Aikwa, 1990; Pruitt, 1968). For example, if a
person who makes $100,000 per year gave you $1,000 (1% of the income), you
would feel less indebted to that person than if you received the same $1,000
from someone who makes $10,000 per year (10% of the income).
Finally, we need to distinguish between
the obligation and sense of gratitude a person receiving help might experience
and how that relates to reciprocity. Obligation is a feeling of “owing” someone
something. So, if I help you with a difficult task, you might feel that you owe
it to me to reciprocate the favor to restore equity. Gratitude is an expression
of appreciation. So, if I help you with that difficult task, you may express
your appreciation by reciprocating the favor. In an interesting study by Goei
and Boster (2005), obligation and gratitude were found to be conceptually different
and affected reciprocity differently. Goei and Boster found that doing a favor
for someone, especially a high-cost favor, increased gratitude but not
obligation. In response to increased gratitude, participants were then willing
to comply with a request for help. So, it may be a response to a feeling of
gratitude that drives the restoration of equity after receiving help.
Threats to Self-Esteem
Perhaps the strongest explanation for the
negative impact of receiving help centers on threats to self-esteem. When
people become dependent on others, especially in Western society, their
self-esteem and self-worth come into question (Fisher et al., 1982). Under
these conditions, receiving help may be a threatening experience.
There is considerable support for the
threat to self-esteem model. In one study, subjects who received aid on an
analogy task showed greater decrements in situational self-esteem (self-esteem
tied to a specific situation) than subjects not receiving help (Balls &
Eisenberg, 1986). In another study, researchers artificially manipulated
subjects’situational self-esteem by providing them with either positive or
negative information about themselves (Nadler, Altman, & Fisher, 1979). The
researchers then created a situation in which the individual either received or
did not receive aid. Subjects who received self-enhancing information (positive
self-information) showed more negative affect when aid was offered than when no
aid was offered. Subjects who received negative self-information showed
positive affect when they were helped.
Thus, subjects who had positive thoughts
about themselves were more negatively affected by help than those who had
negative thoughts about themselves. The offer of help was a greater threat to
those with high self-esteem than to those with low self-esteem. In other words,
not only does receiving help threaten self-esteem but also the higher a person’s
self-esteem is, the more threatened that person is by offers of help. For
example, if you consider yourself the world’s best brain surgeon, asking for
assistance on a case would be more disturbing to you than if you saw yourself
as an average brain surgeon.
When someone with high self-esteem fails
at a task, that failure is inconsistent with his or her positive self-image
(Nadler, Fisher, & Streufert, 1976). Help offered in this situation is
perceived as threatening, especially if it comes from someone who is similar
(Fisher & Nadler, 1974; Nadler et al., 1979). Receiving help from someone
similar may be seen as a sign of relative inferiority and dependency (Nadler et
al., 1979).
Conversely, when a person with high
self-esteem receives help from a dissimilar person, he or she experiences an
increase in situational self-esteem and self-confidence. When a person with low
self-esteem receives help from a similar other, that help is more consistent
with the individual’s self-image. For these individuals, help from a similar
other is seen as an expression of concern, and they respond positively (Nadler
et al., 1979).
A model to explain the complex
relationship between self-esteem and receiving help was developed by Nadler,
Fisher, and Ben Itchak (1983). The model suggests that help from a friend is
more psychologically significant than help from a stranger. This greater
significance is translated into negative affect if failure occurs on something
that is ego involving (e.g., losing a job). Here, help from a friend is seen as
a threat to one’s self-esteem, and a negative reaction follows.
Receiving help can be particularly
threatening when it is unsolicited and imposed by someone (Deelstra et al.,
2003). Deelstra et al. had participants work on a task that did not present a
problem, a task that involved a solvable problem, and a task that presented an
unsolvable problem. In each condition, a confederate either did or did not
provide unsolicited help. The results showed that participants had the
strongest negative reaction to the help imposed when they perceived that no
problem existed or that a solvable problem existed. There was also a
significant change in the participant’s heart rate that paralleled this
finding. Participants showed the most heart rate increase when help was imposed
in the no-problem or solvable-problem conditions. Apparently, receiving
unwanted help is not only psychologically threatening, but it is also
physiologically arousing!
A study conducted in France investigated
how a recipient’s age (young, middle, or older adult) and degree of control
over a situation affected reactions to receiving help (Raynaud-Maintier &
Alaphillippe, 2001). Participants worked on an anagram task and received
varying amounts of help. The researchers found that, consistent with the threat
to self-esteem model, receiving help was threatening, especially when the help
was offered by an older adult or a helper with high self-esteem. The more
control participants had over the situation, the less threatening the help was
and the older the participant, the lower the threat of receiving help.
There are also gender differences in how
people react to receiving help. In one study, males and females were paired
with fictitious partners of comparable, superior, or inferior ability and were
offered help by that partner (Balls & Eisenberg, 1986).
Females paired with a partner of similar
ability showed greater reductions in situational self-esteem than males paired
with a similar partner. Thus, females perceived help as more threatening to
self-esteem than did males. Females, however, were more satisfied than males
with the help they received. Females were also more likely than males to
express a need for help.
Reactions to receiving help, then, are
influenced by several factors, including the ability to reciprocate, the
similarity or dissimilarity of the helper, self-esteem, and gender. Other
factors can play a role as well. For example, if the helper has positive
attributes and is seen as having good motives, the person receiving help is
more likely to feel positive about the experience. A positive outcome is also
more likely if the help is offered rather than requested, if the help is given
on an ego-relevant task, and if the help does not compromise the recipient’s
freedom (e.g., with a very high obligation to repay the helper). Overall, we
see that an individual’s reaction to receiving help is influenced by an
interaction between situational variables (for example, the helper’s
characteristics) and personality variables (Fisher et al., 1982).
Irene Opdyke Revisited
Irene Opdyke offered help to people she
hardly knew and put her life at great risk. Opdyke was undoubtedly an empathic
person who felt the suffering of the Jews. In deciding to help, she almost
surely went through something similar to the process described in this chapter.
She noticed the situation requiring help when she heard about the liquidation
of the ghetto. She labeled the situation as one that required help, and she
assumed responsibility for helping. She knew what she had to do to help: find a
place to hide the Jews. Finally, she implemented her decision to help. Irene
Opdyke’s behavior fits quite well with the five-stage decision model for
helping.
Opdyke’s decision was also similar to the
decisions made by hundreds of other rescuers of Jews. Opdyke and the other
rescuers put their lives on the line to save others. We know something about
Irene Opdyke and her commitment to helping people. After all, she was studying
to be a nurse before the war. It is obvious that Irene Opdyke had empathy for
those in need and was able to translate that empathy into tangible action.
Irene Opdyke provides us with an inspiring example of an altruistic person who
put the welfare of others above her own.
Chapter Review
1. What is altruism and how is it
different from helping behavior? Why is the difference important?
Altruism is behavior that helps a person
in need that is focused on the victim and is motivated purely by the desire to
help the other person. Other, similar behaviors may be motivated by relieving
one’s personal distress or to gain some reward. These behaviors are categorized
as helping behavior. The motivation underlying an act of help is important
because it may affect the quality of the help given.
2. What are empathy and egoism, and how do
they relate to altruism?
Empathy refers to compassionate
understanding of how a person in need feels. Some acts of helping are focused
on and motivated by our desire to relieve the suffering of the victim rather
than our own discomfort. Empathy for a person in need is rooted in perspective
taking. A person who focuses on how a person in distress feels is more likely
to experience empathy. The empathy-altruism hypothesis proposes that arousal of
empathy increases the likelihood of altruism. This hypothesis has received
research support, but it remains controversial. In contrast, egoism refers to a
motive for helping that is focused on relieving our own discomfort rather than
on relieving the victim’s suffering.
3. What about the idea that we may help to
avoid guilt or shame?
This has been raised as a possibility in
the empathy-punishment hypothesis, which states that people help to avoid the
guilt and shame associated with not helping. Research pitting this hypothesis
against the empathy-altruism hypothesis has fallen on the side of
empathy-altruism. However, the book is still open on the validity of the empathy-altruism
hypothesis.
4. What role does biology play in
altruism?
There is evidence that helping has biological roots, as suggested by
sociobiologists. According to this view, helping is biologically adaptive and
helps a species survive. The focus of this explanation is on survival of the
gene pool of a species rather than on survival of any one member of a species.
According to evolutionary biologists, animals are more likely to help members
of their own family through alloparenting. For humans, a similar effect occurs:
We are more likely to help others who are like us and who thus share genetic
material.
Although this idea has some merit, it
cannot account for the complexity of animal or human altruism. We might have
predicted, based on the biological explanation, that Irene Opdyke would not
have been motivated to help the Jews in Ternopol because they were not related
and were members of different ethnic and religious groups.
5. How do social psychologists explain
helping in an emergency situation?
To
explain helping (or nonhelping) in emergencies, social psychologists Darley and
Latané developed a decision model with five stages: noticing the emergency,
labeling the emergency correctly, assuming responsibility to help, knowing what
to do, and implementing the decision to help. At each stage, many variables
influence an individual’s decision to help.
At the noticing stage, anything that makes
the emergency stand out increases the likelihood of help being offered.
However, interpreting a situation as an emergency can be ambiguous, and we may
mislabel it, in which case we do not give help.
Next, we must assume personal
responsibility for helping. This is known as the bystander effect. Three
reasons for this failure to help when bystanders are present are diffusion of
responsibility (assuming that someone else will help), pluralistic ignorance
(responding to the inaction of others), and assuming a social category
relationship (assuming that parties in a situation belong together). Although
the bystander effect is a powerful, reliable phenomenon, there are exceptions
to it. Research shows that when help requires potentially dangerous
intervention, people are more likely to help when in groups than when alone.
The bystander effect is less likely to occur when the helping situation
confronting us involves a clear violation of a social norm that we personally
care about.
Even if we assume responsibility, we may
not help because we do not know what to do or lack skills, or we may think that
someone else is more qualified to help. Finally, we may fail to help because
the costs of helping are seen as too high. Costs are increased when we might be
injured or otherwise inconvenienced by stopping to help.
6. What factors affect the decision to
help?
Mood makes a difference. Bystanders who
are in a positive (good) mood are more likely to help others. However, people
may not help if they think helping will spoil their good mood. Characteristics
of the victim also play a role. Females are more likely to be helped if the helper
is male. Physically attractive people are more likely to be helped than
unattractive people. We also take into account whether we feel that the victim
deserves help. If we believe the victim contributed to his or her own problems,
we are less likely to help than if we believe the victim did not contribute.
This fits with the just-world hypothesis, the idea that people get what they
deserve and deserve what they get. We may relax this standard if we believe the
victim strongly needs our help.
7. If you need help, how can you increase
your chances of receiving help?
You need to help people come to the right
decision at each stage of the decision model. To ensure that you get noticed,
make any plea for help as loud and as clear as possible. This will also help
bystanders correctly label your situation as an emergency. To get someone to
assume responsibility, make eye contact with a bystander. Better yet, make a
direct request of a particular bystander for help. Research shows that making
such a request increases a bystander’s sense of responsibility for helping you
and increases the likelihood of helping.
8. Other than traditional helping in
emergency situations, what other forms of helping are there?
Although social psychologists have
historically focused on helping in relatively benign emergency situations,
there are other forms of help that involve risk. Courageous resistance is one
such form of helping. Courageous resistance is a form of helping that involves
significant risk to the helper (or the helper’s family), requires a long-term
commitment, and occurs after a deliberative process. Courageous resistors
include whistleblowers, political activists, and rescuers of Jews during the
Holocaust. Heroism is another form of helping that is closely related to courageous
resistance. In both cases there is substantial risk to the helper. However,
heroism need not involve a long-term commitment and may not require a
deliberative process to decide to help.
9. How do personality characteristics
relate to helping?
Although
situational factors play an important role in helping, especially spontaneous
helping, they may not give us a true picture of the helper and how he or she
might behave across helping situations. Personality characteristics may become
more relevant when nonspontaneous, long-term helping is considered. In this
case, more planning and thought are required. Some individuals might possess an
altruistic personality, or a cluster of traits, including empathy, that
predisposes a person to helping.
Research on rescuers of Jews in
Nazi-occupied Europe—who have been designated righteous rescuers by
Israel—provides evidence for the existence of an altruistic personality.
Rescuers from Eastern Europe (especially Poland) displayed autonomous altruism,
altruism that is not supported by social norms. Rescuers from Western Europe
were more likely to display normative altruism, altruism that society supports
and recognizes.
10. What situational and personality
variables played a role in the decision to help Jews in Nazi-occupied Europe?
Although situational factors did not exert as strong an influence on the
decision to help as one might expect, two have been found to be significant:
the presence of family or group support and the initiation of rescue efforts as
a result of a specific request for help. After rescuers began helping, they
were likely to continue helping.
There were also personality variables that
related to the decision to become a rescuer. Compared to nonrescuers, rescuers
were higher in emotional empathy (sensitivity to the suffering of others) and
had a strong sense of social responsibility. Other characteristics of rescuers
included an inability to blend with others, a high level of independence and
self-reliance, a commitment to helping before the war, a matter-of-fact
attitude about their helping, and a universalistic view of the needy.
11. What factors contribute to a person’s
developing an altruistic personality?
Oliner and Oliner found that families of rescuers of Jews in
Nazi-occupied Europe and families of nonrescuers differed in their styles.
Families of rescuers provided role models for helping and stressed the
universal nature of all people. They emphasized aspects of religion that focus
on caring for others, and they were less likely to discuss negative stereotypes
of Jews. Parents of altruistic individuals tended to be warm and nurturing in
their parenting style. Parents of rescuers used less physical punishment than
parents of nonrescuers, relying instead on induction.
Cognitive development also contributes to
the development of an altruistic personality. As children get older, they are
more likely to understand the needs of others. This development is a lifelong
process.
Rescuers did not magically become
altruists when World War II broke out. Instead, they tended to be helpers long
before the war. Becoming a rescuer involved a series of small steps. In many
cases, rescuers started with a small act and then moved to larger ones.
12. What is the interactionist view of
altruism?
According to the interactionist view of altruism, personality and
situational factors interact to influence helping. Research has identified four
altruistic orientations: altruistic (those who are motivated to help others but
not to receive help in return), receptive giving (those who help to obtain
something in return), selfish (those who are primarily motivated to receive
help but not give it), and inner sustaining (those who are not motivated to
give or receive help).
Research shows that individuals with an
altruistic orientation are less likely to help if compensation is offered.
There is also evidence that personality factors can help a person overcome the
bystander effect. Esteem-oriented individuals (who are motivated internally)
are more likely to help than safetyoriented individuals (who are externally
motivated) when a passive bystander is present. Additionally, personality and
cost of help might interact. For low-cost behaviors, we would expect
personality factors to be less important than for high-cost behaviors.
13. How does long-term helping relate to
models of emergency helping?
With slight modification, Latané and
Darley’s five-stage model applies to long-term helping. Noticing, labeling,
accepting responsibility, deciding how to help, and implementing the decision
to help are all relevant to acts of longterm help. Additionally, at the
assuming responsibility stage, the norm of social responsibility may have been
activated. This norm suggests that we should help those in need without regard
to reward.
14. What factors influence a person’s
likelihood of seeking and receiving help?
Seeking help from others is a double-edged
sword: The person in need is more likely to receive help but also incurs a
cost. Helping also involves costs for the helper. A person in need of help
weighs these costs when deciding whether to ask for help, progressing through a
multistage process. A person is more likely to seek help when his or her needs
are low, and to seek help from a friend, especially if the cost to the helper
is high. A person is less likely to seek help with something easy than with
something hard.
15. What reactions do people show to
receiving help?
Receiving help is also a double-edged
sword. The help relieves the situation but leads to negative side effects,
including feelings of guilt, lowered self-esteem, and indebtedness to the
helper. Generally, there are four negative reactions to receiving aid: the
creation of inequity between the helper and the recipient, psychological
reactance, negative attributions about the helper, and threats to one’s
self-esteem. There is considerable support for the threat to self-esteem model
of reactions to receiving help. How much a person’s self-esteem is threatened
depends on several factors, including the type of task and the source of the
help. Males and females differ in their responses to receiving help. Females
react more negatively to receiving help but are more satisfied than males with
the help they receive.
From:
*********************************************
Social Psychology
Third Edition
Kenneth S. Bordens Indiana University—Purdue University Fort Wayne
Irwin A. Horowitz - Oregon State University
Social Psychology, 3rd Edition
Copyright ©2008 by Freeload Press
Illustration used on cover © 2008 JupiterImages Corporation
ISBN 1-930789-04-1
No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or
by any means, electronic, mechanical, recording, photocopying, or otherwise, without the prior written
permission of the publisher.
Printed in the United States of America by Freeload Press.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Komentar
Posting Komentar