Persuasion and Attitude Change
6 Persuasion
and Attitude Change 185
Chicago,
1924: Jacob Franks, a wealthy businessman, answered the telephone and listened
as a young but cultivated voice told him that his 14-year-old son, Bobby, had
been kidnapped and could be ransomed for $10,000. The next morning, while Mr.
Franks arranged for the ransom, he was notifi ed that the nude and bloody body
of his son had been found in a culvert on Chicago’s South Side. Franks was sure
that the boy in the morgue was not Bobby, because the kidnappers had assured
him that this was simply a business proposition. He sent his brother to the
morgue to clear up the misidentifi cation. Unfortunately, the body was that of
his son; his head had been split open by a blow from a blunt instrument.
The case
was solved quickly. The police found a pair of eyeglasses near the body and
traced them to Nathan Leopold, Jr., the 20-year-old son of a prominent local
entrepreneur. Leopold denied any connection to the murder, claiming he had
spent the day with his friend, Richard Loeb, the son of a vice president of
Sears, Roebuck, and Company. However, both men soon confessed. Loeb, it seemed,
had always dreamed of committing the “perfect crime.” He had enlisted Leopold,
and together they had gone to their old school playground and followed several
different boys around. They fi nally settled on Bobby Franks and pushed him
into their car. Loeb hit Bobby over the head with a chisel, and then he and
Leopold drove in a leisurely fashion to the culvert, stopping along the way for
a bite to eat. The trial was a media circus. The Leopold and Loeb families
hired the most famous trial lawyer of that time, Clarence Darrow, to plead for
their sons. The men had already confessed, so the issue was not whether they
were guilty. It was whether they would spend the rest of their lives in
prison—or hang. The prosecution argued for hanging the murderers. Darrow
pleaded for mercy.
Darrow had
a tough fi ght: He needed all his persuasive skills to convince Judge Caverly
of his point of view (a jury was not required). He spoke for 12 hours, trying
to provide the judge with a rationale for sentencing the men to life
imprisonment. He argued that life sentences would serve a better, more humane
purpose than bowing to public opinion and hanging those two “mentally diseased
boys.” Darrow also claimed disinterest in the fates of his clients, an
interesting ploy for a lawyer who spoke from morning to night on their behalf.
In fact, he suggested that life in prison would be a worse fate than death. At
the end of Darrow’s oration, the judge was in tears, as were many spectators.
Darrow’s
arguments hit the mark. Judge Caverly sentenced Leopold and Loeb to life
imprisonment for murder and 99 years for kidnapping. Darrow’s impassioned, eloquent
arguments persuaded the judge to spare his clients’ lives (Weinberg, 1957).
Clarence Darrow’s task was to convince the judge that his clients’ lives should
be spared. He knew that the judge favored the death penalty, as did almost all
the American public. If Darrow couldn’t change the judge’s attitude, he had to
convince him that his attitude should not be applied in this case—that is, that
he should behave contrary to his beliefs.
The Persuasion Process
Darrow
used all his powers of persuasionto influence the judge. Persuasion is the
application of rational and/or emotional arguments to convince others to change
their attitudes or behavior. It is a form of social influence used not only in
the courtroom but also in every part of daily social life. The persuasion
process goes on in the classroom, church, political arena, and the media.
Persuasive messages are so much a part of our lives that we often are oblivious
to the bombardment from billboards, TV, radio, newspapers, parents, peers, and
public figures.
Persuasion,
then, is a pervasive form of social influence. We are all agents of social
influence when we try to convince others to change their attitudes or behavior.
We are also targets of social influence when others try to persuade or coerce
us to do what they want us to do.
In this
chapter, we explore the process of persuasion, looking at the strategies
communicators use to change people’s attitudes or behavior. We consider the
techniques of persuasion
used by a brilliant trial lawyer such as Clarence Darrow. How was Darrow able
to be so effective? He was a famous trial lawyer, highly regarded and highly
credible. Was his persuasiveness a function of something about him? Or was it
something about the argument he made? What role did his audience—Judge
Caverly—play in the persuasiveness of the argument? In what ways might the
judge have taken an active role in persuading himself of the validity of Darrow’s
case? And how does persuasion, both
interpersonal and mass persuasion, affect us all every day as we go about our
lives? These are some of the questions addressed in this chapter.
The Yale
Communication Model
What is
the best way to communicate your ideas to others and persuade them to accept
your point of view? An early view suggested that the most effective approach to
persuasion was to present logical arguments that showed people how they would
benefit from changing their attitudes. This view was formulated by Carl
Hovland, who worked for the U.S. government in its propaganda efforts during
World War II. After the war, he returned to Yale University, where he gathered
a team of 30 coworkers and began to systematically study the process of
persuasion. Out of their efforts came the Yale communication model (Hovland,
Janis, & Kelley, 1953).
According
to the Yale communication model, the most important factors comprising the
communication process are expressed by the question, Who says what to whom by
what means? This question suggests that there are four factors involved in
persuasion. The “who” refers to the communicator, the person making the
persuasive argument. The “what” refers to the organization and content of the
persuasive message. The “whom” is the target of the persuasive message, the
audience. Finally, the “means” points to the importance of the channel or
medium through which the message is conveyed, such as television, radio, or
interpersonal face-to-face communication. For each factor, there are several
variables that can potentially influence the persuasion process.
A key
assumption of the Yale model is that these four factors (which can be
manipulated in an experiment) provide input into three internal mediators: the
attention, comprehension, and acceptance mediators. Persuasion, according to
the Yale model, will occur if the target of a persuasive message first attends
to the message, then comprehends (understands) the content of the message, and
finally accepts the content of the message. What this means is that the Yale
model proposes that persuasion is a function of controlled processing of the
message. That is, a person who is persuaded actively attends to the message,
makes an effort to understand the content of the message, and finally decides
to accept the message.
Finally,
the four factors contributing to persuasion are not independent of one another;
they interact to create a persuasive effect. In practice, the content and
presentation of the message depend on the communicator, the audience, and the
channel. Darrow carefully chose his messages according to what arguments best
suited the judge, the public, the trial setting, and his own preferences. We
turn now to a discussion of the four factors, considering selected variables
within each component. We also look at how the factors interact with one
another.
The
Communicator
Have you
ever seen a late-night infomercial on TV? These half-hour commercials usually
push a “miracle” product, such as the car wax that supposedly can withstand a
direct hit from a hydrogen bomb. The car is vaporized but the wax survives.
There is an “expert” (usually the inventor) who touts the product’s
virtues. Do you believe what this person tells
you? Many people must, given the large amounts of money made from infomercials.
However,
many people clearly are not convinced. If you are not persuaded, one thing you
may focus on is the communicator. You may find yourself questioning this fellow’s
integrity (because he
will profit by persuading you to buy the atomic car wax) and, consequently,
disbelieving his claims. In other words, you question his credibility.
Credibility:
Expertise and Trustworthiness
Clarence
Darrow knew the importance of credibility, the power to inspire belief. During
his final arguments in the Leopold and Loeb case, Darrow continually tried to
undermine the prosecution’s credibility and increase his own in the
eyes of the judge. For example,
Darrow said of his opponent:
I have
heard in the last six weeks nothing but the cry for blood. I have heard from
the office of the state’s attorney only ugly hate. I have seen a court urged . . . to
hang two boys, in the face of science, in the face of philosophy, in the face
of the better and more humane thought. (Weinberg, 1957, p. 134)
Although
other variables are important, including a communicator’s
perceived
attractiveness and power, credibility is the most critical variable affecting
the ability to persuade. Credibility has two components: expertise and
trustworthiness. Expertise refers to a communicator’s
credentials and stems from the person’s training and knowledge. For example,
your doctor has the ability to persuade you on health matters because she has the education and experience
that give her words power. Trustworthiness refers to the audience’s
assessment of the communicator’s character as well as his or her motives for delivering the message.
We ask, “Why is this person trying to convince us?” Trustworthiness may be
diminished when we perceive that the communicator has something to gain from
persuading us. For example, you might trust a review of a product published in
Consumer Reports (which accepts no advertising and runs independent tests) more
than a similar review based on research conducted by the manufacturer of the
product.
Expertise
and trustworthiness do not always go together. A communicator may be high in
one but low in the other. A research physician speaking about a new drug to
treat AIDS may have expertise and derive credibility from that expert
knowledge. But if we discover that the physician stands to gain something from
the sale of this drug, we probably will question her trustworthiness. We wonder
about her character and motives and may no longer consider her a credible
source.
A
political figure with the unfortunate mix of high expertise and low
trustworthiness was former President Bill Clinton. He was highly knowledgeable
on matters of state but was not perceived as very trustworthy. During the
“Monica Lewinsky” scandal, there is the enduring image of President Clinton
waving his finger at the TV cameras, saying he never had sexual relations with
“that woman.” In contrast, a source can be highly trustworthy but low in
expertise. This was the case with late President Ronald Reagan. During speeches
he often used unsubstantiated statistics, sending his aides scrambling for
sources. However, the public generally saw him as trustworthy. People wanted to
believe him. Public opinion surveys showed again and again that a majority of
the public viewed President Reagan as personally attractive and likable, and
these qualities prime us to accept a persuader’s message
(Roskos-Ewoldsen & Fazio,
1992).
Trustworthiness
is, in part, a judgment about the motives of the communicator. If someone is
trying very hard to persuade us, we are likely to question his or her motives
(Eagly, Wood, & Chaiken, 1978). We may be more convinced by the communicator’s
arguments if we don’t
think he or she is trying to persuade us (Walster [Hatfield] & Festinger, 1962). This is the theory
behind the hidden-camera technique used by television advertisers. Presumably,
a person touting the virtues of a fabric softener on hidden camera must be
telling the truth. The communicator is not trying to convince us; he or she is
giving an unbiased testimonial.
Interestingly,
messages coming from a trustworthy or untrustworthy source are processed
differently (Preister & Petty, 2003). A target of a persuasive appeal from
a trustworthy source is less likely to process the content of the message
carefully and elaborate in memory, compared to the same message coming from an
untrustworthy source. That is, the arguments made by a trustworthy source are
more likely to be accepted on face value than those presented by an
untrustworthy source. Further, the difference between an untrustworthy and
trustworthy source is greatest when the arguments being presented are weak.
When strong arguments are presented, the trustworthy and untrustworthy sources
are equally likely to produce attitude change (Priester & Petty, 2003).
A
communicator who appears to argue against his or her own best interest is more
persuasive than a communicator who takes an expected stance (Eagly et al.,
1978). This was the case when then newly appointed U.S. Attorney General Janet
Reno took responsibility for the 1993 attack by federal agents on David Koresh’s
Branch Davidian headquarters
in Waco, Texas. The attack, subsequently acknowledged by the government as ill
planned, led to a fiery holocaust in which most of the cult members, including
many children, died. At a time when everyone connected with the attack was
denying responsibility for it, Reno publicly assumed the responsibility for
ordering the assault. Although her statement was not in her own best interest,
it enhanced the public’s sense of her character and credibility. Clarence Darrow also
seemed to be arguing against his own best interest when he suggested to the
judge that he did not care about the fate of his clients. Instead, he
maintained, he was strongly interested in what the verdict meant for the future
of humanity: “I am pleading for the future; I am pleading for a time when
hatred and cruelty will not control the hearts of men, when . . . all life is
worth saving, and that mercy is the highest attribute of man” (Weinberg, 1957,
p. 134).
Darrow
tried to increase his credibility by saying he was not acting out of
self-interest or concern for the fate of Leopold and Loeb; he was fighting for
a moral cause. Of course, Darrow did not mention that his fee was one of the
highest ever paid to an attorney.
Limits on
Credibility: The Sleeper Effect Does a credible communicator have an advantage
over a noncredible one in the long run? Apparently not. Research has shown that
there are limits to a credible communicator’s influence. The
Yale group found that although
the credibility of the communicator has a strong effect on attitude change,
over time people forget who said what, so the effects of credibility wear off.
Initially, people believe the credible source. But 6 weeks later, they are
about as likely to show attitude change from a noncredible source as from a
credible source. So, if you read an article in the National Enquirer, it
probably would have little effect on you right away. But after a few weeks, you
might show some change despite the source’s low credibility. The phenomenon of a message having
more impact on attitude change after a long delay than when it is first heard
is known as the sleeper effect. The sleeper effect has been shown in a wide
variety of persuasion situations, including political attack advertisements
(Lariscy & Tinkham, 1999). In their experiment Lariscy and Tinkham exposed participants
to a televised political attack advertisement. Some participants also saw a
second political advertisement that called the credibility of the attack ad
into question. This defensive advertisement was presented either before or
after the attack advertisement. Lariscy and Tinkham measured perceived
credibility of the source of the attack advertisement and how certain
participants were that they would vote for the candidate who sponsored the
attack advertisement. The results showed that the negative advertisement was
effective, even though participants indicated they disliked the negativity.
Evidence was also found for a sleeper effect. When the defensive advertisement
was presented after the attack advertisement, perceptions of the candidate who
sponsored the attack ad were negative. However, after a delay, the defensive
advertisement lost its power to attenuate the effect of the attack
advertisement. Why does the sleeper effect occur? One possible cause of the
sleeper effect may be that the communicator’s credibility does
not increase the listener’s
understanding of the message (Kelman & Hovland, 1953). In other words,
people understand messages from credible and noncredible communicators equally
well. As the effects of credibility wear off over time, listeners are left with
two equally understood (or misunderstood) messages (Gruder et al., 1979).
Three
factors make it more likely that the sleeper effect will occur (Rajecki, 1990):
1. There is a strong persuasive argument.
2. There is a discounting cue, something that
makes the receiver doubt the accuracy of the message, such as lack of
communicator credibility or new information that contradicts the original
message.
3. Enough time passes that the discounting cue
and the message become disassociated, and people forget which source said what.
A
meta-analysis of the sleeper effect literature (Kumkale & Albarracin, 2004)
found that two other factors were also relevant to the occurrence of the
sleeper effect. First, the sleeper effect is most likely to occur if both the
message and the credibility information are strong. Second, the sleeper effect
is stronger for individuals who are motivated to carefully process and think
about the message and credibility information. This latter finding suggests
that the sleeper effect requires active, controlled processing of the message
content and credibility information. Studies also show that the sleeper effect
occurs most reliably when the receivers get the discounting cue after they hear
the message rather than before (Kumkale & Albarracin, 2004; Pratkanis,
Greenwald, Leippe, & Baumgardner, 1988). If the discounting cue comes
before the message, the receiver doubts the message before it is even conveyed.
But if the discounting cue comes after the message, and if the argument is
strong, the receiver probably has already been persuaded. Over time, the memory
of the discounting cue “decays” faster than the memory of the persuasive
message (Pratkanis et al., 1988). Because the message is stored before the
discounting cue is received, the message is less likely to be weakened. After a
long period has elapsed, all the receiver remembers is the original persuasive
message (Figure 6.1).
Figure
6.1The sleeper effect in persuasion. When attitudes are measured immediately, a
message from a low-credibility communicator is not persuasive. However, after a
delay, the low-credibility communicator becomes more persuasive.
What can
we say happens to a persuasive message after several weeks? When the
discounting cue occurs before the message, the effect of the message
diminishes. When the discounting cue occurs after the message, the power of the
message is reinforced. The lesson for persuaders, then, is that they should
attack their adversary before he or she makes a case or conveys a rebuttal.
Gender of
the Communicator and Persuasion
Does it
matter whether the communicator of a persuasive message is male or female?
Unfortunately, there is not a great deal of research on this. Early research
produced inconsistent results (Flanagin & Metzger, 2003). Sometimes, males
were more persuasive, and sometimes, females were more persuasive. In fact, the
relationship between gender of the communicator and persuasion is not simple,
as we shall see next. In one experiment male and female participants evaluated
information on a personal Web site attributed to either a male or female author
(Flanagin & Metzger, 2003). Participants visited a Web site that was
specially designed for the experiment. On the Web site participants read a
passage on the harmful effects to pregnant women of radiation exposure during
pregnancy. Participants rated the credibility of the source of the message. The
results showed that male participants rated the female author as more credible
than the male author. Conversely, female participants rated the male source as
more credible than the female source.
In another
study (Schuller, Terry, & McKimmie, 2005) male and female participants
evaluated expert testimony (simple or complex) that was presented by either a
male or female expert witness. The results, shown in Figure 6.2, showed that
the male expert witness was more persuasive (resulting in higher dollar awards)
than the female expert witness when the evidence was complex. However, the
female expert witness was more persuasive when the evidence was less complex.
The male expert has an advantage when the content of the message requires more
cognitive effort to process, and the female expert has an advantage when the
message does not require such effort. Gender, then, is used differently
depending on the nature of the cognitive processing required (Schuler et al.,
2005).
Figure
6.2The relationship between the gender of an expert witness and the complexity
of trial testimony
There is
also some evidence for a gender-domain effect, meaning that a male communicator
may be more persuasive for male-oriented issues, and a female communicator may
be more persuasive for female-oriented issues (McKimmie, Newton, Terry, &
Schuller, 2004; Schuller, Terry, & McKimmie, 2001). McKimmie et al. (2004)
found that a male expert was more persuasive than a female expert when the case
was male-oriented (a case involving an automotive service company). When the
case was female-oriented (a case involving a cosmetics company), the female
expert was more persuasive. They also found that jurors evaluated the expert
witness more favorably when he or she testified about a gender-congruent case.
The
Message and the Audience
Thus far,
we have seen that the characteristics of the communicator can influence the degree
to which we modify our attitudes in response to a persuasive message. But what
about the message itself? What characteristics of messages make them more or
less persuasive, and how do these elements interact with the characteristics of
the audience?
We address
these questions next.
What Kind
of Message Is Most Effective? The Power of Fear
An
important quality of the message is whether it is based on rational or
emotional appeals. Early research showed that appeal to one emotion in
particular—fear—can make a message more effective than can appeal to reason or
logic. Psychologists found at first that an appeal containing a mild threat and
evoking a low level of fear was more effective than an appeal eliciting very
high levels of fear (Hovland et al., 1953). Then research suggested that
moderate levels of fear may be most effective (Leventhal, 1970). That is, you
need enough fear to grab people’s attention but not so much you send them running for
their lives. If the message is boring, people do not pay attention. If it is
too ferocious, they are repelled.
However,
persuaders need to do more than make the audience fearful; they also need to
provide a possible solution. If the message is that smoking cigarettes results
in major health risks, and if the communicator does not offer a method for
smokers to quit, then little attitude or behavior change will occur. The smoker
will be motivated to change behavior if effective ways of dealing with the
threat are offered. This principle is in keeping with the Yale group’s
notion that people will accept arguments that benefit them.
Of course,
individuals often avoid messages that make them uncomfortable. This simple fact
must be taken into account when determining a persuasion strategy. For example,
a strong fear appeal on television is not very effective. The message is there
only by our consent; we can always change the channel. This is why the American
Cancer Society’s most effective antismoking commercial
involved a cartoon character named
“Johnny Smoke,” a long, tall cowboy cigarette. He was repeatedly asked, as he
blew smoke away from his gun: “Johnny Smoke, how many men did you shoot today?”
That was it: no direct threat, no explicit conclusion about the harm of
smoking. It was low-key, and the audience was allowed to draw their own
conclusions.
Despite
evidence that high-fear messages tend to repulse people, fear appeals are
widely used in health education, politics, and advertising. The assumption is
that making people afraid persuades them to stop smoking or to vote for a
certain candidate or to buy a particular product (Gleicher & Petty, 1992).
Does fear work? Sometimes it does.
In one
study of the effect of low versus high fear, Gleicher and Petty (1992) had
students at Ohio State University listen to one of four different simulated
radio news stories about crime on campus. The broadcasts were either moderate
in fear (crime was presented as a serious problem) or only mildly fearful
(crime was not presented as a serious problem). Besides manipulating fear, the
researchers varied whether the appeals had a clear assurance that something
could be done about crime (a crime-watch program) or that little could be done
(i.e., the crime-watch programs do not work). The researchers also varied the
strength of the arguments; some participants heard strong arguments, and others
heard weak ones. In other words, some participants heard powerful arguments in
favor of the crime-watch program whereas others heard powerful arguments that
showed that crime-watch programs did not work. In the weak argument condition,
some participants heard not very good arguments in favor of crime-watch
programs whereas others heard equally weak arguments against the effectiveness
of crime-watch programs. In all these variations of the persuasive message, the
speaker was the same person with the same highly credible background.
The
researchers found that under low fear conditions, strong persuasive arguments
produced more attitude change than weak arguments, regardless of whether the
programs were expected to be effective. In other words, if crime did not appear
to be a crisis situation, students were not overly upset about the message or
the possible outcome (effectiveness of the crime-watch program) and were simply
persuaded by the strength of the arguments.
However,
people who heard moderately fearful broadcasts focused on solutions to the
crime problem. When there was a clear expectation that something could be done
about crime on campus, weak and strong arguments were equally persuasive. If
students were confident of a favorable outcome, they worried no further and did
not thoroughly analyze the messages. But when the effectiveness of
crime-fighting programs was in question, students did discriminate between
strong and weak arguments. In other words, when there was no clear assurance
that something effective could be done, fear motivated the participants to
carefully examine the messages, so they tended to be persuaded by strong
arguments. Again, concern for the outcome made them evaluate the messages
carefully.
What we
know from the Petty and Gleicher (1992) study is that fear initially motivates
us to find some easily available, reassuring remedy. We will accept an answer
uncritically if it promises us that everything will be okay. But if no such
promise is there, then we have to start to think for ourselves. So, fear in
combination with the lack of a clear and effective solution (a program to fight
crime, in this case) leads us to analyze possible solutions carefully. Note
that Petty and Gleicher were not dealing with really high fear. Ethical
considerations prevent researchers from creating such a situation in the
laboratory. It may be that very high fear shuts off all critical thinking for
most of us.
What do we
know, then, about the effectiveness of using fear to persuade? The first point
is that if we do scare people, it is a good idea to give them some reassurance
that they can protect themselves from the threat we have presented. The
protection–motivation explanation of how fear appeals work argues that
intimidation motivates us to think about ways to protect ourselves (Rogers,
1983). We are willing to make the effort to evaluate arguments carefully. But,
in keeping with the cognitive miser strategy, if we don’t
need to analyze the arguments,
we won’t.
What is
the bottom line on the effectiveness of fear appeals? Based on the available
research we can conclude that fear appeals are most effective when four
conditions are met (Pratkanis & Aronson, 1992):
1. The appeal generates relatively high levels
of fear.
2. The appeal offers a specific recommendation
about how to avoid any dire consequences depicted in the appeal.
3. The target of the appeal is provided with an
effective way of avoiding the dire consequences depicted in the appeal.
4. The target of the appeal believes that he or
she can perform the recommended action to avoid the dire consequences.
The
Importance of Timing: Primacy Versus Recency
The
effectiveness of any persuasive attempt hinges on the use of an effective
strategy, including the timing of the message’s delivery. When
is it best to deliver your message? If
you were given the option of presenting your message before or after your
opponent in a debate, which should you choose? Generally, persuasive situations
like these are governed by a law of primacy (Lawson, 1969). That is, the
message presented first has more impact than the message presented second.
However, the law of primacydoes not always hold true. It depends on the
structure of the situation. A primacy effect occurs when the two messages
follow one another closely, and there is a delay between the second message and
the audience response or assessment. In this situation, the first message has
the greater impact. But when there is a delay between the two messages, and a
response or assessment is made soon after the second message, we see a recency
effect—the second message has a greater impact (Figure 6.3).
The
primacy and recency effects apply most clearly under certain conditions—when
both sides have equally strong arguments and when listeners are reasonably
motivated to understand them. If one side has a much stronger argument than the
other side, listeners are likely to be persuaded by the strong argument,
regardless of whether it is presented first or last (Haugtvedt & Wegener,
1993). When listeners are very motivated, very interested in the issue, they
are more likely to be influenced by the first argument (the primacy effect)
than by those they hear later on (Haugtvedt & Wegener, 1993).
Figure
6.3Conditions that favor either a primacy effect (top) of recency effect
(bottom). Primacy or recency depends on when a delay is introduced.
Fitting
the Message to the Audience
The Yale
group also was interested in the construction and presentation of persuasive
messages. One of their findings was that messages have to be presented
differently to different audiences. For example, an educated or highly involved
audience requires a different type of persuasive message than an uneducated or
uninvolved audience. Rational arguments are effective with educated or
analytical audiences (Cacioppo, Petty, & Morris, 1983). Emotional appeals
work better with less educated or less analytical groups.
One-Sided
Versus Two-Sided Messages
The nature
of the audience also influences how a message is structured. For less educated,
uninformed audiences, a one-sided message works best. In a one-sided message
you present only your side of the issue and draw conclusions for the audience.
For a well-educated, well-informed audience, a two-sided message works best.
The more educated audience probably is already aware of the other side of the
argument. If you attempt to persuade them with a one-sided argument, they may
question your motives. Also, well-educated audience members can draw their own
conclusions. They probably would resent your drawing conclusions for them.
Thus, a more educated audience will be more persuaded by a two-sided argument
(Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953).
One-sided
and two-sided appeals also have different effects depending on the initial
attitudes of the audience. Generally, a one-sided message is effective when the
audience already agrees with your position. If the audience is against your
position, a twosided message works best. You need to consider both the initial
position of audience members and their education level when deciding on an
approach. A two-sided appeal is best when your audience is educated, regardless
of their initial position. A one-sided appeal works best on an uneducated
audience that already agrees with you.
Inoculating
the Audience
When
presenting a two-sided message, you don’t want to accidentally persuade the audience of the other side.
Therefore, the best approach is to present that side in a weakened form to
“inoculate” the audience against it (McGuire, 1985). When you present a
weakened message, listeners will devise their own counterarguments: “Well, that’s
obviously not true! Any
fool can see through that argument! Who do they think they’re
kidding?” The listeners
convince themselves that the argument is wrong. Inoculation theoryis based on
the medical model of inoculation. People are given a weakened version of a
bacterium or a virus so that they can develop the antibodies to fight the
disease on their own. Similarly, in attempting to persuade people of your side,
you give them a weakened version of the opposing argument and let them develop
their own defenses against it.
In a study
of the inoculation effect, McGuire and Papageorgis (1961) exposed participants
to an attack on their belief that brushing their teeth prevented tooth decay.
Obviously, everybody believes that brushing your teeth is beneficial. This is a
cultural truism, something we all accept without thinking or questioning.
Therefore, we may not have any defenses in place if someone challenges those
truisms.
Participants
in one group heard an attack on the tooth-brushing truism. A second group
received a supportive defense that reinforced the concept that brushing your
teeth is good for you. A third group was inoculated, first hearing a mild
attack on the truism and then hearing a defense of tooth brushing. A fourth
group, the control group, received no messages. Of the three groups who heard a
message, the “inoculated” group was most likely to believe tooth brushing was
beneficial (Figure 6.4). In fact, people in the inoculated group, who were
given a mild rebuttal of the truism, were more likely to believe in the
benefits of tooth brushing than were the people who heard only a supportive
defense of the truism.
Figure
6.4The inoculation effect. A persuasive attack on a truism caused a decrease in
the belief of the validity of the truism unless participants were first
“inoculated” with a weakened form of the persuasive message before receiving
the attack message
Why does
inoculation work? The study just reviewed suggests that inoculation motivates
people to generate their own counterarguments and makes them more likely to
believe the persuader’s side of the issue. In this case,
forewarned is truly forearmed. Inoculation
also appears to operate by increasing attitude accessibility, or the ease with
which a person can call an attitude to mind (Pfau et al, 2003). According to
Pfau et al., inoculation works by making an attitude more accessible, which
increases the strength of that attitude and its resistance to change.
The Role
of Discrepancy
Another
aspect of the audience a persuader has to consider is their preexisting
attitudes in relation to the message the persuader wants to convey. For
instance, imagine you are going to deliver a pro-choice message to a roomful of
people with strong attitudes against abortion. Obviously, your message will be
very different from the preexisting attitudes of your audience. This is a
high-discrepancy situation. On the other hand, if you are trying to convince a
roomful of pro-choice individuals, your message will not be very different from
preexisting attitudes. This is an example of low discrepancy. In either of
these cases, you would not expect much persuasion. In the first case, your
message is too discrepant from the one your audience already holds; they will
reject your message without giving it much thought. In the second case, you are
basically saying what your audience already believes, so there won’t
be much persuasive effect or attitude change. Generally, a moderate amount of discrepancy
produces the greatest amount of change.
Discrepancy
interacts with the characteristics of the communicator. A highly credible
communicator can induce change even when a highly discrepant message—one we
ordinarily would reject or that contradicts a stereotype—is delivered. In one
study, researchers found that Scottish participants had definite stereotypes of
male hairdressers and of “skinheads” (Macrae, Shepherd, & Milne, 1992).
Male hairdressers were perceived as meek, and skinheads were perceived as
aggressive. However, a report from a psychiatrist that stated the contrary—that
a particular hairdresser was aggressive or a skinhead was meek—altered the
participants’opinions of those two groups. Of course, a credible communicator cannot say
just anything and expect people to believe it. An effective communicator must
be aware of the audience’s likely perception of the message. Clarence Darrow carefully staked out
a position he knew the judge would not reject. He didn’t
argue that the death penalty should be abolished, because he knew that the judge would not accept that
position. Rather, he argued that the penalty was not appropriate in this
specific case because of the defendants’ages and their mental state:
And, I
submit, Your Honor, that by every law of humanity, by every law of justice. . .
. Your Honor should say that because of the condition of these boys’minds,
it would be monstrous
to visit upon them the vengeance that is asked by the State. (Weinberg, 1957,
p. 163)
In other
words, even highly credible communicators have to keep in mind how discrepant
their message is from the audience’s views. For communicators with lower credibility, a moderate amount of
discrepancy works best.
Social
Judgment Theory How does discrepancy work? Sherif suggested that audience
members make social judgments about the difference between the communicator’s
position and their own
attitude on an issue (Sherif & Hovland, 1961; Sherif, Sherif, &
Nebergall, 1965). This social judgment theoryargues that the degree of personal
involvement in an issue determines how the target will evaluate an attempt at
persuasion.
Sherif
suggested that an individual’s perception of a message falls into one
of three judgment
categories, or latitudes. The latitude of acceptance is the set of positions
the audience would find acceptable. The latitude of rejectionis the set of
arguments the audience would not accept. The latitude of noncommitment is a
neutral zone falling between the other two and including positions audience
members do not accept or reject but will consider.
The
breadth of the latitudes is affected by how strongly the person feels about the
issue, how ego-involved he or she is. As Figure 6.5 shows, as involvement
increases, the latitudes of acceptance and noncommitment narrow, but the
latitude of rejection increases (Eagly & Telaak, 1972). In other words, the
more important an issue is, the less likely you are to accept a persuasive
message unless it is similar to your position. Only messages that fall within
your latitude of acceptance, or perhaps within your latitude of noncommitment,
will have a chance of persuading you. As importance of an issue increases, the
number of acceptable arguments decreases. Sherif measured the attitudes of
Republicans and Democrats in a presidential election and found that very
committed Republicans and very committed Democrats rejected almost all of the
other side’s arguments (Sherif et
al., 1965). However, voters who were less extreme in their commitment were open
to persuasion. Moderates of both parties usually accepted as many arguments
from the opposition as they rejected. Therefore, as Darrow knew, a persuasive
message must fall at least within the audience’s latitude of
noncommitment to be accepted.
The
Problem of Multiple Audiences
On January
23, 1968, the USS Pueblowas stationed in international waters off the cost of
North Korea. The Pueblowas a “spy ship” and was gathering intelligence about
North Korea. On the morning of January 23, a North Korean subchaser S0-1
approached the Puebloat high speed. At the same time three North Korean torpedo
boats were approaching. Eventually, the North Korean ships fired upon the
Puebloand eventually boarded her. One member of the Pueblocrew was killed and
82 were taken prisoner and held in North Korea. While in captivity, the crew
members were beaten, tortured, and starved. The North Koreans wanted them to
confess that they were actually in North Korean waters running the spy
operation. Propaganda photographs were taken of the crew and were widely
distributed. Movies were taken of the crew in staged situations that made crew
members appear as though they were cooperating. Some members of the crew
decided to send a message home indicating that they were being forced to say
and do things. In one example of this, some crew members clearly displayed the
“Hawaiian good luck sign” (a.k.a., the finger) against their faces or against
their legs. Captain Bucher read statements using a monotone voice so that he
sounded drugged.
Figure
6.5The effect of involvement with an issue on the size of the latitudes of
rejection and acceptance in social judgment theory. High involvement leads to
an increased latitude of rejection and a related decreased latitude of
acceptance.
The
dilemma facing the crew of the Pueblowas to send two messages to two different
audiences. On the one hand, they had to placate their captors by appearing to
cooperate. On the other hand, they wanted to communicate to the American public
and their families and friends that they did not subscribe to what they were
being forced to do and say. This is the multiple audience problem—how to send
different meanings in the same message to diverse audiences (Fleming, Darley,
Hilton, & Kojetin, 1990; Van Boven, Kruger, Savitsky, & Gilovich,
2000).
How do
people manage these difficult situations? Researchers interested in this
question had communicators send messages to audiences composed of friends and
strangers (Fleming et al., 1990). The communicators were motivated to send a
message that would convey the truth to their friends but deceive the strangers.
Participants in this experiment were quite accurate at figuring out when their
friends were lying. Strangers were not so accurate. Recall the fundamental
attribution error and the correspondence bias from Chapter 3: We tend to
believe that people mean what they say. In general, we are not very good at
detecting lies (Ekman, 1985).
Friends
also were able to pick up on the communicator’s hidden message,
because they shared
some common knowledge. For example, one communicator said she was going to go
to Wales, a country her friends knew she loved, and was going to do her
shopping for the trip in a department store her friends knew she hated. The
message was clear to those in the know: She is lying. The department store
reference was a private keythat close friends understood. This is the way
communicators can convey different meanings in the same message. They use
special, private keys that only one audience understands. We often see private
keys used in political ads, especially those ads aimed at evoking stereotypes
and emotional responses. Another instance of the multiple-audience problem is
when you have to maintain different personas to different people at the same
time. For example, if your boss and a potential dating partner are attending a
party you are attending, you probably want to project a “professional” persona
to your boss and a more “fun-loving” persona to your dating interest. Can we
pull this off? Can we, in fact, maintain vastly different personas at the same
time and be successful in communicating them to the appropriate target, while
concealing the other persona from the person we don’t
want to see it? The answer
appears to be that we can.
In one
experiment, Van Boven, Kruger, Savitsky, and Gilovich (2000) had participants
project a “party animal” persona to one observer during an interaction session.
The same participant then projected a “serious studious” persona to a second
observer. In a third interaction session, the participant interacted with both
observers simultaneously. The task facing the participant was to maintain the
correct persona with the correct observer at the same time. The results showed
that the participants were quite successful at the task. In fact, the participants
tended to be overconfident in their ability to successfully project the two
personas to the appropriate observers.
The
Cognitive Approach to Persuasion
You may
have noted that in the Yale model of persuasion the audience seems to be
nothing more than a target for messages. People just sit there and take it,
either accepting the message or not. Cognitive response approaches, on the
other hand, emphasize the active participation of the audience (Greenwald,
1968). The cognitive approach looks at why people react to a message the way
they do, why they say that a message is interesting or that a communicator is
biased.
Cognitively
oriented social psychologists emphasize that a persuasive communication may
trigger a number of related experiences, memories, feelings, and thoughts that
individuals use to process the message. Therefore, both what a person thinks
about when she hears the persuasive message and how the person applies those
thoughts, feelings, and memories to analyzing the message are critical. We now
turn to the individual’s cognitive
response to the persuasive message.
The
Elaboration Likelihood Model
One
well-known cognitive response model is the elaboration likelihood model (ELM).
This model, first proposed by Petty and Cacioppo (1986), makes clear that
audiences are not just passive receptacles but are actively involved in the
persuasion process. Their attention, involvement, distraction, motivation,
self-esteem, education, and intelligence determine the success of persuasive
appeals. The elaboration likelihood model owes a lot to the Yale model,
incorporating much of the Yale research on the important roles of communicator
and message. But its primary emphasis is on the role of the audience,
especially their emotions and motivations. According to ELM, two routes to
persuasion exist: a central processing route and a peripheral processing route.
Persuasion may be achieved via either of these routes.
Central
Route Processing
Central
route processinginvolves elaboration of the message by the listener. This type
of processing usually occurs when the person finds the message personally
relevant and has preexisting ideas and beliefs about the topic. The individual
uses these ideas and beliefs to create a context for the message, expanding and
elaborating on the new information. Because the message is relevant, the person
is motivated to listen to it carefully and process it in an effortful manner.
A juror
listening to evidence that she understands and finds interesting, for example,
will generate a number of ideas and responses. As she assimilates the message,
she will compare it to what she already knows and believes. In the Leopold and
Loeb trial, Judge Caverly may have elaborated on Darrow’s
argument for life imprisonment by recalling
that in the Chicago courts, no one had been sentenced to death after
voluntarily entering a guilty plea, and no one as young as the defendants had
ever been hanged.
Elaboration
of a message does not always lead to acceptance, however. If the message does
not make sense or does not fit the person’s knowledge and beliefs, elaboration may lead to rejection. For example,
Judge Caverly might have focused on the brutal and indifferent attitude that
Leopold and Loeb displayed toward Bobby Franks. If Darrow had not put together
a coherent argument that fit the evidence, the judge probably would have
rejected his argument. But the story Darrow told was coherent. By emphasizing
the “diseased minds” of his clients, enhanced by the suggestion that they
probably were born “twisted,” he explained the unexplainable: why they killed
Bobby Franks. At the same time, he made Leopold and Loeb seem less responsible.
Thus, Darrow presented the judge with credible explanations on which he could
expand to reach a verdict.
Central
route processors elaborate on the message by filling in the gaps with their own
knowledge and beliefs. Messages processed this way are more firmly tied to
other attitudes and are therefore more resistant to change. Attitude change
that results from central route processing is stable, long-lasting, and
difficult to reverse.
Peripheral
Route Processing
What if
the listener is not motivated, is not able to understand the message, or simply
does not like to deal with new or complex information? In these incidences, the
listener takes another route to persuasion, a peripheral route. In peripheral
route processing, listeners rely on something other than the message to make
their decisions; they are persuaded by cues peripheral or marginal to the
message. A juror may be favorably influenced by the appearance of the
defendant, for example. Or perhaps he or she remembers when his or her uncle
was in a similar predicament and thinks, “He wasn’t guilty either.”Emotional cues are very effective in
persuading peripheral route processors (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). Recall the
experiment on the effects of fear appeals in campus crime newscasts: A strong
emotional appeal offering a reassuring solution was accepted regardless of
whether the argument itself was strong or weak. Participants were not
processing centrally; they paid no attention to the quality of the argument.
They simply wanted reassurance, and the existence of a possible solution acted
as a peripheral cue, convincing them that the argument must be valid. High or
moderate fear makes us accept whatever reassuring solution is presented to us.
Familiar
phrases or clichƩs included in persuasive messages can serve as peripheral cues
to persuasion (Howard, 1997). Howard compared familiar (don’t
put all of your eggs in
one basket) and literal (don’t risk everything on a single venture)
phrases for their ability
to persuade via the peripheral route. Howard found that familiar phrases
produced more persuasion under conditions of low attitude involvement
(peripheral route) than under high involvement (central route). The familiar
phrases were also more effective than the literal phrases when the individual
was distracted from the message and when the target of the persuasive
communication was low in the need for cognition.
Peripheral
route processing often leads to attitude change, but because the listener has
not elaborated on the message, the change is not very stable and is vulnerable
to counter-pressures (Kassin, Reddy, & Tulloch, 1990). A juror who
processes centrally will be firm in his or her conclusions about the evidence,
but a peripheral route juror will be an easy target for the next persuader in
the courtroom (ForsterLee, Horowitz, & Bourgeois, 1993).
Although
we have distinguished between the central and peripheral routes, message
processing is not an either/or proposition. In fact, you may process some parts
of a message centrally, others peripherally. For example, a juror may be
interested in and understand the scientific evidence presented at trial and
process that information centrally. However, when an economist takes the stand,
the juror may be bored or may think that people in bow ties are untrustworthy,
and then process that testimony peripherally.
The Effect
of Mood on Processing
Many
speakers try to put their audience in a good mood before making their case.
They tell a joke or an amusing story, or they say something designed to make
listeners feel positive. Is this a good strategy? Does it make an argument more
persuasive? It depends. When people are in a good mood, they tend to be
distracted. Good moods bring out many related pleasant feelings and memories.
Everything seems rosy. People in good moods cannot concentrate very well on
messages; they cannot process information centrally. In one study on the
influence of mood, people were put in either a good or a neutral mood and were
given either an unlimited or very limited amount of time to listen to a message
(Mackie & Worth, 1989). The strength of the persuasive messages also
varied: One message contained strong arguments; the other, only weak arguments.
The researchers reasoned that for the participants in good moods, strong and
weak arguments would be equally effective. As shown in Figure 6.6, this was
found to be the case, but only when there was a limited amount of time to study
the messages. People in good moods did not distinguish between strong and weak
arguments because they were not processing centrally.
Figure
6.6The effect of mood and processing time on the impact of a persuasive
message. When people are in a good mood and have limited time to process the
message, there is no effect of argument strength. Given unlimited time,
participants are more persuaded by the strong argument. In a neutral mood,
participants are more persuaded by strong arguments than weak arguments,
regardless of time limitation.
Good
feelings do not, however, always prevent central processing. If people in good
moods are motivated to carefully evaluate and elaborate on a message, and if
they have enough time, they will process centrally. A good mood will not have a
direct effect on their attitudes, but it may make them think more positive
thoughts about the message, if it is a strong one and they have time to
consider it (Petty, Schumann, Richman, & Strathman, 1993). The good thoughts
then lead to positive attitude change. For those using peripheral route
processing, good moods don’t lead to more positive thoughts and then
to positive attitude
change. These people aren’t thinking about the message at all and
are not elaborating on it. Instead, for them, good mood leads directly to attitude change.
Mood can
act as a resource, helping us fend off the effects of negative information,
increasing the likelihood that personally relevant negative information will be
processed centrally (Raghunathan & Trope, 2002). According to the
mood-as-aresource hypothesis, a good mood acts as a buffer against the
emotional effects of negative information, allowing us to focus on what we can
learn from the information (Raghunathan & Trope, 2002). Raghunathan and
Trope conducted a series of experiments demonstrating this effect. In one
experiment, for example, participants (high- or lowcaffeine consumers) were
induced into either a good or bad mood. They were then exposed to personally
relevant negative information about caffeine consumption. The results showed
that participants who consumed larger amounts of caffeine recalled more of the
negative information about caffeine when in a good mood than when in a bad mood
(there was no such effect for participants who consumed low amounts of
caffeine). In a second experiment, the researchers found that the negative
information about caffeine led to more persuasion among high-caffeine consumers
when they were in a good mood.
Figure 6.7
shows how good mood affects central and peripheral processors differently.
Thus, the relationship between potentially biasing factors in persuasion, such
as mood or likability of the communicator, is a complex one. Variables that
bias the persuasion process still operate when an individual is motivated to
process a message centrally (Petty, Wegener, & White, 1998). Petty and
Wegener (1993) proposed the flexible correction model (FCM) to help us
understand how biasing variables influence the persuasion process. According to
the FCM, individuals using central route processing (highly motivated) are
influenced by biasing variables because they are not aware of the potential
impact of the biasing variable (e.g., mood) during a persuasion situation
(Petty et al., 1998). Furthermore, correction for biasing conditions, according
to the FCM, should take place under the following impact of the biasing
conditions (p. 95):
1. When an individual is motivated to search for
biasing variables.
2. When an individual finds sources of potential
bias after a search.
3. When an individual generates ideas or
theories about the nature of the bias.
4. When an individual is motivated and has the
ability to make a correction for the biasing variable.
Figure
6.7The effect of mood on central or peripheral route processing. When using
central route processing, a good mood leads to the generation of positive
thoughts, which affects attitudes. When using peripheral route processing, a
good mood directly affects attitudes, bypassing the generation of positive
thoughts.
In two
experiments, Petty et al. (1998) tested the assumptions made by the FCM. In
their first experiment, Petty and colleagues varied the likability of the
source of a message (likable and unlikable) along with whether participants
received an instruction to correct for the likability information. Petty and
colleagues found that when no correction instruction was given, the likable
source led to attitude change in the direction of the position advocated in a
persuasive message (positive attitude change), whereas the unlikable source led
to attitude change in the opposite direction (negative attitude change). This
is the usual finding when such variables are manipulated. However, when
participants were given an instruction to correct for the likability of the
source, the results were just the opposite. The unlikable source produced
positive attitude change, whereas the likable source produced negative attitude
change. Additionally, there was greater correction for the unlikable source
than the likable source.
In their
second experiment, Petty and colleagues added a third variable: whether
participants used high- or low-elaboration strategies. When participants used
low-elaboration strategies and no correction instruction was given, the likable
source yielded more persuasion than the unlikable source. However, when a
correction instruction was given, the likable and unlikable sources were
equally persuasive. The opposite occurred under high-elaboration strategies.
Here, in the no-correction condition, the likable and unlikable sources
produced the same levels of persuasion, whereas when the correction instruction
was given, the unlikable source produced more attitude change than the likable
source.
The
results of both studies suggest that when individuals become aware of a biasing
factor (likability or mood), they will be motivated to correct for the biasing
factor under high- or low-elaboration conditions. Thus, when individuals become
aware of such biasing factors, they may not influence persuasion more when peripheral
route processing is used. Additionally, such factors may not bias the
processing of information relevant to the issue contained in a persuasive
message when central route processing is used (Petty et al., 1998). It appears
as though the mechanisms for correction for biasing factors operate
independently from the mechanisms for processing the content of the message
(Petty et al., 1998).
Figure
6.8The effects of audience involvement, expertise of source, and strength of
arguments.
The Effect
of Personal Relevance on Processing
Another
factor affecting central versus peripheral route processing is personal
relevance. If an issue is important to us and affects our well-being, we are
more likely to pay attention to the quality of the message. In one study,
college students were told that the university chancellor wanted to have all
seniors pass a comprehensive examination before they could graduate (Petty,
Cacioppo, & Goldman, 1981). Participants hearing the high-relevance version
of this message were told the policy would go into effect the following year
and, consequently, would affect them. Participants hearing the low-relevance
version were informed that the policy wouldn’t be implemented
for several years and
therefore would not affect them.
The researchers
also varied the quality of the arguments and the expertise of the communicator.
Half the participants heard persuasive arguments, and the other half heard
weaker arguments. Half were told that the plan was based on a report by a local
high school class (low communicator expertise), and the other half were told
the source was the Carnegie Commission on Higher Education (high expertise).
Results
indicated that relevance did influence the type of processing participants used
(Figure 6.8). Students who thought the change would affect them were persuaded
by the strong argument and not by the weak one. In other words, they carefully
examined the arguments, using central processing. Students who thought the
change wouldn’t affect
them simply relied on the expertise of the communicator. They were persuaded
when they thought the plan was based on the Carnegie Commission report,
regardless of whether the arguments were strong or weak. Low relevance, in
other words, enhances the influence of communicator credibility and increases
the likelihood that listeners will use peripheral processing.
Does high
relevance mean that you always will be persuaded by strong and rational
arguments? Not at all. An issue may be highly relevant to you because it
involves an important personal value. In this case, even a very persuasive
argument probably won’t change
your opinion. In the current abortion debate, for example, an extreme position
on either side is based on fundamental values relating to privacy, coercion,
and the nature of life. The issue is certainly relevant to individuals with
extreme views, but they are unlikely to be persuaded to change their opinions
by any argument.
If,
however, an issue is highly relevant because of a particular outcome, rather
than a value, then a strong, persuasive argument might work (Johnson &
Eagly, 1989). If you are strongly opposed to taking a senior comprehensive
exam, a persuasive message about the outcome, such as the possibility that
passing the exam would increase your chances of getting into graduate school,
might well convince you to take it.
Finally,
the impact of a personally relevant message on central route processing also
relates to a process called self-affirmation (which we shall discuss in more
detail later in this chapter). In short, self-affirmation means confirming and
maintaining one’s self-image
(Steele, 1988). Self-affirmation may be especially important when personally
relevant information is threatening (Harris & Napper, 2005). According to
Harris and Napper, self-affirmation promotes processing of threatening
information along the central route. Harris and Napper demonstrated this in an
experiment in which collegeage women were exposed to a “health promotion
leaflet” in which a link was made between alcohol consumption and the risk of
breast cancer. Some of the participants wrote an essay describing the values
that were most important to them and how they affected their daily lives
(self-affirmation condition), whereas other participants wrote about their
least important values. Based on their answers on a pre-experimental
questionnaire concerning alcohol consumption, the participants were divided
into two groups: high-risk women and low-risk women. The results showed that
high-risk women who self-affirmed were more likely to accept the content of the
message contained in the health leaflet compared to those who did not
self-affirm. Further, women in this group reported a perception of higher risk
of developing breast cancer, experienced more negative affect while reading the
leaflet, and indicated a greater intention to reduce their alcohol consumption.
Interestingly, these effects endured over a period of weeks. Thus,
self-affirmation can enhance central processing of a threatening, personally
relevant message (Harris & Napper, 2005) and better judge the merits of the
threatening message (Correll, Spencer, & Zanna, 2004).
The Impact
of Attitude Accessibility on Elaboration
In
addition to the relevance of a persuasive message to an individual, processing
of a persuasive message is also influenced by attitude accessibility. Attitude
accessibility refers to the ease with which an attitude can be automatically
activated when the correspondent attitude object is encountered (Fabrigar,
Priester, Petty, & Wegener, 1998).
Attitude
accessibility is one dimension along which the strength of an attitude can be
measured. Highly accessible attitudes tend to be stronger than less accessible
attitudes. Fabrigar and colleagues reasoned that highly accessible attitudes
may enhance message elaboration because attitude-relevant information is more
readily available than with less accessible attitudes.
Fabrigar
and colleagues (1998) conducted two experiments to investigate the role of
attitude accessibility in persuasion. In the first experiment, attitude
accessibility was measured, and participants’attitudes were
classified as low, moderate, or high
in accessibility. The researchers manipulated the quality of the arguments made
within a persuasive message on nuclear power (high or low quality). The results
of experiment 1 confirmed that individuals with high-accessibility attitudes
were more likely to elaborate the persuasive message than those with low
accessibility attitudes. Specifically, argument quality enhanced attitudes
among moderately and highly accessible attitudes but not for low-accessibility
attitudes. This effect was strongest for the individuals with highly accessible
attitudes. Data from the second experiment confirmed the first.
The bottom
line is that attitude accessibility mediates the amount of elaboration that an
individual will display when exposed to a persuasive message. High
accessibility (high attitude strength) is associated with increased examination
of the content of the message (central route processing). When attitude
accessibility is low (a weak attitude), an individual is less likely to
scrutinize the content of the persuasive message carefully.
Do Vivid
Messages Persuade Better Than Nonvivid Messages?
What about
the effect of vividness on persuasion? Does it make a difference in our
attitudes or behavior? Advertisers and other persuaders certainly believe that
vivid messages, presented in eye- or ear-catching terms, are persuasive. Social
psychologists interested in this issue stated, “Everybody knows that vividly presented
information is impactful and persuasive” (Taylor & Thompson, 1982, p. 155).
However, when these researchers surveyed the literature on vividness, they
found very weak support for the persuasive power of vivid materials.
In one
study of the vividness effect, people were given vivid and nonvivid versions of
crime stories in the news (Collins, Taylor, Wood, & Thompson, 1988). The
vivid versions used colorful language and provided bizarre details. People
listened to a vivid or nonvivid story and then rated its quality in terms of
emotion, imagery, interest, and so forth as well as its persuasiveness. In a
second study, people also had to predict how others would respond to the
stories.
The
studies found no evidence of a vividness effect; vivid messages had about the
same persuasive effect as nonvivid messages. However, people believed that
vivid messages affected other people. What influenced the participants if
vividness did not? Interest: If the message involved a topic that interested
them, people felt the message was more effective. Remember the effects of
personal relevance in the elaboration likelihood model of persuasion.
On the
other hand, some messages, such as political ads, appear to benefit from
vividness—perhaps they work because they interest people and force them to pay
more attention than they normally might. One study examined the effects of
vivid language in a trial concerning a dispute between a contractor and a
subcontractor on a building project (Wilson, Northcraft, & Neale, 1989). People
playing the role of jurors watched different videotapes of the trial. One
version had vivid phrasing; the other, nonvivid language (p. 135):
1. There was a spiderweb of cracks through the
slab. (vivid)
There was
a network of cracks through the slab. (nonvivid)
2. The slab was jagged and had to be sanded.
(vivid)
The slab
was rough and had to be sanded. (nonvivid)
The jurors
tended to award the plaintiff more money when they heard vivid phrases. So, is
there a vividness effect or not? Based on the evidence, it seems that vivid
messages have an initial effect, especially if there is little else to compete
with them. In the trial situation, vivid information had a strong impact when
the jurors were presented with a lot of evidence that was not directly important
for their decision, such as a history of the building project and pictures of
the construction site. Then the jurors heard the vivid language (“a spiderweb
of cracks through the slab”). Given the background of irrelevant information,
they were influenced by the one or two vivid messages they heard.
How can we
reconcile the seemingly conflicting results concerning the impact of vividness?
One approach suggests that the impact of vividness depends on the number of
cognitive resources that are devoted to processing a persuasive message
(Meyers-Levy & Peracchio, 1995). According to Meyers-Levy and Peracchio,
the impact of vivid information depends on the degree of correspondence between
the resources a person has available to process a message and the resources
required to adequately process information. Vivid language or illustrations,
according to Myers-Levy and Peracchio, should have the greatest impact when a
persuasive message requires relatively few resources, and a person is highly
motivated to process the message. Conversely, for a highly motivated individual
and a persuasive message that requires high levels of resources, vivid content
should not have a strong impact. If an individual is not highly motivated to
process a message, then vividness will serve as a peripheral cue and have a
significant impact on persuasion.
Myers-Levy
and Peracchio (1995) conducted two experiments to confirm these predicted
relationships. In their first experiment, they found that for highly motivated
individuals, a demanding persuasive message (an advertisement of a bicycle) was
most effective when vividness was low (a black-and-white photo of the bicycle
and model was used). For a less demanding message, a vivid message (a color
advertisement) was more effective. In the second experiment, low-motivation and
highly motivated individuals were included. They found that for low-motivation
individuals, a vivid message was more effective than a less vivid message. For
highly motivated individuals, the impact of vividness (color) depended on the
level of resources needed to process the message (as described earlier). These
results were supported by three experiments by Keller Punam and Block (1997).
Thus, in a
situation in which much information already has been made available (low
demand), or when the audience is particularly interested in the issue, one
vivid message may not have a significant impact. However, when people are not
particularly interested, a vivid message may have significant impact. In other
words, vividness is a peripheral cue. When individuals find the message
interesting and personally relevant, they process centrally, and vividness has
little effect. But when the cognitive miser is at work, a vivid message may
have a definite influence on attitudes.
Need for
Cognition: Some Like to Do It the Hard Way
Some
people prefer central route processing no matter what the situation or how
complex the evidence. These people have a high need for cognition
(NC).According to Cacioppo, Petty, and Morris (1983), high-NC people like to
deal with difficult and effortful problems. On a scale assessing this cognitive
characteristic, they agree with such statements as, “I really enjoy a task that
invokes coming up with new solutions to problems,” and they disagree with such
statements as, “I only think as hard as I have to.”
High-NC
people are concerned with the validity of the messages they receive, which
suggests that they rely mainly on central route processing (Cacioppo et al.,
1983). High-NC individuals also organize information in a way that allows them
to remember messages and use them later (Lassiter, Briggs, & Bowman, 1991).
Those low in need for cognition tend to pay more attention to the physical
characteristics of the speaker, indicating peripheral processing (Petty & Cacioppo,
1986).
High-NC
individuals are also better able to distinguish the authenticity on persuasive
information than low-NC individuals (Engleberg & Sjƶberg, 2005). Engleberg
and Sjƶberg showed high- and low-NC individuals films about the risks of nuclear
energy. One of the films was the fictional movie The China Syndrome,whereas the
other was the film Chernobyl: The Final Warningbased on a book written by a
bone marrow specialist. Engleberg and Sjƶberg found that high-NC individuals
were more likely to identify Chernobyl as an event that actually happened than
low-NC individuals. Interestingly, however, both high- and low-NC individuals
assessed the risks of nuclear energy at the same levels, regardless of the film
they had seen.
Research
also shows that high-NC individuals are less likely to switch away from a
course of action that has a disappointing outcome than are low-NC individuals
(Ratner & Herbst, 2005). Ratner and Herbst report that people tend to shift
away from a disappointing strategy because of emotional reactions, rather than
focusing on more cognitively based beliefs. Those high in the need for
cognition can apparently stay better focused on the cognitive aspects and not
be ruled by emotional reactions.
Elaboration
likelihood model research shows that people who have a need to process
information centrally—high-NC people—accept and resist persuasive arguments in
a different way than those low in need for cognition. Because they are
processing centrally, they elaborate on the messages they hear. They are
influenced by the qualities of the argument or the product advertised rather
than by peripheral cues (Haugtvedt, Petty, & Cacioppo, 1992). Conversely,
low-NC people are more likely to focus on the peripheral aspects of information
or an advertisement (Sicilia, Ruiz, & Munuera, 2005). Finally, high-NC
individuals hold newly formed attitudes longer and are more resistant to
counterpersuasion (Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992).
The
Heuristic Model of Persuasion
A second
cognitive model of persuasion is the heuristic and systematic
informationprocessing model (HSM). Proposed by Chaiken (1987), the HSM has much
in common with the ELM. As in the ELM, there are two routes for information
processing: the systematic and the heuristic. Systematic processing in the HSM
is essentially the same as central processing in the ELM, and heuristic
processing is the same as peripheral processing. Heuristics, as you recall from
Chapter 3, are simple guides or shortcuts that people use to make decisions
when something gets too complicated or when they are just too lazy to process
systematically.
The main
difference between the two theories lies in the claim of the HSM that reliance
on heuristics is more common than is usually thought (Chaiken, Liberman, &
Eagly, 1989). If motivation and ability to comprehend are not high, individuals
rely on heuristics most of the time. Some of these heuristics might be:
“Experts can be trusted.” “The majority must be right.” “She’s
from the Midwest; she must be trustworthy.” “If it was on the evening news, it must be true.”
Heuristic
processing can be compared to scanning newspaper headlines. The information you
receive is minimal, and the truth or relevance of the headline will be
determined by those simple rules. “Congress Cannot Agree on a Budget,” reads
the headline. Your response would be to quickly check the available heuristics
that might explain the headline. Here it is: “Politicians are incompetent.”
Next headline, please. The HSM suggests that people are more likely to agree
with communicators who are expert and with messages with which most people
agree. Again we see the cognitive miser at work.
Cognitive Dissonance Theory: A Model of Self-Persuasion
Direct
persuasion by a communicator is not the only route to attitude or behavior change.
Attitude change may also occur if we find our existing attitudes in conflict
with new information, or if our behavior is inconsistent with our beliefs.
Festinger (1957) observed that people try to appear consistent. When we act
counter to what we believe or think, we must justify the inconsistency. In
other words, if we say one thing and do something else, we need a good reason.
Usually, we persuade ourselves that we have a good reason, even if it means
changing our previous attitudes. Inconsistency is thus one of the principal
motivations for attitude change.
Cognitive
Dissonance Theory
Festinger’s
cognitive dissonance theory proposed that if inconsistency exists among our attitudes, or between our
attitudes and our behavior, we experience an unpleasant state of arousal called
cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1957). The arousal of dissonance motivates us
to change something, our attitudes or our behavior, to reduce or eliminate the
unpleasant arousal. Reducing the tension helps us achieve consonance, a state
of psychological balance.
Cognitive
dissonance theory is like homeostatic theory in biology. Consider what happens
when you are hungry: Your brain detects an imbalance in your blood sugar
levels, causing a physiological state of hunger. You are motivated to reduce
this unpleasant state of arousal by finding and consuming food. Similarly, when
cognitive consonance is disrupted, you feel tension and are motivated to reduce
it.
The five
key assumptions of cognitive dissonance theory can be summarized as follows:
1. Attitudes and behavior can stand in a
consonant (consistent) or a dissonant (inconsistent) relationship with one
another.
2. Inconsistency between attitudes and behavior
gives rise to a negative motivational state known as cognitive dissonance.
3. Because cognitive dissonance is an
uncomfortable state, people are motivated to reduce the dissonance.
4. The greater the amount of dissonance, the
stronger the motivation to reduce it.
5. Dissonance may be reduced by rationalizing
away the inconsistency or by changing an attitude or a behavior.
How Does
Cognitive Dissonance Lead to Attitude Change?
Exactly
how does cognitive dissonance change attitudes? To find out, imagine that you
have volunteered to be a participant in a social psychological experiment. You
are instructed to sit in front of a tray of objects and repeatedly empty and
refill the tray for the next hour. Then, to add more excitement to your day,
you are asked to turn pegs in holes a little at a time. When your tasks are
over, you are asked to tell the next participant how interesting and delightful
your tasks were. For doing this, you are paid the grand sum of $1. Unbeknownst
to you, other participants go through the same experience and also are asked to
tell an incoming participant how interesting the tasks are, but each is paid
$20.
When this
classic experiment was done in 1959, almost all the participants agreed to
misrepresent how much fun the experiment was (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959).
Several weeks later, the participants were contacted by a third party and asked
whether they had enjoyed the study. Their responses turned out to depend on how
much money they had been paid. You might predict that the participants who got
$20 said that they enjoyed their experience more than those who got only $1.
Well, that’s not what happened. Participants paid $20 said the tasks were
boring, and those paid $1 said they had
enjoyed the tasks. A third group, the control participants, were given no
reward and were not told that anyone else had received one. Like the $20 group,
they said the tasks were boring.
Cognitive
dissonance theory argues that change occurs when people experience dissonance.
Where is the dissonance in this experiment? Being paid $1, a trifling sum even
in 1959, was surely insufficient justification for lying. If a $1 participant
analyzed the situation logically, it would look like this: “I lied to someone
because the experimenter asked me to, and I got paid only a buck.” Conclusion:
“Either I am a liar or I am stupid.” Neither conclusion fits with what we
generally think of ourselves. The dissonance is between what we want to think
of ourselves and how we have behaved. So, how does the participant resolve the
dissonance? The behavior can’t be undone, so the participant engages in
self-persuasion: “I’m not a liar or stupid, so I must have
meant what I said. I
enjoyed the experiment.” The $20-participant has an easily available, if not
very flattering, justification for the lie: “I needed the money.”
The
Reverse Incentive Effect
The
implications of this study and many more that have replicated the effect over
the years are intriguing. One concept that came from the original study is the
reverse-incentive effect: When people are given a large payment for doing
something, they infer that the activity must be difficult, tedious, or risky
(Freedman, Cunningham, & Krismer, 1992). Thus, professional athletes who
once played the game just for fun may now moan about playing the game for $5
million a year. People seem to get suspicious when they are paid large sums for
doing something they enjoyed doing in the first place. They feel a little
apprehensive and develop a less positive view of the activity (Crano &
Sivacek, 1984).
Dissonance
theory argues, then, that the less the reward or the less the threatened
punishment used to make people behave counter to their attitudes, the more
people have to provide their own justifications for their behavior. The more
they have to persuade themselves of the rightness of the behavior, the more
their attitude is likely to change.
The
Importance of Free Choice
An
important condition in the arousal of dissonance is whether behavior is freely
chosen or coerced. In another study of cognitive dissonance, participants were
asked to write an essay arguing a position that ran counter to their real
beliefs (Elkin & Leippe, 1986). Furthermore, they did this
attitude-inconsistent act when they felt they had freely chosen it. Dissonance
theorists call this situation induced compliance. The researchers found that
when participants wrote an essay counter to their beliefs, they showed greater
physiological arousal than if they had written an essay consistent with their
beliefs. This finding is compatible with predictions from cognitive dissonance
theory, specifically that dissonance increases feelings of tension
(physiological arousal).
This study
reinforced the finding that people do not experience dissonance if they do not
choose the inconsistent behavior (Brehm & Cohen, 1962). If they are forced
to do something, the coercion is a sufficient external justification for the
attitude- discrepant
actions. If they don’t have to justify their behavior to
themselves, there is no self-persuasion.
This suggests that attribution processes may play a role in mediating
dissonance arousal and reduction. We explore this possibility later in this
chapter.
Postdecision
Dissonance
Free
choice relates to dissonance in another way when you have to choose between two
mutually exclusive, equally attractive, but different alternatives (e.g.,
between two cars or two jobs). After a choice is made, dissonance is
experienced. It is important to note that postdecision dissonance is not the
same as predecision conflict, where you vacillate between the two alternatives.
Postdecision dissonancecomes after your decision. Here is how it works: Let’s
say you have enough money to buy a car. There are two cars you are considering that are
equally attractive to you. For each car, there is a set of positive cognitions.
Once you have made your choice (let’s say you picked car 1), all the positive cognitions associated
with your chosen alternative are consistent with your choice. However, all the
positive cognitions associated with the unchosen alternative are now
inconsistent with your choice. Dissonance theory predicts that you will take
steps to reduce the dissonance associated with the unchosen alternative. One
way to reduce dissonance would be to change your decision (that is, choose car
2). Of course, this won’t work, because now all of the cognitions
associated with car 1 are inconsistent with your new decision, and the
dissonance remains. More likely, you will begin to think of negative things about the
unchosen car to reduce dissonance. For example, you may reason that the
insurance costs would be higher, the color isn’t exactly what you
wanted, and the
warranty is not as good. At the same time, you may also think of more positive
things about the chosen car. For example, you may point out how comfortable the
seats are, how good the stereo sounds, and how the color fits you perfectly.
The
arousal of postdecision dissonance and its subsequent reduction was
demonstrated in a classic experiment by Brehm (1956). In this experiment,
female participants first rated the desirability of several household products (e.g.,
a toaster). Brehm then offered the women one of the two products they had rated
very closely or they had rated very differently. After the women made their
choices, they again rated the products. Brehm found that when the two choice
alternatives were close in desirability (a difficult decision), ratings of the
chosen alternative became more positive, compared to the original ratings. At
the same time, the ratings of the unchosen product became more negative. This
effect was less pronounced when the choice was between two products that varied
more widely in desirability (easy decision).
Generally,
the greater the separation between alternatives, the less dissonance will be
produced after a decision. After all, a choice between a highly desirable
product and an undesirable product is an easy one. On the other hand, the
closer the alternatives are to one another (assuming they are not identical),
the more difficult the decision and the more postdecision dissonance will be
aroused. Thus, the greatest postdecision dissonance will be realized when you
have to choose between two mutually exclusive (you can only have one), equally
attractive, but different alternatives.
How do we
explain these free-choice dissonance situations? Shultz and Lepper (1999)
suggested that an analogy can be made between dissonance phenomena and the
operation of artificial intelligence neural networks. Networks of cognitions
underlie states of consonance and dissonance and are activated by a set of
constraints imposed by a problem. For example, in a choice between two cars,
you may be constrained by finances, model preference, and color desirability.
According to Shultz and Lepper, the decision we make attempts to satisfy as
many of the constraints as possible. In short, “the motivation to increase
cognitive consonance, and thus to reduce dissonance, results from the various
constraints on the beliefs and attitudes that a person holds at a given point
in time” (p. 238). Consonance results when similar cognitions are activated and
inconsistent cognitions are inhibited. Thus, in the free-choice situation,
linkages among positive cognitions associated with an alternative produce
consonance. However, for the unchosen alternative, the linkages between
inconsistent elements (the unchosen alternative and the positive cognitions
associated with it) produce dissonance.
Shultz and
Lepper (1996) performed computer simulations of Brehm’s
(1956) original
experiment and produced results that matched quite well with Brehm’s
results. That is,
ratings of the unchosen alternative became more negative, and ratings of the
chosen alternative became only slightly more positive. However, Shultz and
Lepper pointed out that in Brehm’s experiment, participants always made a
decision that was both
difficult (two products that were rated very similarly) and between two highly
desirable products. Schultz and Lepper found that when participants had to
choose between two similarly rated but undesirable products, the ratings of the
chosen product became much more positive, but the ratings of the unchosen
product became only slightly more negative.
An
experiment by Shultz, Leveille, and Lepper (1999) sought to test the results
from computer simulations of free-choice experiments against actual behavior of
individuals. Participants in this experiment were given the choice between two
posters after indicating on a rating scale how much they liked each poster. The
choice parameters varied in difficulty. An easy choice was one between two
posters—one with a high initial rating and one with a low initial rating. In
the “high-difficult” condition, a choice was to be made between two posters
that had been rated very positively by participants. Finally, in the
“low-difficult” condition, participants had to choose between two posters that
had been poorly rated. Following the choice procedure, participants again rated
the posters. The results paralleled the computer simulations. In the
high-difficult condition, ratings of the unchosen alternative became
substantially more negative, whereas ratings of the chosen alternative became
only slightly more positive. In the low-difficult condition, the opposite was
true; ratings of the chosen alternative became much more positive. However,
ratings of the unchosen alternative became only slightly more negative. These
results are consistent with Shultz and Lepper’s (1996)
consonance constraint satisfaction model.
Finally,
the way that postdecision dissonance operates may depend partly on one’s
culture (Hoshino-Browne
et al., 2005; Kitayama, Snibbe, Markus, & Suzuki, 2004). In Western culture
personal dissonancereduction dominates. This means that when we are selecting
between two alternatives for ourselves, we are likely to experience dissonance
and resolve it in the manner predicted by dissonance theory. However, in
Eastern cultures (e.g., Japan) personal choices do not arouse as much
dissonance as they do in Western cultures. Instead, interpersonal
dissonancetends to be more important. Interpersonal dissonance arises when an
individual is required to make a choice for someone else. In the Hoshino-Browne
et al. (2005) study, for example, European Canadians (i.e., Canadians born in
Canada) and Asian Canadians (Canadians born in an Asian country) were asked to
rank 10 Chinese cuisine entrees that would be served at an on-campus
restaurant. The rankings were done under two conditions. In one condition,
participants were instructed to rank the entrees based on their own personal
preferences (self-preferences). In the other condition, participants were instructed
to rank the entrees according to the preferences of their best friend (other
preferences). After completing some other measures, participants were offered
two gift certificates for entrees they had ranked (their fifth and sixth
choices were offered). Participants had to choose one of the gift certificates
for themselves (in the self-preference condition) or their friend (in the other
preference condition). The results, as shown in Figure 6.9 showed that when
European Canadians were making a choice for themselves, more dissonance
reduction was shown than when making a choice for the friend. The opposite was
true for the Asian Canadians. They showed more dissonance reduction when making
a choice for their friend than for themselves.
Figure
6.9Dissonance when making a choice for oneself or a friend among Asian and
European Canadians.
Responsibility:
Another View of Cognitive Dissonance
Another
view suggests that cognitive dissonance occurs only when our actions produce
negative consequences (Cooper & Scher, 1992). According to this view, it is
not the inconsistency that causes dissonance so much as our feelings of
personal responsibility when bad things happen (Cooper & Fazio, 1984).
Let’s
say, for example, that you wrote a very good essay in favor of something you believed in, such as not raising
tuition at your school. You knew that the essay could be presented to the
school’s board of trustees, the body that determines tuition rates.
You then learned that
your essay was actually used to convince the board to raise tuition. Or perhaps
you were asked to write an essay taking a position you did not believe
in—raising tuition. You then learned that the essay convinced the board to
raise tuition.
How would
you feel?
According
to this responsibility view, simply doing something counter to your beliefs
will not produce dissonance unless there are negative results. If you are
opposed to tuition hikes and write an essay in favor of them, but there are no
hikes as a result, you do not experience dissonance. In several similar
studies, people were asked to write essays advocating a position—raising
tuition—that conflicted with their beliefs. When rates were increased and
essayists felt responsible for the outcome, they resolved the dissonance by
changing their attitude in the direction of the outcome. That is, they began to
say they were now more in favor of a fee increase than before they wrote the
essay. When students wrote essays in favor of a fee increase, and fees were not
increased, they did not experience dissonance and did not change their
attitudes. When there is no tension, there is no attitude change.
So, what
creates dissonance, inconsistency, or a sense of responsibility? There have
been hundreds, perhaps thousands, of experiments that support the basic ideas
of cognitive dissonance theory—namely, that inconsistency leads to attitude
change. That there are valid alternatives simply means the theory may have to
incorporate those ideas and continue to be revised.
Attribution
Processes and Dissonance
We noted
earlier that dissonance is unlikely to be aroused when a person has a
sufficient external justification (attribution) for his or her
attitude-discrepant behavior. An experiment by Cooper (1998) highlighted the
role of attribution processes in mediating dissonance reactions. Cooper had
participants write a counter attitudinal essay advocating the institution of
7:00 A.M.classes on campus (something students opposed). They wrote the essays
under either a high-choice (participants were asked to write the essay “if you
are willing”) or a low-choice condition (the “if you are willing” phrase was
left out). Participants were also randomly assigned to a misattribution
condition (an instruction that inconsistent lighting makes many feel tense and
aroused) or a no-misattribution condition (the instruction about the lighting
effects was deleted). The main measure was the participants’ratings
(positive or negative) about instituting 7:00 A.M.classes.
Cooper
found that greater attitude change occurred under the high-choice condition.
This confirms our earlier statement that under conditions of free choice,
dissonance is more likely to be aroused and attitude change more likely to
occur. Additionally, there was less attitude change in the direction of the
essay under the misattribution condition than the no-misattribution condition.
Participants in the misattribution condition had an external explanation for
their arousal (dissonance), and were consequently less likely to change their
attitude. The greatest amount of attitude change in the direction of the essay
was realized in the high-choice (participants chose to write the
essay)/no-misattribution condition. In a follow-up experiment using a different
task, Cooper found that participants who had previously misattributed their
arousal to the lighting did not show dissonance-consistent attitude change.
Attribution
style also relates to the arousal of dissonance. Stalder and Baron (1998)
investigated the relationship between attributional complexity (AC) and
dissonance-produced attitude change in a series of experiments. Specifically,
attributional complexity refers to how complex a person’s
attributions are for explaining behavior and events. High-AC individuals are those who normally
engage in thorough attributional searches for information. Thus, a high-AC
person will search long and hard for the source of arousal in a given situation
(e.g., a situation that arouses dissonance). A low-AC person is less likely to
engage in such a search.
The
results from their first experiment confirmed the idea that high-AC individuals
show little dissonance-related attitude change, most likely because they are
able to generate a wide variety of possible causes for the arousal associated
with dissonance (Stalder & Baron, 1998). Having attributed the arousal to
something other than the dissonance-arousing situation, the high-AC individual
would not be expected to show much attitude change. In their second experiment,
Stalder and Baron found that low-AC individuals showed the typical dissonance-related
attitude change after dissonance arousal.
The two
experiments just discussed suggest strongly that dissonance-related attitude
change is mediated by the attributions made about the dissonance situation. If
an alternative to dissonance is provided for an explanation for
dissonance-related arousal, the typical dissonance result does not occur.
Stalder and Baron’s study shows us that there are
individual differences in attributional style, which correlates with
dissonance-related attitude change. Those individuals who are highly motivated
to find causes for their arousal are less likely to show dissonance-related
attitude change because they settle on an alternative attribution for their
arousal, more so than a person who is not so motivated.
Lessons of Cognitive Dissonance Theory
What can we learn about persuasive techniques from cognitive
dissonance theory? The first lesson is that cognitive inconsistency often leads
to change. Therefore, one persuasive technique is to point out to people how
their behavior runs counter to their beliefs. Presumably, if people are aware
of their inconsistencies, they will change. Persuasion may also occur if
individuals are made aware that their behavior may produce a negative outcome
(Cooper & Scher, 1992).
A second lesson is that any time you can induce someone to
become publicly committed to a behavior that is counter to their beliefs,
attitude change is a likely outcome. One reason for the change is that people
use their public behavior as a kind of heuristic, a rule that says people stand
by their public acts and bear personal responsibility for them (Baumeister
& Tice, 1984; Zimbardo & Leippe, 1992). In other words, the rule is,
“If I did it, I meant it.”
Cognitive Dissonance and Cult Membership
Cognitive dissonance plays an important role in the formation
and maintenance of cults. Once people make a public commitment to a leader and
a movement, it is hard for them to acknowledge their misgivings. Instead, they
have to throw more and more resources into maintaining their commitment, even
when it becomes obvious to others that the loyalty is misplaced. This
phenomenon has occurred many times in human history. It happened in 1978 in
Guyana, in Jonestown, the “utopian” community of the Reverend Jim Jones. On his
orders, his followers committed mass suicide by drinking Kool Aid laced with
cyanide. It happened again more recently in Waco, Texas.
In March 1993, a religious cult known as the Branch Davidians
came to national attention at the beginning of its stand-off with the Bureau of
Alcohol, Tobacco, and Firearms (ATF). The cult was led by David Koresh, who
claimed to receive orders from God. Koresh created the group’s social reality.
He separated cult members from the rest of the world, both physically and
psychologically. He told them that he was Jesus and that “others” would deny
the fact and try to destroy the cult. The Davidians stocked arms, food, and
ammunition to prepare for apocalypse and confrontation with the outside world.
Koresh’s predictions seemed to come true when ATF agents came to seize the cult’s
automatic weapons. Guns blazed on both sides, leaving several agents dead and
wounded.
A siege of the compound began that lasted nearly 2 months.
Federal authorities grew increasingly concerned about the welfare of the many
children inside and the lack of progress in the negotiations with Koresh.
Finally, assured by experts that the Davidians would not commit mass suicide if
threatened, agents pumped tear gas into the compound to force them outside.
However, fires erupted inside the buildings, apparently started by the cult.
Eighty-six cult members, including 23 children, were incinerated. Apparently,
the Davidians chose self-destruction rather than destruction of their reality.
Why were members so persuaded by Koresh’s outrageous claims? How did they
become so committed to the cult?
All cults have many characteristics in common. The primary
feature is a charismatic leader. He or she takes on a supernatural aura and
persuades group members to devote their lives and fortunes to the cult. Koresh
was such a charismatic individual, able to convince large groups of people
through clever arguments and persuasive appeals. For example, he refuted
doubters by claiming to possess sole understanding of the Scriptures and changed
interpretations often to keep cult members constantly uncertain and reliant on
him. Koresh used charm and authority to gain control of followers’lives.
However, charisma alone is not enough to account for the behavior of the
Davidians. We must also look at the cognitive dynamics of the individual
members to see how they became so committed to Koresh and his ideals.
Joining the cult was no easy feat. At first, few demands were
made, but after a while, members had to give more. In fact, members routinely
turned over all of their possessions, including houses, insurance policies, and
money. Once in the group, life was quite harsh. Koresh enforced strict (and
changeable) rules on every aspect of members’lives, including personally
rationing all their food, imposing celibacy on the men while taking women as
his wives and concubines, and inflicting physical abuse. In short, residents of
the compound had to expend quite a bit of effort to be members.
All the requirements for membership relate directly to what we
know about attitudes and behavior from dissonance theory. For example,
dissonance research shows that the harder people have to work to get into a
group, the more they value that group (Aronson & Mills, 1959). By turning
over all of their possessions, members were making an irreversible commitment
to the cult. Once such a commitment is made, people are unlikely to abandon
positive attitudes toward the group (Festinger, Riecken, & Schachter,
1982). After expending so much effort, questioning commitment would create
cognitive dissonance (Osherow, 1988). It is inconsistent to prove devotion to a
belief by donating all of your possessions and then to abandon those beliefs.
In other words, to a large extent, cult members persuade themselves. Dissonance
theory predicts that the Davidians would come to value the group highly and be
disinclined to question Koresh. This is, in fact, what happened.
Interestingly, cult members do not lose faith when the
situation begins to sour. In fact, there is sometimes an increase in the
strength of their commitment. One study investigated a “doomsday” society, a
group that predicts the end of the world (Festinger et al., 1982). The study
found that when a prophecy failed, members became more committed to the group.
There are five conditions that must be met before this effect will occur.
1. The belief must be
held with deep conviction and must be reflected in the believer’s overt
behavior.
2. The believer must
have taken a step toward commitment that is difficult to reverse, for example,
giving all of his or her money to the group.
3. The belief must be
specific and well enough related to real-world events that it can be
disconfirmed, or proven false—for example, the prediction that the world will
end on a specified day.
4. There must be
undeniable evidence that the belief is false (the world doesn’t end).
5. The individual
believer must have social support for the belief after disconfirmation.
Most, perhaps all, five conditions were present in the Waco
tragedy. Members were committed to their beliefs and gave everything they had
to Koresh. There was evidence that the situation was unstable; several members
had left the cult, and some were even talking to federal officials. And when it
started to become obvious that Koresh was not invincible, members had each
other to turn to for social support. As negotiations deteriorated, Koresh
altered his rhetoric to emphasize apocalyptic visions, rationalizing the cult’s
destruction and self-sacrifice. Cult members probably came to believe it was
their destiny to die, if necessary. The power of persuasion can be seen in the
tragic results.
Alternatives to Cognitive Dissonance Theory
Not all social psychologists believe cognitive dissonance
theory is the best way to explain what happens when cognitive inconsistencies
occur. Other theories have been proposed to explain how people deal with these
discrepancies. In the sections that follow, we explore some alternatives to
traditional cognitive dissonance theory.
Self-Perception Theory
Daryl Bem, a student of the great behaviorist psychologist B.
F. Skinner, challenged cognitive dissonance theory, because, he asserted, he
could explain people’s behavior without looking at their inner motives. Bem
(1972) proposed self-perception theory, which explains discrepant behavior by
simply assuming that people are not self-conscious processors of information.
People observe their own behavior and assume that their attitudes must be
consistent with that behavior. If you eat a big dinner, you assume that you must
have been hungry. If you take a public stand on an issue, the rule of
selfperception theory is, “I said it, so I must have meant it.” We don’t look
at our motives; we just process the information and conclude that there is no
inconsistency.
Bem supported his theory with some interesting experiments. In
one, he trained people to tell the truth whenever a “truth” (green) light was
lit and to lie whenever a “lie” (red) light was lit. When the green light was
on, people had to say something about themselves that was true. When the red
light was on, people had to lie about themselves. Bem then asked the
participants to make further statements that were either true or false under
both truth and lie lights. Participants who told lies when the truth light was on
came to believe that those false statements were true. Likewise, subjects who
made true statements when the lie light was on reported that they lied.
The point of self-perception theory is that we make inferences
about our behavior in much the same way an outside observer might. If you were
observing the experiment, you would infer, quite reasonably, that whatever
anyone said when the light was red was a lie and anything said under the green
light was true. The participants assumed the same thing. According to
self-perception theory, something does not have to happen “inside” the person
for inconsistencies to be resolved—no tension, no motivation to reconcile
attitudes and behavior, just information processing.
Rationalization
Imagine a group of cigar smokers sitting around a cigar shop
talking about the potential health hazards of their cigar-smoking habit. There
is ample evidence that cigarette smoking poses health risks. There is also
evidence that cigar smoking may have some health risks as well. How do smokers
reconcile the conflict between the health-related risks and continuing to
smoke? Cognitive dissonance theory would predict that dissonance would be
aroused in this situation. The fact that millions of people smoke is proof that
dissonance does not always lead to behavior change. So, how can one continue to
smoke, knowing the health risks? The answer is that smokers often engage in rationalization.
Smokers convince themselves that: “Nothing will happen to me,” “I’ll stop when
I’m 40,” or “My grandfather lived until 80, and he smoked like a chimney.”
Rationalizations are important in maintaining a coherent
self-concept.
An interesting study was conducted by DeSantis (2003) that
illustrates this rationalization process. DeSantis, being a cigar smoker
himself, was part of a group of regulars who meet at a Kentucky cigar store to
smoke their cigars and talk sports. DeSantis decided to study the inner
workings of this group using a participant observation ethnography method.
DeSantis continued his membership and at the same time carefully studied the
interactions among the group members (with their knowledge and permission).
DeSantis found that members generated five rationalizations to support their
continued cigar smoking in the face of evidence of its harmful effects. These
rationalizations are listed in Table 6.1, along with a brief explanation of
each. Interestingly, these rationalizations were maintained even after one of
the members died from heart disease. Rationalization can, indeed, be a powerful
thing.
Self-Affirmation Theory
Dissonance may threaten a person’s self-concept with negative
implications, making the person appear stupid, unethical, or lazy (Steele,
1988). Nonsmokers probably view smokers as being all three. Then why don’t
people in dissonant situations alter their behavior? In the case of cigarette
smoking, a large part of the answer is the highly addictive nature of nicotine.
Many people try to quit and fail, or they can’t face the prospect of never
having another cigarette. So they are stuck with the dissonance.
Selfaffirmationtheory suggests that people may not try to reduce dissonance if
they can maintain (affirm) their self-concept by proving that they are adequate
in other ways:
“Yes, I may be a smoker, but I’m also a good mother, a
respected professional, and an active citizen in my community.” These
self-affirmations remove the sting inherent in a dissonance situation (Zimbardo
& Leippe, 1992). People cope with a threat to one aspect of the self by
affirming an unrelated part of the self (Steele, 1988).
Rationalization
|
Explanation
|
Things done in moderation won’t hurt you
There are health benefits to smoking
Cigars are not as bad as cigarettes
Research on health effects of cigar
smoking is flawed
Life is dangerous
|
Participants expressed that smoking in
moderation won’t be harmful. Some indicated that they cut down or only smoked
in certain, limited situations. Some indicated that their physicians said it
was OK to smoke cigars in moderation.
Participants pointed to the
stress-reducing effect of smoking. Some saw the stress-reducing effect as a
legitimate trade-off for any health risks.
Participants deny that research on
health risks of cigarettes do not apply to cigars, indicating that one smokes
cigars less frequently than cigarettes and that one does not inhale cigars.
Discounting of research on effects of
cigar smoking on the basis that the research is methodologically flawed. Two
fl aws cited: Lack of adequate research and inconsistent nature of findings.
Relative comparisons made between cigar
smoking and other hazards (e.g., air pollution, driving). Dangers of smoking
minimized in the light of other hazards.
|
Table 6.1 Five
Rationalizations Made By Cigar Smokers (Based on data in Desantis, 2003)
The Action-Based Model
Some recent research has called into question the applicability
of self-affirmation theory to cognitive dissonance (Harmon-Jones, 2000).
According to Harmon-Jones, “engaging in self-affirmation following
dissonance-evoking behaviors seems subordinate to resolving the specific
discrepancy aroused by the behavior” (2000, p. 132). As an alternative,
Harmon-Jones suggests that one need not deviate much from the original
cognitive dissonance theory to understand discrepancy reduction. Harmon-Jones
proposed the action-based modelof cognitive dissonance reduction. According to
this model, “cognitive discrepancy generates dissonance motivation because the
cognitive discrepancy has the potential to interfere with effective
unconflicted action” (Harmon-Jones, Petertson, & Vaughn, 2003, p. 69).
Anything that enhances the prospect for effective, unconflicted action should,
according to the model, enhance cognitive dissonance reduction. An experiment
by Harmon-Jones and Harmon-Jones (2002) demonstrated this clearly. Participants
made either a difficult decision (between two equally valued physical exercises
they had previously evaluated favorably) or an easy decision (between a highly
valued and a lowly valued physical exercise) under one of two mind-sets. Half of
the participants in each decision condition wrote down seven things that they
could do to improve their behavior concerning the chosen alternative
(action-oriented mind-set). The other half of the participants in each decision
condition wrote about seven things they do during a normal day (neutral
mind-set). After making their choices, participants once again evaluated the
desirability of the exercises. The researchers predicted that the most
dissonance reduction (evidenced by the greatest change in predecision and
postdecision evaluation of alternatives) would be when the decision was
difficult and an action-oriented mind-set was adopted. The results confirmed
this prediction. The greatest amount of postdecision spread was found when the
decision was difficult and an action-oriented mind-set was adopted.
Psychological Reactance
Psychological tension can be reduced in several ways.
Sometimes, when people realize they have been coerced into doing or buying
something against their wishes, they try to regain or reassert their freedom.
This response is called psychological reactance (Brehm & Brehm, 1981). The
theory of psychological reactance, an offshoot of cognitive dissonance theory,
suggests that when some part of our freedom is threatened, we become aroused
and motivated to restore that freedom.
The Coca-Cola Company found this out in 1985 when it tried to
replace the traditional Coke formula with “New Coke.” The company conducted an
in-depth marketing study of the new product that included 200,000 taste tests.
The tests showed that people really liked New Coke. The company went ahead with
plans to retire the old formula and put New Coke in its place. However, the
issue was not taste; it was perceived choice. People resented having a choice
taken away and reacted by buying the traditional Coke as if it were manna from
heaven, never to be seen again. Some people even formed Old Coke clubs. The
company got over 1,500 angry calls and letters every day. Coca-Cola had to
change its marketing plans, and “Classic Coke” still holds an honored place on
the grocery shelves (Oskamp, 1991). Whether consumers liked New Coke did not
matter. Their emotional ties to old Coke did matter, as did their freedom to
buy it. New Coke just wasn’t it for these folks.
Persuading the Masses through Propaganda
Propaganda: A Definition
We now turn our attention to the application of persuasion
techniques on a mass scale. History abounds with examples of persuasion
techniques aimed at changing the attitudes and behavior of entire populations.
Such mass persuasion can take many forms. Advertisers routinely craft
persuasive messages we call advertisements to get you to buy one product rather
than another. Various public service persuasive messages attempt to get us to
change a wide range of behavior, including not driving drunk, practicing safe
sex, wearing seat belts, and avoiding illegal drugs.
Perhaps the most controversial application of mass persuasion
techniques is the use of propaganda. Propaganda is “a deliberate attempt to
persuade people, by any available media, to think in a manner desired by the
source” (Taylor, 2003, p. 7). Throughout human history there are many examples
of the use of propaganda to shape the attitudes and behaviors of masses of
individuals. For example, propaganda was extensively used during the American
Revolution to both sell the colonists’cause and demonize the British. It was
also used extensively in World War I by both the Germans and Allies. However,
perhaps the best example of the application of propaganda was by the Nazis
during the years leading up to and throughout World War II. Although it is true
that both sides in World War II used propaganda, the Nazis under the guidance
of Josef Goebbels raised propaganda to levels never before seen.
There are a few things that you should understand about
propaganda before we continue with our discussion. First, it is common to
characterize propaganda as a pack of lies used by the enemy to manipulate
attitudes and behavior. While it is true that propaganda is often aimed at one’s
enemy, it is also used extensively to shape the attitudes and behavior of one’s
own citizens. And, as noted earlier, it is also used by the “good guys.” For
example, during World War II the U.S. government engaged in propaganda aimed at
boosting the war effort at home. Hollywood films such as Wake Island(1942)
portrayed the Marines on the island holding out to the last man against the
Japanese onslaught. In fact, there was no such heroic last stand. Many of the
Marines and civilians were captured and a good number of them were murdered by
the Japanese military. The film was intended to provide a much needed boost in
morale on the home front, which it in fact did provide. Second, propaganda is
not always a “pack of lies.” Quite the contrary, modern propaganda attempts to
stay as close to the truth as possible (Taylor, 2003). This is not to say that
lies are never used; they are. However, a good propagandist knows that his or
her credibility is an important commodity. Caught in a lie, this credibility
suffers. Finally, propaganda is neither good nor bad. It is simply a means to
an end (Taylor, 2003).
Characteristics of Propaganda
Ellul (1965) defines two broad characteristics of propaganda.
The internal characteristicsof propaganda refer to the characteristics of the
target of the propaganda. According to Ellul, a good propagandist must know the
“psychological terrain” on which he or she is operating. This means that the
propagandist must know which attitudes and behaviors can be easily manipulated.
Typically, the propagandist stays away from deeply held beliefs and
concentrates on those that are more malleable. For example, Communist
propaganda in cold war Poland shied away from attacking the Catholic Church and
the Catholic religion. This is because Catholicism and the Catholic Church were
extremely important to the Polish people. Conversely, Nazi propaganda exploited
already existing
anti-Semitism to shape the German population’s attitudes about
Jews.
The external characteristicsof propaganda refer to the
characteristics of the propaganda itself. One important point that Ellul makes
is that in order for propaganda to be effective, it must be organized and
total. “Organized” means that the propaganda is the product of a concerted
effort to shape attitudes and behavior. It is not a hit-ormiss proposition. The
good propagandist has a clear plan in mind and uses propaganda to execute that
plan. As an example, consider the fact that the Nazis spent around a million
dollars a day (in 1939 dollars) on propaganda at the start of World War II in
1939. “Total” means that the masses must immerse the population in the
propaganda.
This second characteristic is why propaganda works best in
situations where the propagandist can control all of the outlets for propaganda.
For example, Josef Goebbels had total control over all of the media outlets of
the day: newspapers, radio, and film. Additionally, Nazi propaganda permeated
every aspect of life in Germany. The stamps people put on their letters had
Nazi images, children’s books portrayed Jews in stereotyped ways, museums were
full of Nazi art, and pro-Nazi plays filled the theaters. Another external
characteristic is the fact that propaganda is directed at the individual in the
context of the masses (Ellul, 1965). That is, the propagandist directs
propaganda at individuals but uses the masses to help break down individual
thought. An individual apart from the masses will offer too much resistance to
propaganda (Ellul, 1965). It is for this reason that Nazis held huge rallies
(often at night so that one’s critical thinking skills were not at their peak).
Imagine how difficult it would be for you to counterargue Nazi ideas when you
are part of a huge crowd pledging their undying support for those ideas. In
short, Nazi propaganda was aimed at making each individual feel as though he or
she was a part of something much larger.
A host of other characteristics are typically true of
propaganda. These are listed in Table 6.2.
Table 6.2
Additional Characteristics of Propaganda
The Aims of Propaganda
As noted, propaganda was extensively used during the American
Revolution. For example, Paul Revere made an engraving of the “Boston Massacre”
that depicted the event inaccurately (Go to
http://www.mediaworkshop.org/csd18/csd18web_site/pat/html/attucksphotolink.htm
to see the engraving and a list of errors in the engraving). The British were
shown in a military picket line with their commander behind them giving the order to fire. The scene was
shown in a wide open area between rows of buildings in clear weather. The
Colonists were portrayed as passive and peaceful, only to be ruthlessly mowed
down by the evil British. In fact, the actual event was much different. The
Colonists were armed and were taunting the British. There was much confusion in
the confined space. And, there is evidence that the Colonists fired the first
shot. In fact, a Colonial jury acquitted the British soldiers of any crime in
the event. (The picture at http://www.historywiz.com/bostonmassacre.htmis a
more accurate portrayal of the event.) Despite the inaccuracies, Revere’s
engraving was widely distributed throughout
the Colonies and was successful in its aim of arousing hatred for the British.
Samuel
Adams worked for the Boston Globeat the time and organized a propaganda team
known as the Committee of Correspondence. The committee would gather the news
and report back to Adams, who would then send his version of the events out to
other newspapers (Jowett & O’Donnell, 1986). Adams had a reputation
for being something of
a rabble-rouser. However, he did have a clear vision of his cause (separation
from England) and how to achieve it. Adams developed five aims of propaganda
(Jowett & O’Donnel, 1986). They are as valid today as
they were then:
1. The aims of the cause must be justified.
2. The advantages of victory must be made clear
and known.
3. The people need to be aroused to action by
instilling hatred for the enemy.
4. Logical arguments of the enemy must be
negated.
5. All issues must be stated in clear-cut,
black-and-white terms.
Propaganda
Techniques
The
techniques used by propagandists may vary from case to case. However, the goal
is the same: Persuade the masses. Common propaganda techniques include the
following (Brown, 1967):
• Use of
stereotypes: Propagandists often take advantage of our natural tendency to
stereotype people. Propaganda can eventually lead us to think of a group of
people in terms of the stereotype, rather than as individual human beings.
• Substitution of names: Propagandists often use
derogatory names to refer to disliked groups. Victims of propaganda become
dehumanized, and it becomes easier to persecute them.
• Selection of facts: Propagandists do not
present a balanced view of events. They select specific facts that support
their point of view.
•
Downright lying: Falsehoods are used to persuade others.
•
Repetition: The same message is repeated over and over. Repeated exposure
eventually leads to acceptance of the message.
•
Assertion: Propagandists are not interested in debating. Instead, they assert
their point forcefully.
• Pinpointing an enemy: Propaganda is most
effective if an enemy can be identified who poses a threat to all. This directs
aggression or blame away from the propagandists and strengthens in-group
feelings of unity and solidarity. This technique plays on the “us versus them”
mentality.
• Appeals to authority:Propagandists often make
references to or identify their leaders with higher sources of authority. This
can mean a higher political authority (e.g., approval from a revered leader) or
to a higher power (e.g., God). In either case, the propagandists leave the
impression that their leader has the support and blessing of the higher
authority.
Fritz
Hippler, the head of the Nazi film industry, captured the essence of successful
propaganda. He boiled down propaganda to two main techniques: simplification
and repetition. All messages used in propaganda should be stated in simple
terms so that even the least intelligent members of a society can understand
the message. Once the message is formulated, it is then repeated so it becomes
familiar to the targets of propaganda.
Hitler’s
Rise to Power
Looking
back at the years between 1924 and 1945 when a darkness descended across
Europe, it is obvious to see the outcome of the rise of Nazism and Hitler to
power in Germany. However, how could a failed painter, army corporal, and later
political prisoner rise to the peak of power in Germany in just nine years?
Part of the answer, of course, is the fact that the Nazi Party had a
well-organized paramilitary wing that effectively intimidated or eliminated
opposition parties such as the Communist Party. However, such street muscle
cannot fully explain how a large segment of the German people came to accept
and support Hitler and Nazism. To answer this question we need to look at how
the Nazis, through Josef Goebbels, used propaganda to rise to power,
consolidate power, and prepare the German people for war and for the
extermination of the Jews.
In the
years following the end of World War I, the German people and economy were
suffering greatly. War reparations were causing widespread economic depression.
Inflation ravaged the economy. Within this context Adolph Hitler would emerge
to become the most powerful man in Germany. But it didn’t
happen right away. On September
9, 1923, Hitler and his followers attempted to overthrow the Bavarian
government in Munich. The so-called “Beer Hall Putsch” was a complete failure.
The Bavarian government refused to capitulate and no popular uprising occurred.
Instead, Hitler and his followers were imprisoned in Lansberg Prison. This was
on April 1, 1924. At this point the Nazi Party was in a shambles. Its leaders
were in prison, the party newspaper was shut down, and the party was declared
illegal. During his prison stay, Hitler dictated his manifesto Mein Kampfto
Rudolph Hess.
On
December 24, 1925, Hitler was released from prison. His release provided one of
the first propaganda opportunities for his propagandists. The exit from the
prison was quite ordinary. So, a photograph was taken at a different location
showing an imposing gate and a large black car awaiting the emergence of
Hitler. Soon after his release Mein Kampf was published. Still, the party was
in dire straits. In fact, on March 9, 1925, the government issued an order
prohibiting Hitler from speaking in public. This provided another early
propaganda opportunity for the Nazis. A poster was distributed showing Hitler
with tape across his mouth. The caption read “He alone among 2,000 million people
is forbidden to speak.” It would take a while, but the ban was finally lifted
in September of 1928. But the party was still not terribly strong, though
things were moving along. By 1929 Hitler was the head of the Nazi party. Josef
Goebbels gave the party a better image with his skillful application of
propaganda. Then on October 29, 1929, the German (and world economy in general)
crashed and entered the Great Depression. An already shaky German economy was
devastated. People who had secure jobs in the past found themselves unemployed
and starving. This gave Hitler and the Nazis their best opportunity to take
power. The Nazi message started to sound better and better to many Germans in
misery. The party began to grow and on September 14, 1930, the Nazi Party won
107 seats in the Reichstag (the German Parliament). In April 1932, Hitler lost
a runoff election against the immensely popular President Hindenburg, but he
did garner 36% of the vote. Despite the overwhelming victory by Hindenburg,
political turmoil still existed. With the German government near to collapse
and Hitler agitating for power, the 85-year-old Hindenburg reluctantly
appointed Hitler to be Chancellor of Germany on January 30, 1933. Just a few
short weeks later in March of 1933, Hitler consolidated his power and became
the absolute ruler of Germany, the Reichstag was burned, and Germany entered
into its darkest period of its history—a history that would include persecution
and extermination of Jews and other Eastern Europeans in death camps and the
loss of nearly 80 million people in World War II.
The Power
of Propaganda in Nazi Germany
Let’s
turn our attention to how Josef Goebbels used propaganda at various points in
the Nazi rise to power
and selling of Nazi ideas to the German public and the world. We shall organize
our discussion around the techniques of propaganda reviewed earlier. For each
technique, we shall explore briefly how Goebbels used propaganda to shape the
attitudes and behaviors of the masses. (Examples of Nazi propaganda can be
found athttp://www.calvin.edu/academic/cas/gpa.)
• Use of
stereotypes:As noted earlier, propagandists take advantage of the tendency to
stereotype people. Propaganda from the Nazi era used this technique to
marginalize and demonize the Jews. Various anti-Semitic posters were widely
used. Typically these portrayed Jews as hook-nosed evil characters bent upon
controlling the German people and the world. For example, one such poster
showed a caricature of an evil Jew inciting people into war with the caption
“The Jew. The inciter of war, the prolonger of war.” Another poster, called
“The String Puller, showed a caricature of a Jew as a puppeteer pulling the
strings of the German people. German propaganda movies also were used to
reinforce negative stereotypes of the Jews and instill fear and loathing of
them into the German people. The most infamous of these films was Fritz Hippler’s
The Eternal Jew. In
this film Jews were likened to rats and other vermin and Jewish rituals (e.g.,
Kosher slaughter of animals) were portrayed in hideous ways. Even children’s
books were laced with
anti-Semitic images and themes. The most famous of these was the series of
children’s books called
Der Giftpilz(The Poison Mushroom). As in other propaganda materials, Jews were
portrayed as crafty, evil, hook-nosed characters, often preying on innocent
Germans.
• Substitution of names:Nazi propaganda
succeeded in characterizing Jews and Eastern Europeans as subhuman. One cartoon
that appeared in the Nazi news paper Der Sturmerin February 1930 showed a huge
black spider with a Star of David on its torso sucking Germans that were
hanging in its web dry, the caption reading “Sucked Dry.” Eastern Europeans
were often referred to as “untermenchen” (subhuman) in posters that juxtaposed
the perfect Aryan against the mongrel-like Eastern European.
• Selection of facts:Even when the war was not
going well, Goebbels painted a rosy picture of what was happening by
selectively releasing information. For example, in a 1943 article Gobbels said:
Was there
ever a nation that had so favorable a position after five years of war as we do
today? The front is unbroken. The homeland is morally and materially able to
withstand the bombing terror. A river of war material flows from our factories.
A new weapon against the enemy air attacks is being prepared. Countless able
hands are working at it day and night. We have a hard test of patience before
us, but the reward will come one day. The German farmer is bringing in a good
harvest.
What he failed to mention was that the German
military industry was being pounded almost around the clock by Allied air
forces, the wonder weapons of which he spoke were of little tactical value, and
the German military was experiencing defeats on all fronts.
• Downright lying:Apparently, Hitler wanted a
pretext on which to invade Poland in 1939. So, on August 31, 1939, SS officers
took Polish prisoners from a concentration camp, dressed them in Polish army
uniforms, and shot them. Their bodies were scattered outside a German radio
station and comprised a contrived attack on a German radio station on the
Polish border. In fact, Hitler said, “Polish regular officers fired on our
territory. Since 5:45 a.m. we have been returning the fire.” The German
invasion of Poland began soon after Hitler’s false statement.
• Repetition:Nazi propaganda hammered home the
same messages and images over and over. For example, several propaganda posters
portrayed Hitler as the savior of Germany and a skilled military leader.
• Assertion:In 1943, despite the fact that the
tide of the war was turning against Germany, Josef Goebbels continued to assert
that Germany would win the war. In a New Year’s Eve speech in
1943 he stated, “Our war position has indeed become tighter than it was at the end of 1942,
but it is more than sufficient to guarantee us a certain final victory.” He
went on to list the failures of the Allied army and asserted that the facts
supported a German victory.
•
Pinpointing an enemy:Propaganda works best when it comes out against something.
An old saying goes that nothing unites people like a common enemy. The enemy
becomes the focus of negative thoughts and emotions and serves to deflect
criticism from the propagandist’s group. Nazi propaganda identified two enemies: the Jews and opposing
countries. Of the Jews, Goebbels wrote in 1941, “Every Jew is our enemy in this
historic struggle, regardless of whether he vegetates in a Polish ghetto or
carries on his parasitic existence in Berlin or Hamburg or blows the trumpets
of war in New York or Washington. All Jews by virtue of their birth and their
race are part of an international conspiracy against National Socialist
Germany.” A poster showed a fist smashing the bodies of enemies (one clearly
with a British flag on his back) with the caption “Into dust with all enemies
of Germany.
• Appeals to authority:Even as Hitler rose to
power in 1933, he still had an image problem. People, politicians, and military
leaders were skeptical of Hitler and his party. So, it was important to show
that Hitler had the blessing of someone held in high esteem by the German
people. Nazi propagandists went to work giving the German people the idea that
Hitler had the support and blessing of the much beloved President Hindenburg. A
propaganda poster showed the “Corporal and the Field Marshal” together. In
reality, Hindenburg despised Hitler and handed the chancellorship over to him
only when he had no other choice. Additionally, Nazi art often showed Hitler in
god-like poses and settings, giving the impression that he also had the support
of a supreme being.
The
Leopold and Loeb Case Revisited
Clarence
Darrow used all his powers of persuasion to save his clients, Leopold and Loeb,
from execution. As a skilled communicator, he knew how important it was to establish
and maintain his credibility. Many of his arguments aimed, sometimes subtly,
sometimes not, at destroying his opponent’s credibility and enhancing his own.
Darrow
also understood that a communicator who seems disinterested in persuading his
audience is usually more successful than one who is clearly trying to persuade.
He took the high moral ground, arguing that it would be inhumane to execute two
young men who weren’t entirely responsible for their actions.
Darrow did
not neglect his audiences, the trial judge and the public. He carefully
structured and presented his arguments in order to have the greatest effect on
them. Darrow knew that arguments too far from the judge’s
“latitude of acceptance” would not succeed. He didn’t argue against capital punishment (although he personally
opposed it), just capital punishment in this particular case. He knew Judge
Caverly was listening carefully to his arguments, elaborating on them and
placing them in the context of American criminal justice. He knew the world was
listening, too. The Leopold and Loeb “thrill murder” case became one of the
most infamous incidents in U.S. history, for Americans were shocked at the
spectacle of two wealthy young men who killed just to see what it would feel
like.
Judge
Caverly handed down his decision on September 10, 1924. Leopold and Loeb were
sentenced to life imprisonment for murder and 99 years for kidnapping. Loeb
died in 1936 in a prison fight; a model prisoner, Leopold was released at the
age of 70 and spent the rest of his life in Puerto Rico helping the poor.
Chapter
Review
1. What is
persuasion?
Persuasion
is a form of social influence whereby a communicator uses rational and/or
emotional arguments to convince others to change their attitudes or behavior.
2. What is
the Yale communication model?
The Yale
communication model is a theoretical model that guides persuasion tactics. It
is based on the assumption that persuasion will occur if a persuader presents a
logical argument that clarifies how attitude change is beneficial.
3. What
factors about the communicator affect persuasion?
The Yale
model focuses on the credibility of the communicator, an important determinant
of the likelihood that persuasion will occur. The components of credibility are
expertise and trustworthiness. Although an important factor in the
persuasiveness of a message, communicator credibility may not have long-lasting
effects. Over time, a message from a noncredible source may be as persuasive as
one from a credible source, a phenomenon known as the sleeper effect. This is
more likely to occur if there is a strong persuasive argument, if a discounting
cue is given, and if sufficient time passes that people forget who said what.
Other communicator factors that increase persuasion are physical attractiveness,
similarity to the target, and a rapid, fluent speech style.
4. What
message factors mediate persuasion?
Messages that include a mild to moderate
appeal to fear seem to be more persuasive than others, provided they offer a
solution to the fear-producing situation. The timing of the message is another
factor in its persuasiveness, as is the structure of the message and the extent
to which the communicator attempts to fit the message to the audience. Research
supports inoculation theory, which holds that giving people a weakened version
of an opposing argument is an effective approach to persuasion. Good
communicators also know their audience well enough not to deliver a highly
discrepant message. When this cannot be avoided, as when there is a multiple
audience problem, communicators use hidden messages and private keys and codes
to get their point across.
Additionally,
the amount of discrepancy between the content of a message and the audience
members’existing attitudes makes a difference. According to social judgment theory,
persuasion relates to the amount of personal involvement an individual has with
an issue. A message can fall into a person’s latitude of acceptance (positions found to be
acceptable), latitude of rejection (positions found to be unacceptable), or
latitude of noncommitment (positions neither accepted nor rejected, but to be
considered).
5. What is
the elaboration likelihood model of persuasion?
Cognitive response models focus on the active
role of the audience. They assert that people respond to persuasive messages by
connecting them with their own knowledge, feelings, and thoughts related to the
topic of the message. The elaboration likelihood model (ELM), which examines
how individuals respond to the persuasive message, proposes two routes to
persuasion. The first, central route processing, is used when people have the
capacity and motivation to understand the message and analyze it in a critical
and effortful manner. Central route processors elaborate on the message by
connecting it to their knowledge and feelings. Sometimes this elaboration will
persuade the recipient, depending on the strength of the message. Central route
processors tend to experience more durable attitude changes.
The second
avenue to persuasion is peripheral route processing. This occurs when
individuals do not have the motivation or interest to process effortfully.
Instead, they rely on cues other than the merits of the message, such as the
attractiveness of the communicator. Whether a person uses central or peripheral
route processing depends on a number of factors, including mood, personal
relevance, and use of language. The flexible correction model augments the
elaboration likelihood model. It suggests that individuals using central route
processing are influenced by biasing factors when they are not aware of the
potential impact of those factors—for example, when they are in a good mood.
Under these conditions, correction for biasing factors takes place.
6. What is
the impact of vividness on persuasion?
Overall,
the effect of vividness of a message on persuasion is not very strong. Studies
show, however, that individuals exposed to vivid messages on an issue that was
important to them felt the vivid message was effective. Vividness may be
beneficial in political ads or in jury trials. For example, jurors awarded more
money to a plaintiff when the evidence they heard was vivid as opposed to
nonvivid. Vivid information has its greatest impact when a persuasive message
requires few resources and a person is highly motivated to process the message.
For a message with a highly motivated target that requires many resources,
vividness does not have an effect on persuasion.
7. What is
the need for cognition?
Need for
cognition (NC) is an individual difference variable mediating persuasion.
Individuals who are high in the need for cognition will process persuasive
information along the central route, regardless of the situation or the
complexity of the message. Conversely, individuals low in the need for
cognition pay more attention to peripheral cues (e.g., physical characteristics
of the speaker) and are more likely to use peripheral route processing of a
persuasive message.
8. What is
the heuristic and systematic information model of persuasion?
The
heuristic and systematic information-processing model (HSM) focuses more
heavily on the importance of heuristics or peripheral cues than does the
elaboration likelihood model. This model notes that often issues are too
complex or too numerous for effortful, systematic processing to be practical.
9. What is
cognitive dissonance theory, and what are its main ideas?
Cognitive dissonance theory proposes that
people feel an uncomfortable tension when their attitudes, or attitude and
behavior, are inconsistent. This psychological discomfort is known as cognitive
dissonance. According to the theory, people are motivated to reduce this
tension, and attitude change is a likely outcome. Dissonance theory suggests
that the less reward people receive for a behavior, the more compelled they
feel to provide their own justification for it, especially if they believe they
have freely chosen it. Similarly, the more they are rewarded, the more they
infer that the behavior is suspect. The latter is known as the
reverse-incentive effect.
Additionally,
cognitive dissonance theory states that an individual will experience
dissonance after making a decision between two mutually exclusive, equally
attractive alternatives. This is known as postdecision dissonance.
Another,
more recent view suggests that cognitive dissonance results not so much from
inconsistency as from the feeling of personal responsibility that occurs when
inconsistent actions produce negative consequences.
10. What
is self-perception theory?
One alternative to cognitive dissonance
theory is self-perception theory, which argues that behavior and attitude
change can be explained without assuming that people are motivated to reduce
the tension supposedly produced by inconsistency.
Instead,
self-perception assumes that people are not self-conscious processors of
information. They simply observe their own behavior and assume that their
attitudes must be consistent with that behavior.
11. What
is self-affirmation theory?
Another
alternative to cognitive dissonance, self-affirmation theory explains how
people deal with the tension that dissonant thoughts or behaviors provoke.
Self-affirmation theory suggests that people may not try to reduce dissonance
if they can maintain their self-concept by proving that they are adequate in
other ways—that is, by affirming an unrelated and positive part of the self.
12. What
is psychological reactance?
Individuals
may reduce psychological tension in another way as well. When people realize
they have been coerced into doing or buying something against their will, they
sometimes try to regain or reassert their freedom. This response is called
psychological reactance.
13. What
is propaganda?
Propaganda
is defined as a deliberate attempt to persuade people, by any available media,
to think in a manner desired by the source. The internal characteristics of
propaganda refer to the psychological makeup of the targets of propaganda. In
order for propaganda to be effective, the propagandist must know which
attitudes, sentiments, and behaviors can be easily manipulated. Deeply held
beliefs are commonly left alone. The external characteristics of propaganda
refer to the characteristics of the propaganda itself. In order for propaganda
to be maximally effective, it must be organized and total.
14. How
are the tactics of propaganda used on a mass scale?
Propagandists
use a variety of techniques to persuade the masses. These include use of
stereotypes, substitution of names, selection of facts, downright lying,
repetition, assertion, pinpointing an enemy, and appeals to authority.
*********************************************
Social Psychology
Third Edition
Kenneth S. Bordens Indiana University—Purdue University Fort Wayne
Irwin A. Horowitz - Oregon State University
Social Psychology, 3rd Edition
Copyright ©2008 by Freeload Press
Illustration used on cover © 2008 JupiterImages Corporation
ISBN 1-930789-04-1
No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or
by any means, electronic, mechanical, recording, photocopying, or otherwise, without the prior written
permission of the publisher.
Printed in the United States of America by Freeload Press.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Komentar
Posting Komentar