8 Group Processes 281
8 Group Processes
Never
doubt that a small group of thoughtful committed people can change the world:
indeed
it’s the
only thing that ever has!
—Margaret
Mead
The
mission was supposed to be the crown jewel of the American space program. The
Challengermission was supposed to show how safe space travel had become by
sending along Christa McAuliffe, a teacher from Concord, New Hampshire, who
would become the fi rst civilian in space. She was supposed to teach a
15-minute class from space. The Challenger mission was supposed to be a success
just like the 55 previous U.S. space flights. But, what wasn’t supposed to
happen actually did: Fifty-eight seconds into the flight, the trouble started;
a puff of smoke could be seen coming from one of the solid rocket boosters.
About 73 seconds into the flight, Challenger exploded in a huge fi reball that
spread debris over several miles. The crew cockpit plummeted back to earth and
hit the Atlantic Ocean, killing all seven astronauts. As millions of people
watched, the two solid rocket boosters spiraled off in different directions,
making the image of the letter “y” in smoke. The pattern formed would
foreshadow the main question that was on everyone’s mind in the days that
followed the tragedy: Why?
The answer
to this question proved to be complex indeed. The actual physical cause of the
explosion was clear. Hot gasses burned through a rubber O-ring that was
supposed to seal two segments of the solid rocket booster. Because of the
exceptionally cold temperatures on the morning of the launch, the O-rings
became brittle and did not fi t properly. Hot gasses burned through and ignited
the millions of gallons of liquid fuel on top of which Challengersat. The
underlying cause of the explosion, relating to the decision-making structure
and process at NASA and Morton Thiokol (the maker of the solid rocket booster),
took months to disentangle. What emerged was a picture of a fl awed
decision-making structure that did not foster open communication and free
exchange of data. This flawed decisionmaking structure was the true cause for
the Challengerexplosion. At the top of the decision-making ladder was Jesse
Moore, Associate Administrator for
Space
Flight. It was Mr. Moore who made the fi nal decision to launch or not to
launch. Also in a top decision-making position was Arnold Aldrich, Space
Shuttle Manager at the Johnson Space Center. At the bottom of the ladder were
the scientists and engineers at Morton Thiokol. These individuals did not have
direct access to Moore. Any information they wished to convey concerning the
launch had to be passed along by executives at Morton Thiokol, who would then
communicate with NASA offi cials at the Marshall Space Flight Center. Some
people had one set of facts, others had a different set, and sometimes they did
not share. The Thiokol scientists and engineers had serious reservations about
launching Challenger. In fact, one of the engineers later said that he “knew”
that the shuttle would explode and felt sick when it happened.
In
addition to the communication fl aws, the group involved in making the decision
suffered from other decision-making defi ciencies, including a sense of
invulnerability (after all, all other shuttle launches went off safely),
negative attitudes toward one another (characterizing the scientists and
engineers as overly cautious), and an atmosphere that stifl ed free expression
of ideas (Thiokol engineer Alan McDonald testifi ed before congressional
hearings that he felt pressured to give the green light to the launch). What
went wrong? Here we had a group of highly intelligent, expert individuals who
made a disastrous decision to launch Challengerin the cold weather that existed
at launch time.
In this
chapter, we explore the effects of groups on individuals. We ask, What special
characteristics distinguish a group like the Challengerdecision-making group
from a simple gathering of individuals? What forces arise within such groups
that change individual behavior? Do groups offer signifi cant advantages over
individuals operating on their own? For example, would the launch director at
NASA have been better off making a decision by himself rather than assembling
and relying on an advisory group? And what are the group dynamics that can lead
to such faulty, disastrous decisions? These are some of the questions addressed
in this chapter.
What Is a
Group?
Groups are
critical to our everyday existence. We are born into a group, we play in
groups, and we work and learn in groups. We have already learned that we gain
much of our self-identity and self-esteem from our group memberships. But what
is a group? Is it simply a collection of individuals who happen to be at the
same place at the same time? If this were the case, the people standing on a
street corner waiting for a bus would be a group. Your social psychology class
has many people in it, some of whom may know one another. Some people interact,
some do not. Is it a group? Well, it is certainly an aggregate, a gathering of
people, but it probably does not feel to you like a group.
Groups
have special social and psychological characteristics that set them apart from
collections or aggregates of individuals. Two major features distin guish
groups:
In a
group, members interact with each other, and group members influence each other
through this social interaction. By this definition, the collection of people
at the bus stop would not qualify as a group. Although they may influence one
another on a basic level (if one person looked up to the sky, others probably
would follow suit), they do not truly interact. A true group has two or more
individuals who mutually influence one another through social interaction
(Forsyth, 1990). That is, the influence arises out of the information (verbal
and nonverbal) that members exchange. The Challenger decision-making group
certainly fit this definition. The group members interacted during committee
meetings, and they clearly influenced one another.
This
definition of a group may seem broad and ambiguous, and in fact, it is often
difficult to determine whether an aggregate of individuals qualifies as a
group. To refine our definition and to get a closer look at groups, we turn now
to a closer look at their characteristics.
Characteristics
of Groups
Interaction
and mutual influence among people in the group are only two of a number of
attributes that characterize a group. What are the others?
First of
all, a group typically has a purpose, a reason for existing. Groups serve many
functions, but a general distinction can be made between instrumental groups
and affiliative groups. Instrumental groups exist to perform some task or reach
some specific goal. The Challenger group was an instrumental group, as are most
decisionmaking groups. A jury is also an instrumental group. Its sole purpose
is to find the truth of the claims presented in a courtroom and reach a
verdict. Once this goal is reached, the jury disperses.
Affiliative
groups exist for more general and, often, more social reasons. For example, you
might join a fraternity or a sorority simply because you want to be a part of
that group—to affiliate with people with whom you would like to be. You may
identify closely with the values and ideals of such a group. You derive
pleasure, self-esteem, and perhaps even prestige by affiliating with the group.
A second
characteristic of a group is that group members share perceptions of how they
are to behave. From these shared perceptions emerge group norms, or
expectations about what is acceptable behavior. As pointed out in Chapter 7,
norms can greatly influence individual behavior. For example, the parents of
the children on a soccer team might develop into a group on the sidelines of
the playing fields. Over the course of the season or several seasons, they
learn what kinds of comments they can make to the coach, how much and what kind
of interaction is expected among the parents, how to cheer and support the
players, what they can call out during a game, what to wear, what to bring for
snacks, and so on. A parent who argued with a referee or coach or who used
abusive language would quickly be made to realize he or she was not conforming
to group norms.
Third,
within a true group, each member has a particular job or role to play in the
accomplishment of the group’s goals. Sometimes, these roles are
formally defined; for example,
a chairperson of a committee has specific duties. However, roles may also be
informal (DeLamater, 1974). Even when no one has been officially appointed
leader, for example, one or two people usually emerge to take command or gently
guide the group along. Among the soccer parents, one person might gradually
take on additional responsibilities, such as organizing carpools or
distributing information from the coach, and thus come to take on the role of
leader.
Fourth,
members of a group have affective (emotional) ties to others in the group.
These ties are influenced by how well various members live up to group norms
and how much other group members like them (DeLamater, 1974).
Finally,
group members are interdependent. That is, they need each other to meet the
group’s needs and goals. For example, a fraternity or a sorority
will fall apart if members
do not follow the rules and adhere to the norms so that members can be
comfortable with each other.
What Holds
a Group Together?
Once a
group is formed, what forces hold it together? Group cohesiveness—the strength
of the relationships that link the members of the group (Forsyth, 1990)—is
essentially what keeps people in the group. Cohesiveness is influenced by
several factors:
1. Group members’mutual attraction.
Groups may be cohesive because the members
find one another attractive or friendly. Whatever causes people to like one
another increases group cohesiveness (Levine & Moreland, 1990).
2. Members’propinquity (physical closeness, as when they live or work
near each other). Sometimes, simply being around people regularly is enough to
make people feel that they belong to a group. The various departments in an
insurance company—marketing, research, sales, and so on—may think of themselves
as groups.
3. Their adherence to group norms. When members
live up to group norms without resistance, the group is more cohesive than when
one or two members deviate a lot or when many members deviate a little.
4. The group’s success at moving toward its goals. Groups
that succeed at reaching their goals are obviously more satisfying for their
members and, therefore, more cohesive than those that fail. If groups do not
achieve what the members wish for the group, they cease to exist or at the very
least are reorganized.
5. Members’identification with the group: group
loyalty: The success of a group will
often depend on the degree of loyalty its member have to that group.Van Vugt
and Hart (2004)investigated the role of social identity (how strongly the
members identified with the group) in developing group loyalty,defined as
staying in the group when members can obtain better outcomes by leaving their
group. In one experiment, high (vs. low) group identifiers expressed a stronger
desire to stay in the group even in the presence of an attractive (vs.
unattractive) exit option. Other results revealed that high identifiers’group
loyalty is explained by
an extremely positive impression of their group membership even if other groups
might offer more rewards. Social identity seems to act as social glue. It
provides stability in groups that might otherwise collapse.
How and
Why Do Groups Form?
We know
that humans have existed in groups since before the dawn of history. Clearly,
then, groups have survival value. Groups form because they meet needs that we
cannot satisfy on our own. Let’s take a closer look at what these needs
are.
Meeting
Basic Needs
Groups
help us meet a variety of needs. In many cases, these needs, whether
biological, psychological, or social, cannot be separated from one another.
There are obvious advantages to group membership. Psychology is developing an
evolutionary perspective, and evolutionary social psychologists view groups as
selecting individual characteristics that make it more probable that an
individual can function and survive in groups
(Caporael,
1997; Pinker, 2002). Couched in terms of natural selection, evolution would
favor those who preferred groups to those who preferred to live in isolation.
But groups
meet more than biological needs. They also meet psychological needs. Our first
experiences occur within the context of the family group. Some people believe
that our adult reactions to groups stem from our feelings about our family.
That is, we react toward group leaders with much the same feelings we have
toward our fathers or mothers (Schultz, 1983). Many recruits to religious cults
that demand extreme devotion are searching for a surrogate family (McCauley
& Segal, 1987).
Groups
also satisfy a variety of social needs, such as social support—the comfort and
advice of others—and protection from loneliness. Groups make it easier for people
to deal with anxiety and stress. Human beings are social beings; we don’t
do very well when we
are isolated. In fact, research shows that social isolation—the absence of
meaningful social contact—is as strongly associated with death as is cigarette
smoking or lack of exercise (Brannon & Feist, 1992).
Groups
also satisfy the human need for social comparison. We compare our feelings,
opinions, and behaviors with those of other people, particularly when we are
unsure about how to act or think (Festinger, 1954). We compare ourselves to
others who are similar to us to get accurate information about what to do.
Those in the groups with which we affiliate often suggest to us the books we
read, the movies we see, and the clothes we wear.
Social
comparison also helps us obtain comforting information (Taylor & Brown,
1988). Students, for example, may be better able to protect their self-esteem
when they know that others in the class also did poorly on an exam. B students
compare themselves favorably with C students, and D students compare themselves
with those who failed. We are relieved to find out that some others did even
worse than we did. This is downward comparison, the process of comparing our
standing with that of those less fortunate.
As noted
earlier, groups play a large role in influencing individual self-esteem. In
fact, individuals craft their self-concept from all the groups with which they
identify and in which they hold membership, whether the group is a softball
team, a sorority, or a street gang.
Of course,
groups are also a practical social invention. Group members can pool their
resources, draw on the experience of others, and solve problems that they may
not be able to solve on their own. Some groups, such as families, form an
economic and social whole that functions as a unit in the larger society.
Roles in
Groups
Not all
members are expected to do the same things or obey precisely the same norms.
The group often has different expectations for different group members. These
shared expectations help to define individual roles, such as team captain (a
formal role) or newcomer (an informal role) (Levine & Moreland, 1990).
Newcomers
Group
members can play different roles in accordance with their seniority. Newcomers
are expected to obey the group’s rules and standards of behavior (its
norms) and show that
they are committed to being good members (Moreland & Levine, 1989).
Moresenior members have “idiosyncratic” credit and can occasionally stray from
group norms (Hollander, 1985). They have proven their worth to the group and
have “banked” that credit. Every now and then, it is all right for them to
depart from acceptable behavior and spend that credit. New members have no such
credit. The best chance new members have of being accepted by a group is to behave
in a passive and anxious way.
Deviates
What
happens when the new members find that the group does not meet their hopes or
the senior members feel the recruit has not met the group’s
expectations? The group may
try to take some corrective action by putting pressure on the member to
conform. Groups will spend much time trying to convince someone who does not
live up to group norms to change (Schachter, 1951). If the deviate does not
come around, the group then disowns him or her. The deviate, however, usually
bows to group pressure and conforms to group norms (Levine, 1989).
Deviates
are rejected most when they interfere with the functioning of the group
(Kruglanski & Webster, 1991). Imagine an advisor to the launch director at
NASA objecting to the launch of Challenger after the decision had been made. No
matter how persuasive the person’s objection to the launch, it is very
likely that the deviate would
have been told to be silent; he or she would have been interfering with the
group’s ability to
get the job done. Experimental research has verified that when a group member
dissents from a group decision close to the group’s deadline for
solving a problem, the
rejector is more likely to be condemned than if the objection is stated earlier
(Kruglanski
&
Webster, 1991).
How Do Groups Influence the Behavior
of Individuals?
We have
considered why people join groups and what roles individuals play in groups.
Now let’s consider another question: What effect does being in a group
have on individual behavior and performance? Does group membership lead to
self-enhancement, as
people who join groups seem to believe? Does it have other effects? Some social
psychologists have been particularly interested in investigating this question.
They have looked not just at the effects of membership in true groups but also
at the effects of being evaluated by an audience, of being in an audience, and
of being in a crowd.
Recall
that groups affect the way we think and act even when we only imagine how they
are going to respond to us. If you practice a speech, just imagining that large
audience in front of you is enough to make you nervous. The actual presence of
an audience affects us even more. But how? Let’s take a look.
The
Effects of an Audience on Performance
Does an
audience make you perform better? Or does it make you “choke”? The answer seems
to depend, at least in part, on how good you are at what you are doing. The
presence of others seems to help when the performer is doing something he or
she does well: when the performance is a dominant, well-learned skill, a
behavior that is easy or familiar (Zajonc, 1965). If you are a class-A tennis
player, for example, your serve may be better when people are watching you. The
performance-enhancing effect of an audience on your behavior is known as social
facilitation. If, however, you are performing a nondominant skill, one that is
not very well learned, then the presence of an audience detracts from your
performance. This effect is known as social inhibition.
The social
facilitation effect—the strengthening of a dominant response due to the
presence of other people—has been demonstrated in a wide range of species,
including roaches, ants, chicks, and humans (Zajonc, Heingartner, & Herman,
1969). Humans doing a simple task perform better in the presence of others. On
a more difficult task, the presence of others inhibits performance.
Why does
this happen? How does an audience cause us to perform better or worse than we
do when no one is watching? Psychologists have several alternative
explanations.
Increased
Arousal
Zajonc
(1965) argued that a performer’s effort always increases in the presence of others due to
increased arousal. Increased arousal increases effort; the consequent increased
effort improves performance when the behavior is dominant and impairs
performance when the behavior is nondominant. If you are good at tennis, then
increased arousal and, therefore, increased effort make you play better. If you
are not a good tennis player, the increased arousal and increased effort
probably will inhibit your performance (Figure 8.1).
Figure
8.1The arousal model of social facilitation. The presence of others is a source
of arousal and increased effort. This increase in arousal and effort
facilitates a simple, well-learned task but inhibits a complex, not
well-learned task.
Evaluation
Apprehension
An
alternative explanation for the effects of an audience on performance centers
not so much on the increased effort that comes from arousal but on the
judgments we perceive others to be making about our performance. A theater
audience, for example, does not simply receive a play passively. Instead,
audience members sit in judgment of the actors, even if they are only armchair
critics. The kind of arousal this situation produces is known as evaluation
apprehension. Some social scientists believe that evaluation apprehension is
what causes differences in performance when an audience is present (Figure
8.2).
Figure
8.2The evaluation apprehension model of social facilitation. According to this
model, audience-related arousal is caused by apprehension about being
evaluated.
Those who
favor evaluation apprehension as an explanation of social facilitation and
social inhibition suggest that the presence of others will cause arousal only
when they can reward or punish the performer (Geen, 1989). The mere presence of
others does not seem to be sufficient to account for social facilitation and
social inhibition (Cottrell, 1972). In one experiment, when the audience was
made up of blindfolded or inattentive persons, social facilitation of
performance did not occur. That is, if the audience could not see the
performance, or did not care about it, then evaluation apprehension did not
occur, nor did social facilitation or social inhibition (Cottrell, Wack,
Sekerak, & Rittle, 1968).
The
Distraction-Conflict Effect
Another
explanation of the presence-of-others effect is distraction-conflict
theory(Baron, 1986). According to this theory, arousal results from a conflict
between demands for attention from the task and demands for attention from the
audience. There are three main points to the theory. First, the presence of
other people distracts attention from the task. Our tennis player gets all
kinds of attention-demanding cues—rewards and punishments—from those watching
him play. He may be aware of his parents, his exgirlfriend, his tennis coach,
an attractive stranger, and his annoying little brother out there in the crowd.
This plays havoc with a mediocre serve. Second, distraction leads to conflicts
in his attention. Our tennis player has just so much attentional capacity. All
of this capacity ought to be focused on throwing the ball in the air and
hitting it across the net. But his attention is also focused on those he knows
in the crowd. Third, the conflict between these two claims for attention
stresses the performer and raises the arousal level (Figure 8.3).
Figure
8.3The distraction-conflict model of social facilitation. According to this
model, the source of arousal in facilitation situation is related to the
conflict between paying attention to the task and the audience at the same
time.
Group
Performance: Conditions That Decrease or Increase
Motivation
of Group Members
We have
seen that being watched affects how we perform. Let’s
take this a step further and
examine how being a member of a group affects our performance.
We noted
earlier that people who join groups do so largely for self-enhancement:
They
believe that group membership will improve them in some way. They will become
better speakers, better citizens, better soccer players, better dancers or
singers; they will meet people and expand their social circle; they will make a
contribution to a cause, a political candidate, or society. Does group
membership actually lead to improved performance? Or does it detract from
individual effort and achievement, giving people the opportunity to
underperform? Both effects have been documented.
Enhanced
Performance
Imagine
that you are a bicycling enthusiast. Three times a week you ride 20 miles,
which takes you a little over an hour. One day you happen to come on a group of
cyclists and decide to ride along with them. When you look at your time for the
20 miles, you find that your time is under 1 hour, a full 10 minutes under your
best previous time. How can you account for your increased speed? Did the other
riders simply act as a windshield for you, allowing you to exert less effort
and ride faster? Or is there more to this situation than aerodynamics? Could it
be that the mere presence of others somehow affected your behavior?
This
question was asked by Norman Triplett, one of the early figures in social
psychology (1898). Triplett, a cycling enthusiast, decided to test a theory
that the presence of other people was sufficient to increase performance. He
used a laboratory in which alternative explanations for the improvement in
cycling time (e.g., other riders being a windshield) could be eliminated. He
also conducted what is perhaps the first social psychological experiment. He
had children engage in a simulated race on a miniature track. Ribbons were
attached to fishing reels. By winding the reels, the children could drag
ribbons around a miniature racetrack. Triplett had the children perform the
task either alone or in pairs. He found that the children who played the game
in the presence of another child completed the task more quickly than children
who played the game alone. The improved performance of the children and the
cyclist when they participate in a group setting rather than alone gives us some
evidence that groups do enhance individual performance.
Social
Loafing and Free Rides
Is it true
that the presence of others is always arousing and that participating in a
group always leads to enhanced individual performance? Perhaps not. In fact,
the opposite may occur. Sometimes when we are in a group situation, we relax
our efforts and rely on others to take up the slack. This effect is called
social loafing.
Sometimes,
people are not more effortful in the presence of others; they, in fact, may
loaf when working with others in groups (Harkins & Szymanski, 1987; Latané,
Williams, & Harkins, 1979; Williams & Karau, 1991). In one experiment,
participants were informed that they had to shout as loudly as they could to
test the effects of sensory feedback on the ability of groups to produce sound.
The researchers compared the noise produced by individuals who thought they
were shouting or clapping alone to the noise they made when they thought they
were in a group. If groups did as well as individuals, then the group
production would at least equal the sum of the individual production. But the
research findings showed that groups did not produce as much noise as the
combined amount of noise individuals made (Latané et al., 1979). Some group
members did not do as much as they were capable of doing as individuals: They
loafed. In some instances, then, participation of others in the task (e.g., in
a tug-of-war game) lowers individual motivation and reduces performance on the
task. Simply put, people sometimes exert less effort when working on a task in
a group context (Harkins & Petty, 1982).
Why should
the group reduce individual performance in some cases and enhance it in others?
The nature of the task may encourage social loafing. In a game of tug-ofwar, if
you do not pull the rope as hard as you can, who will know or care? If you don’t
shout as loud as you
can, what difference does it make? You cannot accurately assess your own
contribution, nor can other people evaluate how well you are performing. Also,
fatigue increases social loafing. Hoeksema-van Orden and her coworkers had a
group of people work for 20 hours continuously, individually or in a group.
These researchers found that fatigue increased social loafing in groups,
whereas individuals were less likely to loaf even when fatigued (Hoeksema-van
Orden, Galillard, & Buunk, 1998).
Social
loafing tends not to occur in very important tasks. However, many of our
everyday tasks are repetitive and dull and are vulnerable to social loafing
(Karau & Williams, 1993).
Regardless
of the task, some individuals work harder than others in groups (Kerr, 1983).
Free riders do not do their share of the work. Why not? They are cynical about
the other members; they think others may be holding back, so they hold back
also. People do not want to be suckers, doing more than their share while
others take it easy. Even if they know that their coworkers are doing their
share and are competent, individuals may look for a free ride (Williams &
Karau, 1991).
The larger
the group, the more common are social loafing and free riding. It is harder to
determine individual efforts and contributions in big groups. People are likely
to feel more responsible for the outcome in smaller groups (Kerr, 1989). Of
course, not everyone loafs in groups, nor do people loaf in all group
situations.
Motivation
Gains in Groups: Social Compensation and the Kohler Effect While social loafi
ng shows that being in a group may decrease some members’motivation
to perform, that is not
always the case. What decreases the likelihood of social loafing? It is less
likely to occur if individuals feel that it is important to compensate for
other, weaker group members (Williams & Karau, 1991). When the task is
important and motivation to perform is high, then social compensation—working
harder to make up for the weakness of others—seems to overcome the tendency
toward social loafing and free riding.
Social
loafing is also less likely when individual contributions can be clearly
identified. Generally, when individuals can be identified and cannot simply
blend in with the background of other workers, they are less likely to loaf
(Williams, Harkins, & Latané, 1981). The members of an automobile
manufacturing team, for example, are more careful about their tasks and less
willing to pass on defective work if they have to sign for each piece they do.
If responsibility for defects is clear, if positive effort and contribution are
rewarded, and if management punishes free riders, then social loafing will be
further diminished (Shepperd, 1993). Similarly, Shepperd and Taylor (1999)
showed that if group members perceive a strong relationship between their
effort and a favorable outcome for the group, social loafing does not happen,
and there are no free riders.
Social
loafing is a phenomenon that is very robust and occurs in a variety of
situations and cultures (Forgas, Williams, & von Hippel, 2004; Karau &
Williams, 1993). It has been found to be more common among men than women and
among members of Eastern as opposed to Western cultures. These cultural and
gender differences seem to be related to values. Many women and many
individuals in Eastern cultures attach more importance to group harmony and
group success and satisfaction. Many men, especially in Western cultures,
attach more value to individual advancement and rewards and to other people’s
evaluations. Groups tend to mask individual differences. For this reason, Western men may have less
inclination to perform well in group situations. The result is social loafing
(Karau & Williams, 1993).
Karau and
Williams have therefore shown that groups do not necessarily generate
conditions that depress some individual members’motivations to
perform well. Recently, Kerr
and his coworkers have rediscovered another motivational gain in groups known
as the Kohler effect(Kerr & Tindale, 2005; Kerr, Messe, Parke, &
Sambolec, 2005;
Messe et
al., 2002). These researchers rediscovered work done by Kohler (1926) in which
the researcher reported that a less-capable member of a two-person group (a dyad)
working together on a task works harder and performs better than expected when
the group product is to be a result of the combined (conjunctive) effort of the
two members. This seems to be the opposite of social loafing. The weaker member
of the group, rather than free-riding or loafing, in fact increases his or her
effort. For, example, Kohler found that members of a Berlin rowing club worked
harder at a physical performance task as part of a two- or three-man crew than
when they performed as individuals. Hertel et al. (2000) called this a Kohler
motivation gain. The question then was how this Kohler motivation gain occurs.
It is
possible in a small group (and two or three is as small as one can get) that
the least-competent member “knows” that her performance is crucial to a good
group outcome. Or, conceivably, the weakest member might feel that she is in
competition with the other members. These were but two of the possible
motivations for the Kohler effect that Kerr et al. (2005) examined in their research.
Kerr and his colleagues reasoned that the amount of feedback individuals were
given with respect to their performance might be the crucial factor. For
example, if you are not as good at the task as the other members, information
about how the better members are doing should affect your effort and
performance. So Kerr et al. (2005) varied the amount of feedback individuals
were provided with. The results revealed that knowledge about level of
performance (feedback) was not necessary for the Kohler effect (increase
performance by the weaker member of the dyad). However, if the group members
were anonymous and were given absolutely no feedback about performance, then
motivation gain was wiped out.
With no
information about the effect of the weakest member’s
contribution and no possibility
for recognition, there is no motivation gain. Well, that’s
not surprising. So it
appears that motivation gains in groups may occur due in part to social
comparison effects, in which there is some competition between two group
members, as well as the personal motivation of the weakest member to see how
well that member can perform (Kerr et al., 2005).
Groups,
Self-Identity, and Intergroup Relationships
Groups not
only affect how we perform, but they also influence our individual sense of
worth—our self-esteem—which, in turn, has an impact on how one group relates to
other groups in a society. In 1971, Tajfel and his colleagues showed that group
categorizations, along with an in-group identification, are both necessary and sufficient
conditions for groups to discriminate against other groups (Rubin &
Hewstone, 1998). Recall that in Chapter 4 Tajfel showed that even if people
were randomly assigned to a group (minimal group categorization), they tended
to favor members of that group when distributing very small rewards (the
in-group bias; Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, 1971). For example, boys
in a minimal group experiment (“you overestimated the number of dots on a
screen and, therefore, you are in the overestimator group”) gave more money to
members of their group (the in-group) than to members of the underestimator
group (the out-group). Therefore, even the most minimal group situation appears
to be sufficient for an in-group bias (favoring members of your group) to occur.
Tajfel’s
findings suggested to him that individuals obtain part of their self-concept, their social identity, from their
group memberships and that they seek to nourish a positive social (group)
identity to heighten their own self-esteem. Groups that are successful and are
held in high esteem by society enhance the esteem of its members. The opposite
is also true. All of this depends on the social comparison with relevant
out-groups on issues that are important to both (Mummendey & Wenzel, 1999).
Favorable comparisons enhance the group and its members. Social identity, then,
is a definition of the self in terms of group membership (Brewer, 1993;
Caporael, 1997). Changes in the fate of the group imply changes in the
self-concept of the individual members.
Tajfel’s
theory is called self-identity theory (SIT) and proposes that a number of factors predict one group’s
reaction to other competing groups in society. It pertains to what may arise from
identification with a social category (membership in a social, political,
racial, religious group, etc.). It does not say that once we identify with a
group, we inevitably will discriminate against other groups. However, SIT does
lay out the conditions under which such discrimination may take place.
Generally, SIT assumes that the potential that one group will tend to
discriminate or downgrade another group
will be
affected by four factors:
1. How strongly the in-group members identify
with their group
2. The importance of the social category that
the in-group represents
3. The dimension on which the groups are
competing (the more important the
dimension,
the greater the potential for conflict)
4. The group’s relative status and the difference in status between
the in-group and the out-group (Oakes, Haslam, & Turner, 1994)
Therefore,
if members strongly identify with the group; if the group represents a crucial
identification category—say, race, religion, or more affiliative groups such as
a social organization; if the competition occurs on a crucial dimension (jobs,
college entrance possibilities, intense sports rivalries); and if the result
can be expected to affect the status of the group relative to its competitor,
SIT predicts intergroup discrimination. Low or threatened self-esteem will
increase intergroup discrimination because of the need to enhance one’s
social identity (Hogg & Abrams, 1990). Groups that are successful in
intergroup discrimination will enhance social
identity and self-esteem (Rubin & Hewstone, 1998).
When
self-esteem is threatened by group failure, people tend to respond in ways that
can maintain their positive identity and sense of reality. For example, Duck
and her colleagues examined the response of groups in a hotly contested
political campaign. These researchers found that individuals who strongly
identified with their political party were more likely to see the media
coverage of the campaign as biased and favoring the other side (Duck, Terry,
& Hogg, 1998). This was particularly strong for members of the weaker
political party, as SIT would predict, because the weaker party was more
threatened. However, when the weaker party won, they were less likely to think
that the media were biased, whereas the losing, stronger party began to think
the media were biased against them.
A member
who threatens the success of a group also threatens the positive image of the
group. This leads to the black-sheep effect, the observation that whereas an
attractive in-group member is rated more highly than an attractive member of an
out-group, an unattractive in-group member is perceived more negatively than an
unattractive out-group member (Marques & Paez, 1994). The SIT inference is
that the unattractive in-group member is a serious threat to the in-group’s
image (Mummendey &Wenzel,
1999).
The Power
of Groups to Punish: Social Ostracism
Although
groups may serve to increase our self-esteem by enhancing our social identity,
groups have the power to exact painful, even dreadful, punishment. Baumeister
and Leary (1995) observed that there is little in life so frightful as being
excluded from groups that are important to us. Most of us spend much of our
time in the presence of other people. The presence of others provides us not
only with opportunities for positive interactions but also for risks of being
ignored, excluded, and rejected. Kipling Williams (Williams, Fogas, & von
Hippel, 2005; Zadro, Williams, & Richardson, 2004) provided an innovative
approach to the study of the effects of being ignored or rejected by the group.
Such behavior is called social ostracism and is defined by Williams as the act
of excluding or ignoring other individuals or groups. This behavior is
widespread and universal. Williams noted that organizations, employers,
coworkers, friends, and family all may ignore or disengage from people (the
silent treatment) to punish, control, and vent anger. The pervasiveness of
ostracism is reflected by a survey conducted by Williams and his coworkers that
showed that 67% of the sample surveyed said they had used the silent treatment
(deliberately not speaking to a person in their presence) on a loved one, and
75% indicated that they had been a target of the silent treatment by a loved
one (Faulkner & Williams, 1995). As you might imagine, the silent treatment
is a marker of a relationship that is disintegrating. From the point of view of
the victim of this silent treatment, social ostracism is the perception of
being ignored by others in the victim’s presence (Williams & Zadro, 2001).
Williams
and his colleague Sommer identified several forms of ostracism (Williams &
Sommer, 1997). First, they distinguish between social and physical ostracism.
Physical ostracism includes solitary confinement, exile, or the time-out room
in grade school. Social ostracism is summed up by phrases we all know: the cold
shoulder, the silent treatment.
In the
social psychological realm, punitive ostracism and defensive ostracism are
among the various guises ostracism may take. Punitive ostracism refers to
behaviors (ignoring, shunning) that are perceived by the victim as intended to
be deliberate and harmful. Sometimes, Williams and Sommer pointed out, people
also engage in defensive ostracism, a kind of preemptive strike when you think
someone might feel negatively toward you.
The
purpose of ostracism from the point of view of the ostracizer is clear:
controlling the behavior of the victim. Ostracizers also report being rewarded
when they see that their tactics are working. Certainly, defensive ostracism,
ignoring someone before they can harm you or ignore you, seems to raise the
self-esteem of the ostracizer (Sommer, Williams, Ciarocco, & Baumeister,
1999).
Williams
developed a number of creative methods to induce the perception of being
ostracized in laboratory experiments. Williams and Sommer (1997) used a
balltossing game in which two individuals working as confederates of the
experimenters either included or socially ostracized a participant during a
5-minute ball-tossing game. Participants who were waiting for a group activity
to begin were placed in a waiting room that happened to have a number of
objects, including a ball. Three people were involved, the two confederates and
the unknowing research participant. All participants were thrown the ball
during the first minute, but those in the ostracized condition were not thrown
the ball during the remaining 4 minutes. The experimenter then returned to
conduct the second part of the study.
After the
ball-tossing ended in the Williams and Sommer (1997) experiment, participants
were asked to think of as many uses for an object as possible within a
specified time limit. They performed this task in the same room either
collectively (in which they were told that only the group effort would be
recorded) or coactively (in which their own individual performances would be
compared to that of the other group members) with the two confederates.
Williams and Sommer predicted that ostracized targets—those excluded from the
ball tossing—would try to regain a sense of belonging by working comparatively
harder on the collective task, thereby contributing to the group’s
success. Williams and
Sommer found support for this hypothesis, but only for female participants.
Whether they were ostracized in the ball-tossing task, males displayed social
loafing by being less productive when working collectively than when working
coactively. Females, however, behaved quite differently, depending on whether
they had been ostracized or included. When included, they tended to work about
as hard collectively as coactively, but when ostracized, they were actually more
productive when working collectively compared to when they worked coactively.
Women also
demonstrated that they were interested in regaining a sense of being a valued
member of the group by displaying nonverbal commitment (i.e., leaning forward,
smiling), whereas males tended to employ face-saving techniques such as combing
their hair, looking through their wallets, and manipulating objects, all in the
service of being “cool” and showing that they were unaffected by the ostracism.
We can conclude that ostracism did threaten sense of belonging for both males
and females, but ostracized females tried to regain a sense of belonging,
whereas males acted to regain self-esteem (Williams & Sommer, 1997;
Williams et al., 2005).
Ostracism
is not limited to face-to-face contacts. The power of ostracism is observed
even in computer games in which one player is excluded from a ball-tossing
(Internet) computer game called cyberball(Zadro et al., 2004). At a
predetermined point in the game, one of the players is excluded. That is, the
other players no longer “throw” the ball to that person. Players that are
excluded report a loss of self-esteem. A study by Smith and Williams (2004)
also reported that the negative effects of ostracism are not limited to
face-to-face contacts. The power of ostracism can also be felt via text
messages on cell phones. Smith and Williams (2004) in the text message study
devised a three-way interaction via cell phones in which all three people are
initially included in the text messaging. However, in one of the conditions of
the study, one participant is excluded from the conversation. That person no
longer received any direct messages nor did the person see the messages
exchanged between the other two text messengers. Those excluded reported feeling
lower levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and “meaningful existence”
(Smith & Williams, 2004).
Deindividuation
and Anonymity: The Power of Groups
to Do
Violence
Although
ostracism refers to essentially psychological methods of exclusion from the
group, other more dangerous behaviors occur in group settings. We have seen
that when certain individuals feel they can’t be identified by
their actions or achievements, they
tend to loaf. This is a common group effect. A decline in individual identity
seems to mean a decline in a person’s sense of responsibility. Anonymity can alter people’s
ethical and moral
behavior.
Observers
of group behavior have long known that certain kinds of groups have the
potential for great mischief. Groups at sporting events have engaged in murder
and mayhem when their soccer teams have lost. One element present in such
groups is that the individuals are not easily identifiable. People get lost in
the mass and seem to lose their self-identity and self-awareness. Social psychologists
have called this loss of inhibition while engulfed in a group deindividuation
(Zimbardo, 1969).
People who
are deindividuated seem to become less aware of their own moral standards and
are much more likely to respond to violent or aggressive cues (PrenticeDunn
& Rogers, 1989). In fact, deindividuated people are quick to respond to any
cues. Research suggests that when people are submerged in a group, they become
impulsive, aroused, and wrapped up in the cues of the moment (Spivey & Prentice-Dunn,
1990). Their action is determined by whatever the group does.
Groups and
organizations whose primary purpose involves violence often attempt to
deindividuate their members. Certainly, the white sheets covering the members
of the Ku Klux Klan are a prime example of this. So, too, are the training
methods of most military organizations. Uniforms serve to lower a sense of
self-awareness and make it easier to respond to aggressive cues.
There is
some evidence that the larger the group, the more likely it is that individual
group members will deindividuate. Differences have been found in the behavior
of larger and smaller crowds that gather when a troubled person is threatening
to leap from a building or bridge (Mann, 1981). Out of 21 such cases examined,
in 10, the crowds baited the victim to jump, whereas in the remaining 11, the
victim was not taunted and was often rescued. What was the difference between
these two sorts of cases?
The
baiting crowds tended to be larger—over 300 people. The baiting episodes were
more likely to take place after dark, and the victim was usually situated
higher up, typically above the 12th floor. Additionally, the longer the episode
continued, the more likely was the taunting. All these factors—the large size
of the crowd, the distance between that crowd and the victim, the anonymity
lent by darkness—contributed to the deindividuation of the members of the
crowd. And the longer these deindividuated people waited, the more irritable
they became.
Another
study found that when a crowd is bent on violence, the larger the crowd, the
more vicious the behavior (Mullen, 1986). Larger crowds and smaller numbers of
victims can lead to atrocities such as hangings, torture, and rape.
Group
Performance
Individual
Decisions and Group Decisions
First of
all, let’s consider whether group decisions are in fact better than
individual decisions.
Is it better to have a team of medical personnel decide whether our CAT scan
indicates we need surgery, or is that decision better left to a single surgeon?
Did the launch director at NASA benefit from the workings of the group, or
would he have been wiser to think through the situation on his own?
Does a
Group Do Better Than the Average Person?
In
general, research shows that groups do outperform individuals—at least the
average individual—on many jobs and tasks (Stasser, Kerr, & Davis, 1989).
Three reasons have been proposed for the observed superiority of groups over
the average person. First of all, groups do a better job than the average
person because they recognize truth—accept the right answer—more quickly.
Second, groups are better able to reject error—reject incorrect or implausible
answers (Laughlin, 1980; Laughlin, VanderStoep, & Hollingshead, 1991; Lorge
& Solomon, 1955). Third, groups have a better, more efficient memory system
than do individuals. This permits them to process information more effectively.
However,
groups do not appear to live up to their potential. That is, their performance
seems to be less than the sum of their parts (i.e., the individual members
[Kerr & Tindale, 2005]). So let’s keep that in mind as we first see what
advantages groups have over individuals.
Groups may possess what has been called transactive memory systems, a shared
system for placing events into memory (encoding), storing those memories, and
retrieving that information. Wegner (1996) used the example of a
directory-sharing computer network to explain the three legs of a transactive
memory system:
1. Directory updating, in which people find out
what other group members know
2. Information allocation, the place where new
information is given to the person who knows how to store it
3. Retrieval coordination, which refers to how
information is recovered when needed to solve a particular problem
Group
members learn about each other’s expertise and assign memory tasks on
that basis. This not
only leaves others to concentrate on the memory tasks they do best, it also
provides the group with memory aids. Someone in the group may be good in math,
for example, so that person is assigned the task of remembering math-related
information. When the group wants to recall that information, they go to this
expert and use him or her as an external memory aid. Memory thus becomes a
transaction, a social event in the group. For some or all of these reasons,
groups seem to outperform the average person on many decision-related tasks
(Laughlin, Zander, Knievel, & Tan, 2003).
Hollingshead
(1998) showed the effectiveness of transactive memory. She studied intimate
couples as compared to strangers who worked on problems, some face to face and
others via a computer-conferencing network. Intimate couples who were able to
sit face-to-face and process their partner’s verbal and nonverbal cues were able to
solve problems better
than couples comprised of strangers, because the intimate couples were able to
retrieve more information. Intimate couples who worked via a
computerconferencing system did not do as well, again suggesting that the
nonverbal cues were important in pooling information. In fact, recent research
shows that in small groups in which the individual members do not submerge
their personal identities but rather express them, the individuals’identification
with that group is enhanced (Postmes, Spears,
Lee, & Novak, 2005).
Does a Group
Do Better Than Its Best Member?
We noted
that research shows that groups outperform the average person. But does the
group perform better than the best member, the smartest person, the “best and
brightest” member of the group?
To test
the hypothesis that groups can find correct responses better than individuals,
college students were asked to try to discover an arbitrary rule for separating
a deck of cards into those that did and did not fit the rule (Laughlin,
VanderStoep, & Hollingshead, 1991). If the rule was “hearts,” for example,
then all cards of the hearts suit would fit the rule, and all others would not.
Subjects had to guess the rule, and then test it by playing a card. The
feedback from the experimenter gave them information on which to base their
next guess. The researchers also varied the amount of information that subjects
had to process. They presented some subjects with only two arrays of cards,
others with three, and others with four: The more arrays, the more difficult
the task.
The performance
of four-person groups was then compared to the performance of each of the four
group members, who had to do a similar task individually. The best individual
was able to generate more correct guesses than the group or any other
individual member. The group’s performance was equal to its
second-best member. The third-
and fourth-best members were inferior to the group. As the task became more
difficult—the arrays increased to four, which made much more information
available—the performance of both the best individual and the group fell. The
researchers also compared the abilities of groups and their individual members
in rejecting implausible hypotheses. The fewer implausible ideas subjects or
groups raised, the better they did with respect to rejecting false leads.
Groups and the best individual were better at rejecting false leads than were
the second-, third-, and fourth-best individuals.
This
research suggests that groups in general perform as well as their best or
secondbest individual member working independently. You might ask, Why not just
let the best member do the task? But keep in mind that it is often not possible
to identify the group’s best member prior to completing the
task. This finding tells us that groups tend to perform competently, particularly when the
information load is not overwhelming.
In
addition, it may very well be that the kind of problem that the group has to
deal with may influence whether or not a very good individual is or is not
better than the group solution.
The Harder
the Problem, the Better the Group
Recent
work suggests that we may have underrated the ability of groups to reach
solutions, especially more difficult problems. Crott, Giesel, and Hoffman
(1998) argued that their research on group problem solving suggests that
difficult tasks provoke creativity in groups. When faced with a problem that
required the group to come up with a number of hypotheses to discover the
correct answers, groups more than individuals were able to generate a number of
novel explanations. Groups were also shown to be less likely to be prone to the
confirmation bias than were individuals (Crott et al., 1998).
Similarly,
Laughlin, Bonner, and Altermatt (1998) showed that groups were as good as the
best individual in solving difficult inductive (proceeding from specific facts
to general conclusions) problems and better than all the remaining group
members. Groups are especially effective in dealing with information-rich
problems because they have more resources (Tindale, Smith, Thomas, Filkins,
& Sheffey, 1996).
The
finding that the best member of a group may outperform the group is also
modified by the size of that groupand by the type of problem. Laughlin and his
colleagues studied groups that varied in size from two to five people
(Laughlin, Hatch, Silver, & Boh, 2006). The groups had to deal with a
complex intellectual problem that required different strategies. The
researchers first determined the best, second-best, third-best, and fourth- and
fifth-best member of each group. Laughlin et al. then compared the solutions to
these complex problems submitted by individual members and those submitted by
three-, four-, and five-person groups. These researchers found, contrary to
some previous findings, that the groups took significantly less time to solve
problems and the quality of the solutions were better than those of the best
member of the group. That is, each of the three-, four-, and five-person groups
solved the problems more quickly and produced more complex solutions to the
problems than the best individual member. And, there were no significant
differences between three-, four-, and five-person groups. This is interesting
because we might have expected some “motivation loss” due to free riders (see
our earlier discussion) as the group got larger.
What about
the two-person groups? The two-person groups performed less well than the other
groups. Laughlin et al. (2006) concluded that groups of three that are
“necessary and sufficient” perform better than the very best individual on
difficult intellective problems.
We have
seen how well groups perform with respect to the abilities of their members.
Let’s take a closer look at the workings, the dynamics, of how
those decisions are
made. How do groups gather and use the information possessed by individual
members? How do they reach decisions?
The Group’s
Use of Information: Hidden Profiles
One
advantage groups have over individual decision makers is that a variety of
individuals can usually bring to the discussion a great deal more information than
can one person. This is usually seen as the great advantage of groups. But does
the group make adequate use of that information? Research shows that group
members tend to discuss information that they share and avoid discussing
information that only one person has. This research on the insufficient sharing
of information that one member of the group may have is known as the hidden
profileparadigm. The hidden profile paradigm refers to a situation in which the
group’s task is to pick the best alternative, say the best job applicant, but the relevant
information to make this choice is distributed among the group members such
that no one member has enough information to make the right choice alone
(Greitmeyer & Schulz-Hardt, 2003).
In one
experiment, each member of a committee received common information about three
candidates for student government (Stasser & Titus, 1987). Each also
received information about each candidate that none of the others received
(unshared information). The committee members met in four-person groups to rank
the candidates. The sheer number of facts available to the members varied from
one group to the next. When the number of facts was high, the raters ignored
information that was unshared. That is, they rated the candidates based solely
on the information that they held in common. The information they chose to
share tended to support the group decision; they did not share information that
would have conflicted with the decision. Because the results of this study
indicate that group members try to avoid conflict by selectively withholding
information, the researchers concluded that face-to-face, unstructured
discussion is not a good way to inform group members of unshared information
(Stasser, 1991).
There
appear to be at least two reasons for the failure of face-to-face groups to
report and use unshared information. The first has to do with the way people
think. Whatever is most salient (the shared information) tends to overwhelm
that which recedes into the background (the unshared information). In other
words, group members hear the shared information and simply neglect to bring up
or take into account the unshared information. The second reason is that
individuals may be motivated to ignore or forget information (unshared) that they
think may cause conflict. Individuals also avoid discussing or disclosing
information that goes counter to the group’s preferred decision (Greitemeyer & Schulz-Hardt,
2003).
The nature
of a group’s task may also affect how the group searches for information
and uses shared and unshared facts. To investigate this possibility,
experimenters hypothesized
that groups would be more likely to share all information if they knew that the
problem had a definitively correct answer than if the task called only for a
judgment (Stasser & Stewart, 1992). Subjects in this study were given
information about a crime. In some groups, all the information was given to all
the members. In other groups, some information was given only to individual
members. In other words, in the latter groups, some members had unshared
information. In addition, half the groups were told that there was enough
evidence to solve the crime, whereas others were informed that because the
evidence was less than full, the group would have to make a judgment call.
The
results showed that groups given the task with the correct answer were much
more likely to search for the unshared information and get the right answer
than groups given a judgment problem. What differed was the expectation that
there was or was not a correct answer (Stasser & Stewart, 1992). When the
group members think or know that the task has a definite answer, they are more
forthright in bringing up anything (unshared) that could help the group. The
group strategy changes because people want to search for any information that
helps them to be successful. Greitmeyer and SchulzHardt (2003) have shown that
if a hidden profile has incorrect information, you are unlikely to detect that
error. If you do not share your hidden profile with others, then it is
improbable that the error would be rectified.
The
research of James R. Larson, Jr., showed that access to unshared information is
crucial to good group decision making. For example, Larson, Christensen, Franz,
and Abbot (1998) examined the decision making of medical teams. Three-person
physician teams had to diagnose cases and were given shared information (to all
three MDs), whereas the rest of the diagnostic data were divided among the
three. Compared with unshared information, the physicians discussed shared
information earlier in the discussion. However, the unshared information, when
discussed, proved to lead to more accurate (correct diagnosis) outcomes.
In other
research, Larson’s team reached similar conclusions.
Winquist and Larson (1998)
gave three-person groups the task of nominating professors for teaching awards.
Discussion focused more on shared information, but the quality of the decision
was determined by the amount of unshared information that was pooled in the
discussion (Henningsen, Dryden, & Miller, 2003). One way to increase the
likelihood that unshared hidden profiles will be brought to the discussion is
to suggest to the group members that they think in a counterfactual way. That
is, if you have some information that nobody else has, you might say “What if
this is inaccurate, what would it mean?” If that is done, it seems to be the
case that more unshared information sees the light of day (Galinsky & Kray,
2004).
The Effect
of Leadership Style on Group Decision Making
How can we
make sure groups gain access to unshared information? What is the best way of
making sure that group members who have information that others do not are
motivated to pool that information?
We know
that leadership style is important in determining how groups function (Fiedler,
1967). In one study, researchers identified two common styles of leadership.
The first, the participative leader, shares power with the other members of the
group and includes them in the decision making. Another leadership style, the
directive leader, gives less value to participation, emphasizes the need for
agreement, and tends to prefer his or her own solution.
Directive
and Participative Leaders
Research
using these leadership styles indicated that participative leaders provoked
their groups to discuss more information, both shared and unshared, than did
groups with a directive leader (Larson, Foster-Fishman, & Franz, 1998).
However, directive leaders were more likely to repeat information that had been
pooled, especially unshared information. In other words, directive leaders made
unshared information more prominent.
It seems,
then, that participative leaders worked to get the group to bring out more
information but that directive leaders were more active in managing the
information once it was put on the table. What about the quality of the
decisions? Interestingly, groups under participative leadership made many more
incorrect decisions. This was counter to the researchers’expectations
(Larson et al., 1998). If directive leaders have information that favors the best alternative, they
use it and bring the group to a goodquality decision. They do this much better
than participative leaders. The downside to directive leaders is that they may
not be able to get the group members to bring out all the necessary information
for good decision making.
Gender and
Leadership
Eagly and
her colleagues have investigated the possible differences in leadership styles
exhibited by men and women. These differences may be important for effective
group functioning because the behavior of the leader is critical for group
performance (Eagly, Johansen-Schmidt, & van Engen, 2003; Eagly & Karau,
2002). Eagly’s analysis is based on
social roles theory, which suggests that leaders occupy roles determined both
by their position in whatever group they are part of, and by the limits imposed
by genderbased expectations (Eagly & Karau, 2002). For example, if the
leader is a manager of a warehouse, that role is in part determined by the
tasks that must be done to keep that warehouse functioning—scheduling
workloads, monitoring inventory, dealing with unions. But each manager also has
some leeway as to carrying out those functions. Eagly points out that there is
often an incompatibility between leadership roles and the gendered expectations
of women.
Eagly and
her colleagues analyzed almost 50 studies that compared the leadership styles
of males and females (Eagly et al., 2003). They found that as social roles
theory predicted, leadership styles were determined by both gender and demands
placed on the leaders. They found significant gender differences with respect
to the type of leadership styles men and women exhibited. Women leaders were
more transformativethan were male leaders. Transformative leaderstend to focus
on communicating the reasons behind the group’s mission and to
show optimism and excitement about reaching the group’s goals. Transformative leaders also tend
to mentor their group members and to freely
promote new ideas and ways of getting things done.
In
contrast, male leaders are more transactional. That is, they deal in rewarding
positive results but also focus on the mistakes and errors that members have
made. Compared to transformative leaders, who may intervene before serious
problems occur, transactional leadersmay wait until problems become severe
before intervening. In other words, males are more hands-off leaders, more
disengaged, while females seem to be more active.
What do we
make of these differences? Do they matter in the functioning of, say, a
corporation, or a university? Eagly et al. (2003) point out that the difference
between men and women leaders is relatively small. That is, gender accounts for
a relatively small part of the variation of leadership styles. That being said,
however, the qualities that distinguish women leaders from their male
counterparts appear to be directly related to greater group effectiveness. For
example, research has demonstrated the difficulty of motivating workers to
adopt new safety regulations. Research has shown that handson positive
leadership, which defines the transformational leader, can be very effective
(Kelloway, Mullen, & Francis, 2006).
Why Group
Members Obey Leaders: The Psychology of Legitimacy
Tyler
(1997) provided insight into when and why groups voluntarily follow their
leaders. In order for groups to function, the members have to decide that the
leader ought to be obeyed. Although leaders often have access to coercive
methods to get members to follow their orders, voluntary compliance is
necessary oftentimes for a group to successfully achieve its goals.
Tyler was
interested in the judgment by group members that they should voluntarily comply
with the rules laid down by authorities, regardless of the probability of
punishment or reward. Tyler (1997) suggested that the feeling of obligation to
obey the leader is best termed legitimacy. Following earlier work by French and
Raven (1959), a leader has legitimate power to influence, and the member has
the obligation to obey when all have accepted (internalized) the central values
of the group. Tyler’s work suggests that the basis of a leader’s
legitimacy resides in its psychological foundations. That is, it is not enough for the leader to be
successful in getting the group’s work done, although clearly that is quite important.
Among the
factors that are crucial for legitimacy is, first, how people are treated by
authorities, regardless of how the leaders have evaluated them, and second,
whether the members share group membership with the authorities. Finally, Tyler’s
work indicated that
people value the leader’s integrity more than they do the leader’s
competence. This description
of legitimacy is called the relational model.
The
relational model emphasizes that individuals are most likely to internalize
group values when they are treated with procedural fairness (van den Bos,
Wilke, & Lind, 1998). In fact, people make judgments about authorities when
little information is available about them, based on whether the authorities
give them dignified, fair treatment (van den Bos et al., 1998). Neidermeier,
Horowitz, and Kerr (1999) reported that some groups (juries) may deliberately
and willfully disobey the commands of authorities (judges) when they determine
that following the authority’s instructions would result in an unfair and unjust
verdict. People will be more likely to accept a leader when that leader
exhibits interpersonal respect, neutrality in judgment, and trustworthiness
(Tyler, 1997).
Again, we
should not overlook the importance of instrumental factors in leadership.
Getting the group’s work done is crucial. It is likely that
under some circumstances, relational issues may not be important at all (Fiedler, 1967). If someone has
the ability to lead a group out of a burning building, relational issues matter
not. But Tyler’s earlier work indicated that in judging
authorities with whom we have no contact
(the U.S. Congress, the Supreme Court), concerns about fairness come into play
(Tyler, 1994).
Factors
That Affect the Decision-Making Ability
of a Group
What makes
a good decision-making group? Is there a particular size that works best? What
about the abilities of the group members? What other factors have an impact on
the abilities and effectiveness of a group? Consider President Kennedy’s
advisory group that
decided to invade Cuba. It was fairly large, perhaps 12 or more people attended
each session, and group members were similar in temperament, background, and
education. Is that a good recipe for a decision-making group?
Group
Composition
Several
group investigators emphasize the composition of a group as its most
fundamental attribute (Levine & Moreland, 1990). Questions often arise
about how to best constitute groups, especially decision-making groups. For
example, some people have asked whether random selection of citizens is the
best way to put together a jury, especially for a complex trial (Horowitz,
ForsterLee, & Brolly, 1996).
Some
researchers have investigated whether groups with high-ability members perform
better than groups composed of individuals of lesser abilities. In one study,
the composition of three-person battle tank crews was varied (Tziner &
Eden, 1985). Some crews had all high-ability members, some had mixtures of
high- and low-ability members, and others had all low-ability members. Their
results showed that tank groups composed of all high-ability individuals
performed more effectively than expected from the sum of their individual
talents. Groups composed of all low-ability members did worse than expected.
Psychologist
Robert Steinberg believes that every group has its own intelligence level, or
“group IQ” (Williams & Steinberg, 1988). The group’s
IQ is not simply the sum
of each member’s IQ. Rather, it is the blending of their
intellectual abilities with their
personalities and social competence. In one study, Steinberg asked volunteers
who had been tested on their intelligence and social skills to devise a
marketing plan for a new product, an artificial sweetener (Williams &
Steinberg, 1988). Other groups had similar tasks, all of which required
creative solutions. The decision-making groups that produced the most creative
solutions were those that contained at least one person with a high IQ and
others who were socially skillful, practical, or creative. In other words, the
successful groups had a good mix of people with different talents who brought
different points of view to the problem.
This
research highlights the fact that everybody in the group must have the skills
to make a contribution. If one member of the group is extremely persuasive or
extremely good at the task, the other members may not be able to use their
abilities to the best effect. According to one study, successful leaders should
have IQ scores no more than 10 points higher than the average IQ score of the
group (Simonton, 1985). This minimizes the possibility that the most talented
person will dominate the group. If this person is more extraordinary, then the
collective effort will be hurt by his or her presence (Simonton, 1985).
The gender
of group members also influences problem-solving ability (Levine &
Moreland, 1990). Research shows that although groups composed of all males are
generally more effective than all-female groups, the success of the groups
really depends on the kind of problem they have to solve. Male groups do better
when they have to fulfill a specific task, whereas female groups do better at
communal activities that involve friendship and social support (Wood, 1987).
Racial
Effects on Group Decision Making
One might
expect that the racial composition of a group might affect the type and perhaps
the quality of decision making of groups. But how and why? As one example, a
goal of the judicial system is to ensure that juries be formed from fair
cross-sections of the population. This doesn’t mean that each
jury must represent a fair cross-section but
that the group from which the jury is selected is a good representation of the
community. Therefore, from a public policy and a constitutional point of view,
diverse juries are perceived as a societal “good.” But what impact does
diversity have on both the process and outcomes of group decision making?
Sommers
(2006) studied the effects of the racial composition of one unique group, the
jury in criminal trials, on verdicts. Using a “mock jury” paradigm in which
participants are asked to play the role of jurors, Sommers constructed juries
that were either composed of all whites or all blacks, or were racially mixed.
Mock jurors were brought to a county courthouse and essentially went through
the same procedures any prospective juror would. After being formed into
juries, they watched a videotaped trial of a sexual assault case involving an
African American defendant and a white victim. Several questions were asked of
the jurors before seeing the trial that were designed to make them think about
their racial attitudes and to make them salient, uppermost in their minds.
The
results suggested that the differences between racially diverse groups and
racially homogeneous groups were reflected in jury decision making. For
example, whites in diverse groups were more likely to be lenient toward a black
defendant than were whites in all-white groups. Whites in diverse juries
processed more information and brought out more facts that whites in homogeneous
white groups. Diverse juries took more time to deliberate, and diverse groups
discussed more racial issues.
What of
verdicts? Diverse groups showed some tendency to hang, and that goes hand in
hand with the longer deliberation times. However, only 1 of the 30 six-person
juries in the research convicted the defendant. The racial effects in this
research are primarily expressed in the quality of the jury process rather than
in verdicts, generally.
Group Size
Conventional
wisdom tells us that two heads are better than one. If this is so, then why
wouldn’t three be better than two, four better than three, and so on?
Does increasing a group’s
size also increase its ability to arrive at correct answers, make good
decisions, and reach
productivity goals?
Increasing
the number of members of a group does increase the resources available to the
group and therefore the group’s potential productivity. On the other hand,
increasing group size also leads to more process loss (Steiner, 1972). In other
words, the increase in resources due to more group members is counterbalanced
by the increased difficulty in arriving at a decision. Large groups generally
take more time to reach a decision than small groups (Davis, 1969).
Yet,
smaller is not always better. We often misperceive the effect of group size on
performance. Researchers interested in testing the common belief that small
groups are more effective than large groups gave a number of groups the task of
solving social dilemmas, problems that require individuals to sacrifice some of
their own gains so that the entire group benefits, such as conserving water
during a drought (Kerr, 1989).
Those who
participated in the study thought that the size of their group was an important
determinant of their ability to satisfactorily resolve social dilemmas. People
in larger groups felt there was very little they could do to influence the
decisions of the group. They tended to be less active and less aware of what
was going on than comparable members of smaller groups. They believed that
smaller groups would more effectively solve social dilemmas than larger groups,
mainly by cooperating.
In fact,
there was no difference in effectiveness between the small and large groups in
solving social dilemmas. People enjoyed small groups more than large ones, but
the product and the quality of the decisions of both sizes of groups were much
the same. Thus, small groups offer only an illusion of efficacy. That is, they
think they are more effective than larger groups, but the evidence suggests they
may not be, based on their actual productivity (Kerr, 1989).
The Group
Size Effect
Price,
Smith, and Lench (2006) found a group size effect in the area of risk judgment.
When people are asked to make judgments about themselves or another individual,
or groups of individuals, with respect to potential negative life events (heart
attacks, unwanted pregnancies, etc.), they tend to rate themselves, friends,
and family at the lowest risks but rate others at higher risk. So female
college students rate themselves and their friends at lowest risk for unwanted
pregnancies, but rate the “average college woman” at higher risk and the
“average woman” at much higher risk.
There are
a number of possible explanations for the group size effect in the judgment of
risk, but one is that we have favorable opinions of people we know and less
favorable ones of people we don’t know. We are also more optimistic about
ourselves and our
closest friends and family. We tend to believe that our best friend will take
precautions to prevent unwanted pregnancies, but the “average woman” may not be
so careful or so smart. Another application of this group size effectcan be
seen in the research on stereotypes presented in Chapter 4. We have stereotypes
about various social groups, but a friend of ours who is a member of one of
these groups will not be likely to be perceived as having the negative
qualities that the “average” and unknown member of that group is presumed to
possess (Price et al., 2006).
Group
Cohesiveness
Does a
cohesive group outperform a noncohesive group? When we consider decisionmaking
or problem-solving groups, two types of cohesiveness become important:
task-based
cohesiveness and interpersonal cohesiveness (Zachary & Lowe, 1988). Groups
may be cohesive because the members respect one another’s
abilities to help obtain
the group’s goals; this is task-based
cohesiveness. Other groups are cohesive because the members find each other to
be likable; this is interpersonal cohesiveness.
Each type
of cohesiveness influences group performance in a somewhat different way,
depending on the type of task facing the group. When a task does not require
much interaction among members, task-based cohesiveness increases group
productivity, but interpersonal cohesiveness does not (Zaccaro & McCoy,
1988). For example, if a group is working on writing a paper, and each member
is responsible for different parts of that paper, then productivity is
increased to the extent that the members are committed to doing a good job for
the group. The group members do not have to like one another to do the job
well.
Now, it is
true that when members of the group like one another, their cohesiveness
increases the amount of commitment to a task and increases group interaction as
well (Zachary & Lowe, 1988). However, the time they spend interacting may
take away from their individual time on the task, thus offsetting the
productivity that results from task-based cohesiveness.
Some tasks
require interaction, such as the Challenger decision-making group. On these
tasks, groups that have high levels of both task-based and interactive
cohesiveness perform better than groups that are high on one type but low on
the other or that are low on both (Zaccaro & McCoy, 1988).
Cohesiveness
can also detract from the successful completion of a task when group members
become too concerned with protecting one another’s feelings and do
not allot enough
attention to the actual task. Groups that are highly cohesive have members who
are very concerned with one another. This may lead group members to stifle
criticism of group decisions.
Members of
strongly cohesive groups are less likely to disagree with one another than are
members of less cohesive groups, especially if they are under time pressure to
come up with a solution. Ultimately, then, very high cohesiveness may prevent a
group from reaching a high-quality decision. Cohesiveness is a double-edged
sword: It can help or hurt a group, depending on the demands of the task.
The
Dynamics of Group Decision Making: Decision
Rules, Group
Polarization, and Groupthink
Now that
we have considered various aspects of group decision making, let’s
consider how the
decision-making process works. Although we empower groups to make many
important decisions for us, they do not always make good decisions (Janis,
1972). However, the reason we use groups to make important decisions is the
assumption that groups are better at it, more accurate than are individual
decision makers (Hastie & Kameda, 2005).
Group
Decisions: How Groups Blend Individual Choices
A decision
rule is a rule about how many members must agree before the group can reach a
decision. Decision rules set the criteria for how individual choices will be
blended into a group product or decision (Pritchard & Watson, 1992). Two
common decision rules are majority rule (the winning alternative must receive
more than half the votes) and unanimity rule (consensus, all members must
agree).
Groups
will find a decision rule that leads to good decisions and stick with that rule
throughout the life cycle of the group (Miller, 1989). The majority rule is
used in most groups (Davis, 1980). The majority dominates both through
informational social influence—controlling the information the group uses
(Stasser, Kerr, & Davis, 1989)—and through normative social
influence—exerting the group’s will through conformity pressure.
A
unanimity rule, or consensus, forces the group to consider the views of the
minority more carefully than a majority rule. Group members tend to be more
satisfied by a unanimity rule, especially those in the minority, who feel that
the majority paid attention and considered their point of view (Hastie, Penrod,
& Pennington, 1983).
The
decision rule used by a group may depend on what kind of task the group is
working on. When the group deals with intellective tasks—problems for which
there is a definitive correct answer, such as the solution to an equation—the
decision rule is truth wins. In other words, when one member of the group
solves the problem, all members (who have mathematical knowledge) recognize the
truth of the answer. If the problem has a less definitively correct answer,
such as, say, the solution to a word puzzle, then the decision rule is that
truth supported wins. When one member comes up with an answer that the others support,
that answer wins (Kerr, 1991).
When the
group deals with judgmental tasks—tasks that do not have a demonstrably correct
answer, such as a jury decision in a complex case—then the decision rule is
majority wins (Laughlin & Ellis, 1986). That is, whether the formal
decision rule (the one the judge gives to the jury) is unanimity or a 9 out of
12 majority (a rule common in some states), a decision usually is made once the
majority rule has been satisfied. Even if the formal rule is unanimity, all jurors
tend to go along with the majority once 9 or 10 of the 12 jurors agree.
The
Goodness of Decision Rules
Hastie and
Kameda (2005) considered a number of group decision rules to determine which
are best in reaching an accurate decision under conditions in which the correct
answer is uncertain. For example, in the world of political decision making, we
may find decision-making rules involving either democratic or dictatorial
options. Democratic decision rules may involve a pluralityrule, in which the
winner of an election is the one who gets the most votes when no one has more
than 50% of all votes cast, or a majorityrule in which the one with more than
50% wins. This is contrasted with a dictatorial system (one “best” member
decides). In contrast, nondemocratic systems often are, in essence, a “best
member” rule; that is, the leader decides. Hastie and Kameda’s
cogent analysis shows
that most of the time the plurality rules give the most adaptive outcomes—that
is, the outcomes that best favor the members of the group. In fact, both
majority rule and pluralityrule perform quite well most of the time in helping
groups determine the most accurate decision (Hastie & Kameda, 2005).
Group
Polarization
A
commonplace event observed in group decision making is that groups tend to
polarize. Group polarization (Moscovici & Zavalloni, 1969; Myers &
Lamm, 1976) occurs when the initial-decision tendency of the group becomes more
extreme following group discussion. For example, researchers asked French
students about their attitudes toward Americans, which prior to group
discussion had been negative (Moscovici & Zavalloni, 1969). After group
discussion, researchers measured attitudes again and found that group
discussion tended to polarize, or pull the attitude to a more extreme position.
The initial negative attitudes became even more negative after discussion.
In another
study, researchers found that if a jury initially was leaning in the direction
of innocence, group discussion led to a shift to leniency. If, on the other hand,
the jury was initially leaning in the direction of guilt, there was a shift to
severity (Myers & Lamm, 1976). Group polarization can also be recognized in
some of the uglier events in the real world. Groups of terrorists become more
extreme, more violent, over time (McCauley & Segal, 1987). Extremity
shifts, as we have seen, appear to be a normal aspect of group decision making
(Blascovich & Ginsburg, 1974).
Why does
group polarization occur? Researchers have focused on two processes in group
discussion: social comparison and persuasive arguments. Group discussion, as we
have seen, provides opportunities for social comparison. We cannot compare how
we think with how everyone else thinks. We might have thought that our private
decision favored a daring choice, but then we find that other people took even
riskier stands. This causes us to redefine our idea of riskiness and shift our
opinion toward more extreme choices.
The second
cause of group polarization is persuasive arguments (Burnstein, 1982; Burnstein
& Vinokur, 1977). We already have seen that people tend to share
information they hold in common. This means that the arguments put forth and
supported are those the majority of group members support. The majority can
often persuade others to accept those arguments (Myers & Lamm, 1975). For
example, most people in Kennedy’s advisory
group spoke in favor of a military response to Cuba and persuaded doubters of
their wisdom.
Research
supports the idea that discussion polarizes groups. In one early study on the
risky shift, group meetings were set up under several conditions (Wallach &
Kogan, 1965). In some groups, members merely exchanged information about their
views by passing notes; there was no discussion, just information exchange. In
others, individuals discussed their views face-to-face. In some of the
discussion groups, members were required to reach consensus; in others, they
were not. The researchers found that group discussion, with or without reaching
consensus, was the only necessary and sufficient condition required to produce
the risky shift. The mere exchange of information without discussion was not
enough, and forcing consensus was not necessary (Wallach & Kogan, 1965).
Groupthink
The late
Irving Janis (1972, 1982) carried out several post hoc (after-the-fact)
analyses of what he terms historical fiascos. Janis found common threads
running through these decision failures. He called this phenomenon groupthink,
“a mode of thinking that people engage in when they are deeply involved in a
cohesive in-group, when the members’striving for unanimity overrides their
motivation to
realistically appraise alternative courses of actions” (Janis, 1982, p. 9).
Groupthink is a breakdown in the rational decision-making abilities of members
of a cohesive group. As we have seen, members of a highly cohesive group become
motivated to reach unanimity and protect the feelings of other group members
and are less concerned with reaching the best decision.
In
examining poor decisions and fiascos, we have to acknowledge the benefits we
gain from hindsight. From our privileged point of view here in the present, we
can see what we believe to be the fatal flaws of many decisions of the past,
especially those with disastrous outcomes. This is obviously dangerous from a
scientific perspective (a danger that Janis recognized). It can lead us to
overstate the power of groupthink processes. What would have happened, for
example, if the invasion of Cuba had been a rousing success and a democratic
government installed there? How many historical decisions had all the markings
of groupthink but led to good outcomes? It is important to keep a sense of
perspective as we apply concepts such as groupthink to both historical and
contemporary events.
Conditions
That Favor Groupthink
Social psychologist
Clark McCauley (1989) identified three conditions that he believed are always
involved when groupthink occurs:
1. Group insulation.The decision-making group
does not seek analysis and information from sources outside the group.
2. Promotional leadership. The leader presents
his or her preferred solution to the problem before the group can evaluate all
the evidence.
3. Group homogeneity. Groups that are made up of
people of similar background and opinions are prone to have similar views.
These
three antecedents, according to McCauley, lead the group to a premature
consensus.
Symptoms
of Groupthink
Groups
that suffer from groupthink show a fairly predictable set of symptoms. Unlike
the antecedent conditions just discussed, which increase the likelihood of
groupthink, the symptoms protect the group against negative feelings and
anxieties during the decision process. Janis (1972) defined several major
symptoms of groupthink.
1. The illusion of invulnerability. Group
members believe that nothing can hurt them. For example, officials at NASA
suffered from this illusion. In the 25 space flights before Challenger
exploded, not one astronaut was lost in a spacelaunch mission. Even when there
was a near disaster aboard Apollo 13, NASA personnel were able to pull the
flight out of the fire and bring the three astronauts home safely. This track
record of extraordinary success contributed to a belief that NASA could do no
wrong. Another example of this illusion can be seen in the decision on how to
defend Pearl Harbor, in Honolulu, Hawaii. Prior to the Japanese attack on Pearl
Harbor in 1941, advisors to the U.S. commander believed that Pearl Harbor was
invincible. Typically, this illusion leads to excessive optimism: The group
believes that anything it does will turn out for the better.
2. Rationalization. Group members tend not to
realistically evaluate information presented to them. Instead, they engage in
collective efforts to rationalize away damaging information. For example, prior
to the space shuttle Challenger exploding in 1986, officials apparently
rationalized away information about the O-rings, whose failure caused the
explosion. Negative information about the O-rings dating back as far as 1985
was available but ignored. Six months before the disaster, a NASA budget
analyst warned that the O-rings were a serious problem. His warning was labeled
an “overstatement.”
3. Stereotyped views.If group members see the
enemy as too weak, evil, or stupid to do anything about the group’s decision, they are displaying a
stereotyped view of that enemy. An enemy need not be a military or other such
foe. The enemy is any person or group that poses a threat to a group’s
emerging decision. The
enemy in the Challenger decision was the group of Thiokol scientists and engineers
who recommended against the launch. These individuals were characterized as
being too concerned with the scientific end of things. In fact, one engineer
was told to take off his engineer’s hat and put on his management hat. The implication here is that
engineers are too limited in their scope.
4. Conformity pressures.We have seen that
majority influences can operate within a group to change the opinions of
dissenting members. Strong conformity pressures are at work when groupthink
emerges. That is, group members who raise objections are pressured to change
their views. One of the engineers involved in the Challenger launching was
initially opposed to the launch. Under extreme pressure from others, he changed
his vote.
5.
Self-censorship.Once it appears that anyone who disagrees with the group’s
view will be pressured
to conform, members of the group who have dissenting opinions do not speak up
because of the consequences. This leads to selfcensorship. After the initial
opposition to the Challenger launching was rejected rather harshly, for
example, other engineers were less likely to express doubts.
6. The illusion of unanimity.Because of the
strong atmosphere of conformity and the self-censorship of those members who
have doubts about the group decision, the group harbors the illusion that
everyone is in agreement. In the Challenger decision, a poll was taken of
management personnel (only), who generally favored the launch. The engineers
were present but were not allowed to vote. What emerged was a unanimous vote to
launch, even though the engineers strongly disagreed. It looked as if everyone
agreed to the launch.
7. Emergence of self-appointed mindguards.In
much the same way as a person can hire a bodyguard to protect him or her, group
members emerge to protect the group from damaging information. In the
Challenger decision, managers at Morton Thiokol emerged in this role. A
high-ranking Thiokol manager did not tell Arnold Aldrich about the dissension
in the ranks at Thiokol. Thus, Jesse Moore was never made aware of the concerns
of the Thiokol engineers.
The
ChallengerExplosion Revisited
The space
program never had an in-flight disaster. Astronauts had been killed before, but
in training missions, and very early in the program’s
develop ment. Despite the patently dangerous
nature of space travel, the possibility of disaster had been dismissed because
it simply hadn’t happened. In fact, it was deemed so
safe that an untrained civilian, a school
teacher, was chosen to be a crew member on the Challenger.
When the
leaders of groups have a preferred outcome and are under pressure to make
decisions quickly, it becomes highly likely that information that does not
conform to the favored point of view will be ignored by decision-making groups.
Understanding how groups interact and influence their members is crucial to
designing procedures that will provide for rational decision-making processes.
Chapter
Review
1. What is
a group?
A group is an assemblage of two or more
individuals who influence one another through social interaction. Group members
share perceptions of what constitutes appropriate behavior (group norms), and
they have formal and informal roles. Group members are interdependent; that is,
they depend on one another to meet group goals, and they have emotional
(affective) ties with one another. Groups can be either instrumental (existing
to perform a task or reach a goal) or affiliative (existing for more general,
usually social, reasons).
Groups
vary in cohesiveness, the strength of the relationships that link the members
of the group. Groups may be cohesive because the members like one another
(interpersonal cohesiveness), because they are physically close to one another
(propinquity), because they adhere to group norms, or because they help each
other do a good job and, therefore, attain group goals (task-based
cohesiveness).
2. Why do
people join groups?
Groups
help people meet their biological, psychological, and social needs. Groups were
certainly useful in the evolutionary history of humans, aiding the species in
its survival. Among the basic needs groups meet are social support, protection
from loneliness, and social comparison—the process by which we compare our
feelings, opinions, and behaviors with those of others in order to get accurate
information about ourselves. People join groups to fulfill these needs and to
enhance themselves.
3. How do
groups influence their members?
In addition to fulfilling members’needs,
groups also influence members’individual
senses of worth and self-esteem, which, in turn, has an impact on how one group
relates to other groups in a society. Self-identity theory suggests that much
of our self-esteem derives from the status of the groups to which we belong or
with which we identify.
Members
who threaten the success of a group also threaten the positive image of the
group. This leads to the black-sheep effect, the observation that whereas an
attractive in-group member is rated more highly than an attractive member of an
out-group, an unattractive in-group member is perceived more negatively than an
unattractive out-group member. Although groups may serve to increase our
self-esteem by enhancing our social identity, groups also have the power to
exact painful, even dreadful, punishment, including social ostracism, which is
defined by Williams (1997) as the act of excluding or ignoring other
individuals or groups.
4. What
effect does an audience have on performance?
The
presence of other people or audiences may enhance our performance, a process
known as social facilitation. Other times, the presence of a critical audience
or an audience with high expectations decreases performance (“choking”).
Research has shown that the presence of others helps when people perform a
dominant, well-learned response but diminishes performance when they perform a
skill not very well learned or nove (social inhibition). This may be due to
increased effort as a result of increased arousal; or it may be due to anxiety
about being judged (evaluation apprehension), which increases arousal; or,
according to distraction-conflict theory, it may be due to conflicts for
attention.
5. What
motivational decreases affect performance?
Sometimes,
being in a group enhances performance. Other times, individuals performing in
groups display social loafing, a tendency not to perform to capacity. This
seems to occur when the task is not that important or when individual output
cannot be evaluated. When people become free riders, others often work harder
to make up for their lack of effort, a process known as social compensation.
6. What motivational gains occur because of
group interaction? What is the Kohler effect?
Kerr and
his colleagues rediscovered work done by Kohler (1926) in which the researcher
reported that a less-capable member of a two-person group (a dyad) working
together on a task works harder and performs better than expected when the
group product is to be a result of the combined (conjunctive) effort of the two
members. This seems to be the opposite of social loafing. The weaker member of
the group, rather than free riding or loafing, in fact increases his or her
effort. Why does this occur? It seems that motivation gains in groups may occur
due in part to social comparison effects, in which there is some competition
between two group members, as well as the personal motivation of the weakest
member to see how well that member can perform.
7. What
are the potential negative aspects of groups?
When members of a crowd cannot be identified
individually, and therefore feel they have become anonymous, they may
experience deindividuation, a loss of self-identity. Their sense of personal
responsibility diminishes, and they tend to lose their inhibitions. This is
more likely to happen if the crowd is large or is physically distant from a
victim. Deindividuation can be a factor in mob violence. Loss of personal
identity can also be positive, such as when group members act without thinking
to save others’lives.
Although
groups may serve to increase our self-esteem by enhancing our social identity,
they also have the power to exact painful, even dreadful, punishment. Kipling
Williams has studied the effects of being ignored or rejected by the group.
Such behavior is called social ostracism and is defined by Williams as the act
of excluding or ignoring other individuals or groups. This behavior is
widespread and universal. Williams noted that organizations, employers,
coworkers, friends, and family all may ignore or disengage from people (the
silent treatment) to punish, control, and vent anger. The pervasiveness of
ostracism is reflected by a survey conducted by Williams and his coworkers that
showed that 67% of the sample surveyed said they had used the silent treatment
(deliberately not speaking to a person in their presence) on a loved one, and 75%
indicated that they had been a target of the silent treatment by a loved one.
From the point of view of the victim of this silent treatment, social ostracism
is the perception of being ignored by others in the victim’s
presence.
8. With
regard to solving problems: Are groups better than individuals, or are
individuals better than groups?
Groups are
more effective in processing information than are the individual members of the
group, perhaps because they use transactive memory systems, by which each
member may recall different things so that the group can produce a more
complete memory then any one member can. Groups do not usually perform better
than their very best individual member, but recent work has shown that groups
may be superior when dealing with complex problems, because they have more
resources and can be more creative than can individuals. In one study, three-,
four-, and five-person groups solved the problems more quickly and produced
more complex solutions to the problems than the best individual member. So,
when problems are really intellectually challenging, groups do better than the
best member working alone.
9. What
are hidden profiles, and what effects do they have on group decision making?
“Hidden
profiles” refers to a situation in which the group’s
task is to pick the best
alternative—say, the best job applicant—but the relevant information to make
this choice is distributed among the group members such that no one member has
enough information to make the right choice alone. It appears that group
members try to avoid conflict by selectively withholding information; the
researchers concluded that face-to-face, unstructured discussion is not a good
way to inform group members of unshared information.
10. What
is the effect of different leadership styles on group decision making?
Leadership is also a factor in group
effectiveness. Research has identified two common styles of leadership. The
first, the participative leader, is someone who shares power with the other
members of the group and includes them in the decision making. Another
leadership style, the directive leader, gives less value to participation,
emphasizes the need for agreement, and prefers his or her solution. Groups
under participative leadership made many more incorrect decisions.
Participative leaders can get members to bring out more unshared information,
and that is important because it is usually unshared information that leads to
the most accurate decisions. However, a directive leader makes the group focus
more on unshared information and therefore tends to produce fewer mistakes than
do participative leaders.
Gender
accounts for a relatively small part of the variation among leadership styles.
However, some research indicates that the qualities that distinguish women leaders
from their male counterparts appear to be directly related to greater group
effectiveness. Research has shown that hands-on positive leadership, which
defines the transformational leader (the preferred style of women), can be
effective.
11. How do
groups reach decisions?
Decision-making
groups need to develop decision rules—rules about how many people must agree—in
order to blend individual choices into a group outcome. Two common decision
rules are majority and unanimity (consensus). Generally, majority wins is the
dominant decision rule, but the selection of a decision rule often depends on
the group task.
12. What
makes a leader legitimate in the eyes of the group members?
Two
factors that are crucial for legitimacy are, first, how people are treated by
authorities, regardless of how the leaders have evaluated them, and second,
whether the members share group membership with the authorities. Finally,
research shows that people value the leader’s integrity more
than they do the leader’s
competence.
13. What
factors affect the decision-making ability and effectiveness of a group?
Group composition is important to the
decision-making ability of a group. Groups of high-ability individuals seem to
perform better than groups of lowability individuals, but members’abilities
blend and mix in unexpected ways to
produce a group IQ. Groups seem to perform better when members have
complementary skills but when no single member is much more talented than the
others.
Group size
also affects group productivity. Although increasing group size increases the
resources available to the group, there is also more process loss; that is, it
becomes harder to reach a decision. As more people are added to the group, the
number of people who actually make a contribution—the group’s
functional size—does not increase.
Research
has shown differences between racially diverse groups and racially homogeneous
groups in jury decision making. For example, whites in diverse groups were more
likely to be lenient toward a black defendant than were whites in all-white
groups. Whites in diverse juries processed more information and brought out
more facts that whites in homogeneous white groups. Diverse juries took more
time to deliberate and diverse groups discussed more racial issues. However,
racial composition did not affect verdicts.
Some
groups and group processes offer an illusion of efficacy; people think they are
more effective than they are. This is true of small groups, which many people
erroneously think are better at solving social dilemmas than are larger groups.
Another
factor in group effectiveness is group cohesiveness. When a task does not
require much interaction among members, task-based cohesiveness—cohesiveness
based on respect for each other’s abilities—increases group productivity, but
interpersonal cohesiveness—cohesiveness based on liking for each other—does
not. Sometimes, interpersonal cohesiveness can impede the decision-making
abilities of the group, because people are afraid of hurting each other’s
feelings.
14. What
is group polarization?
Group
decision making often results in group polarization—that is, the initial
decision tendency of the group becomes more extreme following group discussion.
It seems that the group discussion pulls the members’attitudes
toward more extreme
positions as a result of both social comparison and persuasive arguments.
15. What
is groupthink?
Groups often make bad decisions when they
become more concerned with keeping up their members’morale
than with reaching a realistic decision. This
lack of critical thinking can lead to groupthink, a breakdown in the rational
decision-making abilities of members of a cohesive group. The group becomes
driven by consensus seeking; members do not want to rock the boat.
Groupthink
is favored by group cohesiveness, stress, and the persuasive strength of the
leader. It is also more likely to occur when a group is insulated and
homogeneous and has a leader who promotes a particular point of view. Several
measures can be taken to prevent groupthink, including encouraging a critical
attitude among members, discussing group solutions with people outside the
group, and bringing in outside experts who don’t agree with the group’s solution.
Another
approach suggests that group polarization, risk taking, and the possibility of
a disastrous decision being reached all increase when a decision is framed in
terms of potential failure. If all outcomes are seen as potentially negative,
according to this view, group members will tend to favor the riskier ones over
the more cautious ones. Finally, groupthink has been found to occur more often
when the group process doesn’t allow everyone to speak freely and fully and when group leaders
become obsessed with maintaining morale.
*********************************************
Social Psychology
Third Edition
Kenneth S. Bordens Indiana University—Purdue University Fort Wayne
Irwin A. Horowitz - Oregon State University
Social Psychology, 3rd Edition
Copyright ©2008 by Freeload Press
Illustration used on cover © 2008 JupiterImages Corporation
ISBN 1-930789-04-1
No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or
by any means, electronic, mechanical, recording, photocopying, or otherwise, without the prior written
permission of the publisher.
Printed in the United States of America by Freeload Press.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Komentar
Posting Komentar