Attitudes
The
ultimate determinant in the struggle now going on for the world will not be
bombs and rockets but a test of wills and ideas—a trial of spiritual resolve:
the values we hold, the beliefs we cherish and the ideals to which we are
dedicated.
—Ronald
Reagan
Ida
Tarbell is not a name most of us recognize. A history of American women doesn’t
give her even a single line (Hymowitz & Weissman, 1984). Yet, she was at
the center of American life for the fi rst three decades of the 20th century.
Teddy Roosevelt hurled the mocking epithet “muckraker” at her. It was a label
she eventually wore proudly, for she, perhaps more than anyone else, told the
American people about the corruption, conspiracies, strong-arm tactics, and
enormous greed that went into “business as usual” at the turn of the century
(Fleming, 1986).
Tarbell
grew up in Titusville, Pennsylvania. In the last decades of the 19th century,
it was the center of the booming oil industry. It was also the town that would
make Standard Oil Company and its founder, John D. Rockefeller, richer than
anyone could imagine.
Tarbell
grew up among derricks and oil drums, in oil-cloaked fields, under oil-flecked
skies. In 1872 her father’s business was threatened by a scheme devised by
Rockefeller and his partners that would allow them to ship their oil via the
railroads at a much cheaper fare than any other producer, thus driving their
competition out of the business. Frank Tarbell and the others fought this
scheme and forced the railroads to treat everyone fairly, at least temporarily.
Ida was well informed about the conspiracy and, possessing her father’s strong
sense of justice, was outraged. She vowed that if she were given the chance,
she would make people aware of the greed and dishonesty she had witnessed. At
this time she was 15 years old (Weinberg & Weinberg, 1961).
In
college, Tarbell was a free spirit. She became friends with whomever she
wanted, ignored all the unwritten social rules, learned to be critical and
disciplined in her work, and graduated with a degree in natural science. After
working as a schoolteacher, she went off to Paris to become a writer.
For
years, she wrote articles and biographies, but in 1900, she started to write
about oil. She began to form an idea about a series of articles on the Standard
Oil Company, which supplied almost all the oil that was used to light American
homes in the days before electricity. Although Standard Oil had been
investigated on charges of bribery and other illegal tactics by authorities for
almost the entire 30 years of its existence, very little evidence existed in
the public domain. Tarbell got around that by getting to know one of the
company’s vice presidents, Henry Rogers, who let her have access to private
records. Rogers was unapologetic about his role. He cheerfully admitted that
Rockefeller lied, cheated, double-dealt, and used violence or the threat of it
to build an enormously successful, powerful, and effi cient company (Fleming,
1986).
Tarbell’s
book, The History of the Standard Oil Company, published in 1904, appeared in
monthly installments in McClure’smagazine. It was a sensation. It read like a
suspense story, and readers couldn’t wait until the next month’s issue. The
book had a ready-made villain: John D. Rockefeller. He was portrayed as a
money-hungry rogue without a shred of humanity, and that is the image of him
that has come down to us 100 years later. After the book came out, he tried to
restore his image by giving some $35 million to charity. At the time, he was
estimated to be worth over $900 million, a sum equivalent to many billions in
today’s currency.
Tarbell’s
work had a tremendous impact on the nation. It led not only to a number of
lawsuits against the oil industry for its monopolistic practices, but also to
federal antitrust laws that dismantled the original Standard Oil Company.
Today, we have a number of independent Standard Oil companies (Ohio, New Jersey,
etc.) as a result of Tarbell’s work.
Even
more remarkable than what Tarbell did was the way she did it. She was entirely
skeptical of all the common beliefs of her time. She did not believe in the
theory of the inferiority of women, prevalent in the early years of her life,
nor did she believe in the turn-of-the-century theory that women were morally
superior and evolutionarily more advanced. She joined no organizations or
social reform movements. Yet she took on the most powerful men in the country
and became a formidable adversary (Fleming, 1986).
Tarbell
was determined, controlled, and unafraid, but her attitudes and behavior were
also shaped and informed by her experience. She grew up in a family that
supported her in her independent ways and encouraged her to do what she thought
was right. She was powerfully infl uenced by her father, within whom she saw a
strong sense of justice. Events that occurred during her formative years
motivated and inspired her and forever altered the way she viewed the world.
The
attitudes that Tarbell held played a fundamental role in the way she perceived
the world around her. Like other mechanisms of social cognition, they organized
her experiences, directed her behavior, and helped defi ne who she was. We
begin by exploring what attitudes are and what role they play in our lives.
What are the elements that go into attitudes? How do they fl ow from and
express our deepest values? What are the processes by which we acquire or
develop attitudes? And what is the relationship between attitudes and behavior
in our day-to-day lives? How do attitudes express the relationships among what
we think, what we feel, what we intend to do, and what we actually do? These
are some of the questions addressed in this chapter.
The
study of attitudes has been of fundamental concern to social psychologists
throughout the history of the field. Other issues may come and go, dictated by
fashion in theory and research and influenced by current events, but interest
in attitudes remains. This preoccupation with attitudes is easy to understand.
The concept of attitudes is central to explaining our thoughts, feelings, and
actions with regard to other people, situations, and ideas.
In
this section, we explore the basic concept of attitudes. First we look at and
elaborate on a classic definition of the term. Then we consider how attitudes
relate to values, what functions attitudes serve, and how attitudes can be
measured.
Allport’s
Definition of Attitudes
The
word attitudecrops up often in our everyday conversation. We speak of having an
attitude about someone or something. In this usage, attitude usually implies
feelings that are either positive or negative. We also speak of someone who has
a “bad attitude.” You may, for example, think that a coworker has an “attitude
problem.” In this usage, attitude implies some personality characteristic or
behavior pattern that offends us.
Social
psychologists use the term attitudedifferently than this. In order to study and
measure attitudes, they need a clear and careful definition of the term. Gordon
Allport, an early attitude theorist, formulated the following definition: “An
attitudeis a mental and neural state of readiness, organized through
experience, exerting a directive or dynamic influence upon the individual’s response to all objects and situations with which it is
related” (1935). This is a rich and comprehensive definition, and although
there have been many redefinitions over the years, Allport’s definition still captures much that is essential about
attitudes (see Figure 5.1). Consequently, we adopt it here as our central
definition. The definition can be broken into three parts, each with some
important implications (Rajecki, 1990).
First,
because attitudes are mental or neural states of readiness, they are necessarily
private. Scientists who study attitudes cannot measure them directly in the
way, for example, that medical doctors can measure blood pressure. Only the
person who holds an attitude is capable of having direct access to it. The
social psychological measures of an attitude must be indirect.
Figure
5.1
Second,
if attitudes are organized through experience, they are presumably formed
through learning from a variety of experiences and influences. Our attitudes
about, say, appropriate roles for men and women are shaped by the attitudes
passed on by our culture, especially by parents, friends, and other agents of
socialization, such as schools and television. Recall that even though the
wider society was not supportive of women in nontraditional roles in Ida
Tarbell’s time, her parents were very supportive.
The notion that our attitudes arise only from experience is too limiting,
however. There
is also increasing evidence that some attitudes also have a genetic element
(Tesser, 1993). Finally, because attitudes exert a directive or dynamic
influence on a person’s response to objects, people, and
situations, attitudes are directly related to our actions or
behavior.
Attitude
Structures
An
attitude is made up of four interconnected components: cognitions, affective
responses, behavioral intentions, and behaviors. To understand this
interconnectedness, let’s consider the attitude of someone
opposed to gun-control legislation. Her attitude can be stated as, “I am
opposed to laws in any way controlling the ownership of guns.”
This
attitude would be supported by cognitions, or thoughts, about laws and gun
ownership. For example, she might think that unrestricted gun ownership is a
basic right guaranteed by the Second Amendment of the Constitution. The
attitude would also be supported by affective responses, or feelings. She might
feel strongly about her right to do what she wants to do without government
interference, or she might feel strongly about protecting her family from
intruders.
The
attitude, and the cognitions and feelings that support it, can result in
behavioral intentions and behaviors. Our hypothetical person might intend to
send money to the National Rifle Association or to call her representative to
argue against a gun-control bill. Finally, she might turn that intention into
some real action and send the money or call her legislator.
An
attitude is really a summary of an attitude structure, which consists of these
interconnected components (Zimbardo & Leippe, 1992). Thus, the attitude “I
oppose laws that restrict handgun ownership” comprises a series of interrelated
thoughts, feelings, and intentions.
A
change in one component of an attitude structure might very well lead to
changes in the others (Zimbardo & Leippe, 1992), because an attitude structure
is dynamic, with each component influencing the others. For example, if a close
relative of yours lost his job because of a new gun-control law, a person who
favors strong gun-control laws may change her mind. The attitude structure
would now be in turmoil. New feelings about guns might lead to new thoughts;
intentions might change and, with them, behaviors.
Generally,
the affective component dominates the attitude (Breckler & Wiggins, 1989).
When we think of a particular object or person, our initial response is usually
some expression of affect, as in, “I feel women will make good political
candidates.” We do not simply have attitudes about war, or the president, or
baseball: We like these things, or we do not. When an attitude is evoked, it is
always with positive or negative feeling, although, to be sure, the feeling
varies in intensity. It is likely that our most intensely held attitudes in
particular are primarily affective in nature (Ajzen, 1989). Thus, you might
think of an attitude as primarily a response emphasizing how you feel about
someone or something, as primarily an evaluation of the person or object. But
keep in mind also that this evaluation is based on all the thoughts,
intentions, and behaviors that go into the structure of the attitude (Zanna
& Rempel, 1988).
Attitudes
as an Expression of Values
Our
attitudes flow from and express our values (Ball-Rokeach, Rokeach, & Grube,
1984). A valueis a conception of what is desirable; it is a guideline for a
person’s actions, a standard for behavior.
Thus, for example, the attitude that more women and members of different ethnic
groups should be elected to office might flow from the value of equality. The
attitude that public officials who lie or cheat should be punished severely
might flow from the value of honesty. Ida Tarbell placed a high value on
fairness and justice and was outraged by the actions of Standard Oil Company.
Notice
that attitudes are directed toward objects, people, and situations; values are
broad, abstract notions. Because values are more general than attitudes, there
are few values but many attitudes. Just as an attitude can be seen as a system
of cognitive, affective, and behavioral components, so a value can be seen as
containing many interrelated attitudes. The value of equality could give rise
not only to the attitude, say, that more women and members of different ethnic
groups should hold office but also to countless other attitudes relating to the
innumerable people, objects, issues, and ideas toward which one might direct
thoughts, feelings, and behaviors.
Milton
Rokeach, a social psychologist who spent most of his professional life studying
how people organize their value systems, argued that there are two distinct
categories of values (1973, 1979). He called one category terminal values.
Terminal values according to Rokeach (1973) refer to desired “end states.” For
example, equality, freedom, a comfortable life, and salvation would all be end
states. The other category he called instrumental values. Instrumental values,
which flow from our preferred end states, could be values such as being
forgiving, broadminded, and responsible.
According
to Rokeach, two fundamental terminal values, equality and freedom, are
especially predictive of a whole range of attitudes. Attitudes about the role
of government, for example, often can be predicted by knowing how someone ranks
these two values. A person who values equality more highly probably would want
the government to take an active role in education, health, and other social welfare
issues. A person who values freedom more highly probably would prefer that
government stay out of the way and let everyone fend for themselves. Consider a
person who rates equality higher than freedom. How might this affect her
attitudes on specific issues? A high value placed on equality implies that the
individual is more concerned with the common good than with individual freedoms
(although freedom might still be ranked relatively highly by that person). This
individual might be in favor of “sin taxes” (such as high tobacco and alcohol
taxes) to raise money for national health care and also might be in favor of
stronger gun-control laws. A person who considers freedom to be more desirable
than equality probably would be against sin taxes (“It’s none of the government’s
business if people want to kill themselves”) and also against government regulation of
gun ownership.
When
asked, do people account for their attitudes by referring to specific values?
And do people on opposing sides of an issue hold opposing values? In one study,
researchers measured participants’attitudes
toward two issues,
abortion and nuclear weapons (Kristiansen & Zanna, 1988). Next,
participants were asked to rank the (personal) importance of 18 values, such as
freedom, equality, an exciting life, family security, and so on, and then
relate each value to their attitudes on these two issues.
People
with different attitudes consider different values important. People who oppose
the right to abortion, for example, give a higher ranking to certain values
(e.g., mature love, wisdom, true friendship, salvation, and a world of beauty)
than do people who support the right to abortion. Those who support the right
to abortion give a higher ranking to other values (e.g., happiness, family security,
a comfortable life, pleasure, an exciting life, and a sense of accomplishment)
than do those who oppose the right to abortion.
At
the same time, both groups shared many values. Both ranked freedom, inner
harmony, and equality as the values most important to their attitude.
Differences in the rankings of other values were slight. The results also
suggest that people on either side of volatile issues might be much closer in
their values than they realize.
Explicit
and Implicit Attitudes
In
many cases we freely express and are aware of our attitudes and how they
influence our behavior. An attitude falling into this category is known as an
explicit attitude. Explicit attitudes operate on a conscious level, so we are
aware of them—aware of the cognitive underpinnings of them—and are conscious of
how they relate to behavior. They operate via controlled processing and take
some cognitive effort to activate. For example, you may know how you feel
toward a given political candidate and match your behavior (e.g., voting for
him or her) to that attitude. It is these explicit attitudes that we often find
having a directive effect on behavior.
Although
many of our attitudes operate on this conscious level, there are others that
operate unconsciously. This form of an attitude is known as an implicit
attitude. Specifically, an implicit attitude is defined as “actions or
judgments that are under control of automatically activated evaluation without
the performer’s awareness of that causation”
(Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998, p. 1464). In other words, implicit
attitudes affect behaviors automatically, without conscious thought, and below
the level of awareness. For example, an individual may have a quick negative
reaction toward a member of a minority group, even though the individual
professes positive and tolerant attitudes toward that group. The “gut-level”
reaction occurs without thought and is often distasteful to the individual
(Wilson, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000).
Wilson,
Lindsey, and Schooler (2000) proposed a model of dual attitudes to explain the
relationship between explicit and implicit attitudes. They suggested that when
one develops a new attitude, the new attitude does not erase the old attitude.
Instead, the two attitudes coexist. The new attitude serves as the explicit
attitude; the old attitude remains in memory and takes on the role of the
implicit attitude. This implicit attitude can override the explicit attitude
when the situation is right. For example, a person who has changed from a
racially prejudiced attitude to a nonprejudiced attitude may still have an
automatic negative reaction to a member of a minority group, despite the newly
formed positive attitude. In this case, the underlying unconscious implicit
attitude has overridden the explicit attitude.
Researchers
have usually assumed that when people develop new attitudes, they tend to
override or obliterate the old attitudes. However, Petty, Tormala, Brinol, and
Jarvis (2006) have found that when attitudes change, the old attitude may not
only remain in memory but in fact can affect behavior. Petty and his colleagues
did several experiments in which they created new attitudes in people and then
changed those attitudes for some of the experimental participants and did not
change them for others. The researchers found that when participants were given
new attitudes via a “priming” procedure in which the people were not aware of
the influence attempt, their response to the person or object was ambivalent.
In other words, if you were conditioned to like Phil but then were primed with
negative words about Phil (presented very quickly, just below the level of
conscious awareness), your attitude should have changed from positive to
negative. We might expect that the new attitude would override the old, as
Wilson et al. (2000) originally suggested. However, that was not quite what
happened. The new attitude toward Phil was ambivalent; you liked him and you
didn’t like him. You weren’t quite sure how you felt
about Phil. This suggests that the old attitude hasn’t disappeared and is still affecting your judgments about Phil.
This also suggests that when you take a test of implicit attitudes, which are
discussed later in this chapter, an older prejudicial attitude may leak and
merge with a newer, nonprejudiced one. This may be why lots of people who take
implicit attitude tests are surprised, even astounded, that they are as
prejudiced as the test seems to say they are.
How
Are Attitudes Measured?
What
happens when investigators want to learn about people’s attitudes on a particular issue, such as affirmative action,
illegal aliens, or capital punishment? As pointed out earlier in this chapter,
attitudes are private; we can’t know what a
person’s attitudes are
just by looking at her or him. For this reason, social psychologists use a
variety of techniques to discover and measure people’s attitudes. Some of these techniques rely on direct
responses, whereas others are more indirect.
The
Attitude Survey
The
most commonly used techniques for measuring attitudes are attitude surveys. In
an attitude survey, the researcher mails or emails a questionnaire to a
potential respondent, conducts a face-to-face interview, or asks a series of
questions on the telephone. Because respondents report on their own attitudes,
an attitude survey is a self-report measure. A respondent indicates his or her
attitude by answering a series of questions.
There
may be several types of questions on an attitude survey. Open-ended questions
allow respondents to provide an answer in their own words (Oskamp, 1991). For
example, respondents might be asked, What qualifications do you think are
necessary in a president of the United States? Although this type of question
yields rich, in-depth information, the answers can be difficult to analyze. Consequently,
most of the questions on an attitude survey are close-ended, or restricted,
questions such as, Are women qualified to be president of the United States?
Respondents would check a box indicating a response, e.g., yes, no, or don’t know. Notice that this type of question forces
respondents into making one of a limited number of choices.
Another
kind of survey item is the rating scale, in which respondents indicate the
extent to which they agree or disagree with a statement by circling a number on
a scale. One of the most popular of these methods is the Likert scale. Likert
items ask the person to agree or disagree with such attitude statements as the
following on a 5-point scale: “I believe women are qualified to serve in
national office.” Likert’s technique is a summated rating scale,
so called because individuals are given an attitude score based on the sum of their
responses.
In
evaluating election preferences or other attitudes, social psychologists
usually are interested in the attitudes of a large group. Because it is not
possible to survey every member of the group, researchers conducting an
attitude survey select a sample or small subgroup of individuals from the
larger group, or population. Don’t
think that you need a
huge sample to have a valid survey. In fact, most nationwide surveys use a
sample of only about 1,500 individuals.
Although
a sample need not be large, it must be representative. As you recall from
Chapter 1, a representative sample is one that resembles the population in all
important respects. Thus, for any category that is relevant to the attitude
being measured (e.g., race and ethnicity, socioeconomic class, gender, age),
the sample would contain the same proportion of people from each group within
the category (e.g., from each race and ethnic group) as does the population
whose attitudes are being measured. A representative sample contrasts with a
biased sample, which is skewed toward one or more characteristics and does not
adequately represent the larger population.
Potential
Biases in Attitude Surveys
Although
attitude surveys, containing various types of questions, are very popular, they
do have several problems that may make any responses made by research
participants invalid. Schwarz (1999) suggested that the way a person responds
to a survey question depends on a variety of factors, including question
wording, the format of the question, and the context within which the question
is placed.
For
example, presidential candidate Ross Perot commissioned a survey in March 1993 that
included the following question: Should laws be passed to eliminate all
possibilities of special interests giving huge sums of money to candidates?
Ninety-nine percent of the people who responded to the survey said yes. A
second survey done by an independent polling firm asked the same question in a
different way: Do groups have the right to contribute to the candidate they
support? In response to this question, only 40% favored limits on spending.
This is a textbook example of how the wording of the question can influence
polling data (Goleman, 1993).
Phrasing
is important, but so are the specific words used in a question. For example, in
one survey commissioned some years ago by the American Stock Exchange,
respondents were asked how much stock they owned. Much to everyone’s surprise, the highest stock ownership was found in the Southwest.
It seems that the respondents were thinking of stock of the four-legged kind,
not the Wall Street type. The moral is that you must consider the meaning of
the words from the point of view of the people answering the questions.
Finally,
respondents may lie, or to put it somewhat differently, they may not remember
what they actually did or thought. Williams (1994) and his students asked
voters whether they had voted in a very recent election; almost all said they
had. Williams was able to check the actual rolls of those who had voted (not
how they voted) and found that only about 65% of his respondents had voted.
Now, some may have forgotten, but many simply did not want to admit they had
failed to do a socially desirable thing—to vote in an election (Paulhus &
Reid, 1991).
Behavioral
Measures
Because
of the problems associated with self-report techniques, social psychologists
have developed behavioral techniques of measuring attitudes. These techniques,
in one way or another, avoid relying on responses to questions.
Unobtrusive
measuresassess attitudes by indirect means; the individual whose attitudes are
being measured simply is never aware of it. For example, in one early study,
investigators measured voting preferences by tallying the number of bumper
stickers for a particular candidate on cars in a parking lot (Wrightsman,
1969). Other researchers measured attitudes toward competing brands of cola by
searching through garbage cans. Still others attempted to determine the most
popular exhibit at a museum by measuring the amount of wear and tear on various
parts of the carpet (Webb, Campbell, Schwartz, Sechrist, & Grove, 1981).
Another
example of unobtrusive measurement of attitudes is the lost-letter technique
(Milgram, Mann, & Hartner, 1965). If a researcher wants to measure a
community’s attitudes toward, say, its foreign
residents, she might not get honest answers on a Likerttype questionnaire. But,
if she has some stamps and envelopes, she can try the lost-letter technique.
This is what the researcher does: She addresses an envelope to someone with a
foreign-sounding name at a local address. She puts a stamp on the envelope and
then drops it on a crowded street near the post office so that it can easily be
found and mailed. As her baseline control, she drops a stamped envelope
addressed to someone whose name doesn’t
sound foreign. She repeats the procedure as many times as necessary to get a
large enough
sample. Then all she has to do is count the envelopes that turn up in the mail
and compare the number with the names that sound foreign to the number with
names that doesn’t. This is her measure of that community’s
attitude toward foreigners.
Cognitive
Measures: The Implicit Association Test (IAT)
In
recent years a new test has been developed to tap our implicit attitudes,
self-concepts, and other important aspects of our cognitive system. The term
implicitin this context refers to relatively automatic mental associations
(Hofman, Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le, & Schmitt, 2005). The most well-known
implicit measures test is the Implicit Association Test (IAT)
(https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/) developed by Greenwald, McGhee, and
Schwartz (1998). Implicit attitudes, as we suggested earlier, are attitudes
that we hold but are not aware of, so that you are not able to directly report
that attitude. These implicit attitudes can only be measured by indirect means.
The IAT aims at determining the strength of connection between two concepts.
For example, the IAT asked test-takers to assign a stimulus, which can be word
or pictures, as quickly as they possibly can, to a pair of targets. Consider
the following example.
Barry
Bonds vs. Babe Ruth
As
I write this chapter, the controversial San Francisco Giant left fielder, Barry
Bonds, has passed Babe Ruth for second place on the all-time homerun list.
Bonds is an African American and Ruth was white, playing in an era when African
Americans were barred from playing in the major leagues. On the IAT Web site,
you are asked to respond as quickly as you can to different photos of Barry or
the Babe. In addition, you are asked to respond to the pairing of the words
good or bad when used with photos of the two stars. The strength of connection
(associative strength) between two concepts is therefore assessed by combining
a pair of categories—in this case, race (African American vs. Caucasian) and a
pair of attributes (good-bad). These are combined in both association
compatible (Babe—good [presumably]) and incompatible (Babe—bad). The scoring of
these associations may take a number of different forms, but basically, the
differences in the time it takes to respond to these pairings (mean response
latencies) is the measure of the relative strength between the two pairs of
concepts (Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003). The fundamental assumption
behind the IAT is that we “don’t always ʻspeak our minds,’” and as is noted on the IAT Web site, we may not even know
our own
minds. The IAT is an attempt to tap into our unconscious associations. It has
been used to explore the unconscious bases of prejudicial attitudes of all
kinds.
What
Has the IAT Taught Us about Our Racial and Ethnic Attitudes?
The
results of the millions of tests on IAT Web sites showed that 88% of white
people had a pro-white or antiblack implicit bias; nearly 83% of heterosexuals
showed implicit biases for straight people over gays and lesbians; and more
than two-thirds of non-Arab, non-Muslim volunteers displayed implicit biases
against Arab Muslims. In addition, similar results were obtained for religious,
gender, and socioeconomic attitudes. The most interesting finding is that these
results contrast not only with what people say about their own attitudes but
also with what they actually believe about their true attitudes. Marajin
Banaji, who helped develop the IAT, has said that “The Implicit Association Test
measures the thumbprint of the culture on our minds. If Europeans had been
carted to Africa as slaves, blacks would have the same beliefs about whites
that whites now have about blacks” (Vedantam, 2005). How Are Attitudes Formed?
We
can see now that attitudes affect how we think, feel, and behave toward a wide
range of people, objects, and ideas that we encounter. Where do our attitudes
come from? Are they developed, as Allport suggested, through experience? If so,
just how do our attitudes develop through experience? And are there other ways
in which we acquire our attitudes?
The
term attitude formation refers to the movement we make from having no attitude
toward an object to having some positive or negative attitude toward that
object (Oskamp, 1991). How you acquire an attitude plays a very important role
in how you use it. In this section, we explore a range of mechanisms for
attitude formation. Most of these mechanisms—mere exposure, direct personal
experience, operant and classical conditioning, and observational learning—are
based on experience and learning. However, the last mechanism we will look at
is based on genetics.
Mere
Exposure
Some
attitudes may be formed and shaped by what Zajonc (1968) called mere exposure,
which means that simply being exposed to an object increases our feelings,
usually positive, toward that object. The mere-exposure effect has been
demonstrated with a wide range of stimuli, including foods, photographs, words,
and advertising slogans (Bronstein, 1989).
In
one early study, researchers placed ads containing nonsense words such as
NANSOMA in college newspapers (Zajonc & Rajecki, 1969). Later, they gave
students lists of words that included NANSOMA to rate. Mere exposure to a
nonsense word, such as NONSOMA, was enough to give it a positive rating. In
another study, participants were exposed to nonsense syllables and to Chinese
characters (Zajonc, 1968). Repeated exposure increased the positive evaluations
of both the nonsense syllables and the Chinese characters.
Generally,
this means that familiarity, in fact, may not breed contempt. Familiar faces,
ideas, and slogans become comfortable old friends. Think of the silly
commercial jingle you sometimes find yourself humming almost against your will.
In
fact, repeated exposures often work very well in advertising. The Marlboro man,
invented to convince male smokers that taking a drag on a filtered cigarette
would enhance their manhood, lasted through a generation of smokers. (The ad
lasted, the original model didn’t—he died of lung cancer.)
When we walk down the aisle to buy a product, be it cigarettes or soap suds, the
familiar name brand stands out and says, “Buy me.” And we do.
Now,
there are limits to the effect, at least in the experimental studies. A review
of the mere-exposure research concluded that the effect is most powerful when
it occurs randomly over time, and that too many exposures actually will
decrease the effect (Bornstein, 1989). A constant bombardment does not work
very well.
Repeated
exposures increase liking when the stimuli are neutral or positive to begin
with. What happens when the stimuli are negative? It seems that continual
exposure to some object that was disliked initially increases that negative
emotion (Bornstein, 1989; Perlman & Oskamp, 1971). Say, for example, a
person grew up disliking a different ethnic group because of comments she heard
her parents make. Then, on repeated encounters with members of that group, she
might react with distaste and increasing negativity. Over time, these negative
emotions are likely to produce hostile beliefs about the group (Drosnick, Betz,
Jussim, & Lynn, 1992). Thus, negative feelings of which a person might
hardly be aware can lead, with repeated exposure, to the object of those
feelings, to increased negative emotions and, ultimately, to a system of
beliefs that support those emotions. Stimuli, ideas, and values to which we are
exposed shape us in ways that are not always obvious to us.
Direct
Personal Experience
A
second way we form attitudes is through direct personal experience. If we get
mugged one Saturday night coming home from a movie, for example, we may change
our attitudes toward criminals, the police, personal safety, and a range of
other concerns. Or if we have a flat tire and someone stops to help, we may
change our attitude about the value of going out of our way to assist others.
If our father’s business is put in peril because of the
dirty tactics of a large corporation, like that of Ida Tarbell’s, we
would resent such organizations for the rest of our lives. Direct personal
experience has the power to create and change attitudes.
Attitudes
acquired through direct experience are likely to be strongly held and to affect
behavior. People are also more likely to search for information to support such
attitudes. For example, people who had experience with flu shots gathered
further information about the shots and were more likely to get vaccinated each
flu season (Davison, Yantis, Norwood, & Montano, 1985). People are also
less likely to be vulnerable to someone trying to persuade them to abandon the
attitude. If, for example, your attitude that the environment needs preserving
was formed because you lived near a river and observed directly the impact of
pollution, you will be less likely to be persuaded even by powerful
counterarguments (Wood, 1982).
Direct
experience continues to form and shape our attitudes throughout life. One study
examined the effects of direct experience with government agencies on younger
and older individuals’attitudes toward government (Tyler &
Schuller, 1991). The experiences involved, for example, getting a job, job
training, unemployment compensation, and medical and hospital care. The older
people changed their attitudes following a positive or negative experience as
much as, if not more than, the younger people. This finding argues against the
impressionable-years model, which assumes that young people are more open to
forming new attitudes, and supports the lifelong-openness model, which
emphasizes that people can form new attitudes throughout their life. We should
note here that in later years, Ida Tarbell came to know John D. Rockefeller’s successor, Judge Gary, who caused her to write a more favorable
second edition to The History of the Standard Oil Company.
Operant
and Classical Conditioning
Most
social psychologists would agree that the bulk of our attitudes are learned.
That is, attitudes result from our experiences, not our genetic inheritance.
Through socialization, individuals learn the attitudes, values, and behaviors
of their culture. Important influences in the process include parents, peers,
schools, and the mass media.
As an
example, let’s look
at the formation of attitudes about politics. The formation of some of these
attitudes begins early, perhaps at age 6 or 7. In one early study, gradeschool
students thought that the American system was the best and that “America is the
best country in the world” (Hess & Torney, 1967). When children are young,
parents exert a major influence on their political attitudes, but later, peers
and the mass media have a greater impact. In fact, by the time young adults are
seniors in high school, there is a fairly low correlation between the political
attitudes of children and those of their parents (Oskamp, 1991). Parents and
children may identify with the same political party, but their attitudes about
politics are likely to differ.
During the
course of socialization, a person’s attitudes may be formed through operant
and classical
conditioning, two well-known learning processes. In operant conditioning, the
individual’s behavior is strengthened or weakened by means of reward or
punishment. Parents
may, for example, reward their daughter with praise when she expresses the
attitude that doing math is fun. Each time the child is rewarded, the attitude
becomes stronger. Or, parents may punish their son with a verbal rebuke when he
expresses that same attitude. In these examples, operant conditioning serves to
impart attitudes.
Simply
rewarding people for expressing an attitude can affect what they believe. In
one study, participants took part in a debate and were randomly assigned to one
or the other side of an issue (Scott, 1957). Those debaters who were told,
again randomly, that they won were more likely to change their attitudes in the
direction of their side of the topic than those who were told that they lost.
In
classical conditioning, a stimulus comes to evoke a response it previously did
not call up. Classical conditioning occurs by repeatedly pairing this stimulus
(the conditioned stimulus) with a stimulus that does have the power to evoke
the response (the unconditioned stimulus).
How might
attitudes be learned through classical conditioning? In one experiment, when an
attitude object (a person) was paired with positive or negative stimuli,
participants came to associate the person with the positive or negative
emotions (Krosnick et al., 1992). Participants were shown nine different slides
in which a target person was engaged in various activities, such as walking on
a street or getting into a car. Immediately before each slide there were very
short exposures (13 milliseconds) of positive slides (e.g., newlyweds, a pair
of kittens) or negative slides (e.g., a face on fire, a bloody shark). The
participants then reported their impressions of the person. Generally,
participants who had seen the person paired with warm, positive stimuli rated
the person as having a better personality and as more physically attractive
than did those who had seen the person paired with violent, negative stimuli.
Observational
Learning
Although
we often learn attitudes by getting rewarded, we can also learn simply by
observing. One often hears parents, shocked by the aggressive attitudes and
behavior of their child, ask, “Now, where could she have gotten that from?”
Research shows that children may learn to act aggressively by watching violent
movies or by seeing their friends fight (Bandura, 1977). Observational
learningoccurs when we watch what people do and then model, or imitate, that
behavior. For example, a child who hears her mother say, “We should keep that
kind of people out of our schools,” will very likely express a version of that
attitude.
Observational
learning does not depend on rewards, but rewards can strengthen the learning.
In the preceding example when the child expresses the attitude she has
imitated, the mother might reward her with an approving smile. Furthermore,
people are more likely to imitate behavior that is rewarded. Thus, if
aggressive behavior seems to be rewarded—if children observe that those who use
violence seem to get what they want—it is more likely to be imitated.
When there
are discrepancies between what people say and what they do, children tend to
imitate the behavior. A parent may verbally instruct a child that violence is a
bad way of solving conflicts with other children. However, if the child
observes the parent intimidate the newspaper carrier into bringing the paper to
the front door rather than dropping it on the driveway, the child has noticed
the truth of the matter. The parent thinks she is imparting one attitude toward
violence but in fact is conveying another.
The Effect
of the Mass Media
Mass media
play an important role in our society. For example, media heroes tend to be a
very important influence in the development of our attitudes toward all manner
of things: race, gender, violence, crime, love, and sex. Issues given extensive
coverage in the media become foremost in the public’s
consciousness. For example, the saturation coverage of the 2004 presidential election elevated
politics to a level not often considered by the average person. Television is a
particularly pervasive medium, with 99% of children between the ages of 2 and
10 living in homes with a television, and 89% living in homes with more than one
television (Kaiser Family Foundation, 1999). Research shows that children 8 to
18 years of age watch nearly 7 hours per day (Kaiser Family Foundation, 1999).
What do
they see during those hours? Most get a constant fare of violence. This
violence affects the attitudes of at least some children in their interactions
with peers, and the more violence they see, the more aggressive their
interaction style. This effect is strongest in children in neighborhoods where
violence is commonplace; the TV violence evidently serves as reinforcement.
In
addition to providing aggressive models, many TV programs emphasize situations
that are linked to violence. People who watch a lot of TV are likely to
overestimate by far the amount of violence and crime that occurs in the world
(Jowett & O’Donnell, 1992). As a result, they are more likely to anticipate
violence in their own lives. Anderson, Carnagey, and Eubanks (2003) studied the
effects of songs with violent lyrics on both the listeners’attitudes
and their feelings. In a series of five studies, Anderson and his colleagues reported that college
students who listened to a violent song felt more hostile and reported an
increase in aggressive thoughts compared to another group that heard a similar
but nonviolent song (Anderson et al., 2003, p. 960). Of course, it may not
always be the lyrics themselves that cause these changes in attitudes and
feelings. Research suggests that tense, pounding musical scores provoke
aggressive feelings also (Rubin, West, & Mitchell, 2001). In fact, Rubin et
al. (2001) reported that college students who preferred heavy metal and rap
music expressed more hostile attitudes. It’s not clear what the line of causality is in this case.
It is reasonable to suggest that people prefer rap because they feel hostile in
the first place, and thus it is not necessarily the lyrics that cause the
attitudes. However, as Anderson et al. (2003) observe, every exposure to a
violent media event (TV, music, violent video games, violent movies) is a
“learning trial in which one rehearses aggressive thoughts and feelings,” and
these repetitive events make hostile attitudes quite prominent and easy to
recall and access (Anderson et al., 2003, p. 964). By emphasizing some events
and ignoring others, television, movies, and music, along with other mass
media, define reality for us. They directly affect how many of us think and
feel about the world. In one study, Chinese and Canadian children were asked to
imagine that they were an animal and then write a story including themselves as
that animal. The results showed that male children selected animals that were
dangerous, strong, and wild. On the other hand, female children selected
animals that were safe, weak, and tame (Harvey, Ollila, Baxter, & Guo,
1997). In another study, Trepainer and Romatowski (1985) analyzed stories
written by male and female children for a “young author’s”
competition. Specifically, they analyzed the stories for portrayals of male and female characters. As one might
expect, male authors included more male characters in their stories, and female
authors included more female characters. However, overall, male characters
outnumbered female characters. Positive attributes were more likely to be
attributed to male characters (74%) than to female characters (26%). Both male
and female authors assigned fewer occupational roles to female characters than
male characters. Additionally, males tended to have a wider variety of
interesting roles assigned to them than females. Thus, the themes in children’s
stories reflect the
content of books to which they are exposed. The media have a definite role in
shaping a child’s worldview
of appropriate gender-based roles.
Wells and
Twenge (2005) combined 530 studies that studied over a quarter of a million
subjects in a “meta-analysis” and discovered not unexpectedly that sexual
attitudes and behavior have undergone enormous changes from 1943 to 1999. This
analysis showed that the largest changes occurred among girls and young women.
Both young men and women became more sexually active over time, as indicated by
a younger age of first intercourse, which was lowered from 19 to 15 years among
young women, and percentage of sexually active young women, from 13% to 47% in
1999 (Wells & Twenge, 2005). Feelings of sexual guilt decreased for both
men and women. Wells and Twenge observe that their data support the idea that
culture has a large effect on women’s sexuality.
Why the
change? Wells and Twenge (2005) note the enormous cultural changes that
occurred in the past 50 years. Changes in sexual attitudes and behaviors are
among the most noticeable and striking of these shifts. The authors believe
that the mass media had an enormous impact on sexual attitudes and behavior.
They note that “television programs and movies regularly mention topics such as
teenage pregnancy, abortion, sexually transmitted diseases, and rape, whereas
30 years ago these topics were taboo. This sexual revolution has dramatically
altered American culture, especially for women” (Wells & Twenge, 2005).
How Video
Games and Other Violent Media Affect Attitudes
about
Aggression and Violence
Exposure
to violent video games has been shown to both affect attitudes about violence
as well as increase aggressive behavior (Anderson, 2006; Barthelow, Sestir,
& Davis, 2005). Media consumption is perhaps the favorite activity of most
Americans. At least, it occupies a large chunk of time. Barthelow et al. report
that the average 17-year-old spends the equivalent of two full working days a
week playing video games. The concern is not so much the time spent playing
these games but rather the nature of the games themselves. The content tends to
realistically, graphically violent (Barthelow, Dill, Anderson, & Lindsay,
2003). For example, Barthelow et al. (2005) had college students play violent
video games and compared them to other students who played nonviolent
videogames. These researches then took short- and modestly long-term measures
of the effect of playing these games. The results show that those who play
violent video games become less empathetic and more hostile concerning other
people and are more likely to feel and act aggressively. It appears that
playing these games affects the players’attitudes about violence. They become less upset by violence; it
becomes more acceptable to them. This is known as desensitization. Being
desensitized to acts of violence lowers the threshold for the commission of
aggressive acts (Anderson & Carnagey, in press) One explanation for the
heightened aggressive attitudes of video game players is that the violent games
bring forth a “hostile expectation bias” (Bushman & Anderson, 2002).
This bias
suggests that violent game players come to expect that other people will
respond to potential conflicts by responding violently. In other words, the games
condition them to expect that others will also act violently. Bushman and
Anderson use the General Aggression Model (GAM) to explain these findings. The
GAM model suggests that playing a violent videogame promotes thinking about
violence, increases the players’level of arousal, and creates angry feelings (Anderson, 2006;
Bushman & Anderson, 2002).
What we do
not know about the effect of violent video games is the long-term impact on the
players. Experimenters have defined “long term” by hours or days, not years.
Obviously, it is rather difficult to study participants over a long term of
months and years. It is necessary to be able to control for the participants’earlier
levels of violence to
obtain a pure reading of the effects of video games. While studies have been
done showing the long-term effects of violent TV shows, similar research on
video games has yet to be done (Anderson, 2006).
The Role
of the Media in Setting the Agenda
How is it
that Michael Jackson gets more play in the media than, say, nominees to the
federal courts? Does it matter? So what if those “desperate housewives” get
more space in the media than a discussion of potential changes in the
immigration laws? Again, does it matter?
Communication
researchers have long argued that the topics most salient in the mass media
tend to set the public agenda. This agenda settingoccurs because the topics
most prominent in the news shape the public’s cognitions, increasing the focus on certain
issues as opposed to others (Kiousis, McDevitt, & Wu, 2005). And how do
these issues get into the media? Sometimes the issues get “hot” just because
they sell newspapers or magazines. Did the actor Robert Blake hire someone to
kill his wife or not? Who cares? Well, it appears lots of people do, so Blake
had his moments of fame.
More
seriously, some argue that through the process of “agenda-building,” various
interest groups, policymakers, TV, and other media personalities and outlets,
including newspaper and magazines, determine which issues receive the most attention
(Scheufele, 2005). What is important about setting the agenda is that it may
work just like priming does in a social psychological experiment—when a
stimulus is primed, it becomes more salient and everything about it is more
easily retrieved by the individual. People who attend to the most salient
topics in the media have strong opinions about those topics and are more likely
to identify with others who believe the way they believe. Issues such as
abortion, immigration, and others are good examples of this (Kiousis, 2005).
Indeed, these issues tend to fracture the public into several, often
antagonistic, opinion groups.
The
Heritability Factor
Most
theories about the formation of attitudes are based on the idea that attitudes
are formed primarily through experience. However, some research suggests that
attitudes as well as other complex social behaviors may have a genetic
component (Plomin, 1989).
When
studying the origins of a trait or behavior, geneticists try to calculate what
proportion of it may be determined by heredity, rather than by learning or
other environmental influences involved. Heritabilityrefers to the extent to
which genetics accounts for differences among people in a given characteristic
or behavior. For example, eye color is entirely determined by genetics; there
are no environmental or learning influences. If the heritability of a
characteristic is less than 100%, then other influences are involved. Height,
for example, is about 90% heritable; nutrition also plays a determining role.
Eye color
and height are clearly based in one’s heredity. But how can complex social structures such as attitudes have a
genetic basis? The answer is that genetics may have an indirect effect on our
attitudes. That is, characteristics that are biologically based might
predispose us to certain behaviors and attitudes. For example, genetic
differences in sensory structures, such as hearing and taste, could affect our
preferences for certain kinds of music and foods (Tesser, 1993). As another
example, consider aggressiveness, which, as research has shown, has a genetic
component. Level of aggressiveness can affect a whole range of attitudes and
behaviors, from watching violent TV shows and movies, to hostility toward women
or members of other groups, to attitudes toward capital punishment (Oskamp,
1991). In this case, a biologically based characteristic affects how one
thinks, feels, and acts.
Plomin,
Corley, Defries, and Fulker (1990) were interested in children’s
attitudes and behaviors
related to television viewing. Learning—particularly the influence of parents
and friends—certainly plays a role in the formation of TV-viewing attitudes and
behaviors. Is it possible that genetics could also play a role? If so, how
could we know this? To answer these questions, Plomin studied the TV viewing of
adopted children, comparing it to the TV-viewing habits of the children’s
biological parents and
adoptive parents. The question he asked was, Would the child’s
behavior more closely resemble that
of the biological parents or that of the adoptive parents? A close resemblance
to the habits of the biological parents would argue for a biological
interpretation, because the biological parents did not share the child’s
environment. A close resemblance to the
habits of the adoptive parents, on the other hand, would argue for an
environmental interpretation. Thus, the study of adoptive children made it
possible to calculate the extent to which TV viewing is determined, indirectly,
by genetics.
Plomin’s
findings were
surprising. There was a very high resemblance between the TV viewing of the
children and that of the biological parents. Although shared environment
influenced the amount of viewing, the genetic component was much higher. This
doesn’t mean that
children whose biological parents watch a lot of TV are doomed to be glued to
the TV for the rest of their days. It simply suggests that there is something
in our genetic makeup that may incline us to certain behaviors and attitudes.
Attitudes
that have a high heritability factor might be expected to differ in certain
ways from those that are primarily learned. Specifically, they might be
expected to be more strongly held. Is this, in fact, the case? There are at
least two indicators of attitude strength: A person responds quickly on
encountering the object of that attitude, and the person is unlikely to give in
to pressure to change the attitude. Evidence suggests that both these
indicators are indeed present with attitudes that have a high heritability
factor (Tesser, 1993). However, genes will be expressed differently in
different environments, so speed and yielding to pressure are not perfect
measures of heritability.
Bourgeois
(2002) found that members of groups also show greater variability the higher
the heritability of the attitude. Thus, if you are against “permissiveness” in
everyday life, an attitude with a fairly high heritability factor, the less
likely your neighbors will influence you to change your opinion. This explains
greater variability in attitudes with high heritability components (Bourgeois,
2002). Usually, groups tend to produce pressures that make people conform,
especially on important issues. But those attitudes that have a high
heritability loading appear to be much more difficult to change. The Importance
of Groups and Networks
While we
have so far emphasized the individual in the learning and expression of
attitudes, many of our attitudes are learned and reinforced in group settings.
Indeed, recent social psychological research has shown that group influence is
the most influential factor in which opinions we express.
It should
not be surprising that group membership is a powerful influence on our
attitudes and their expression. We know by that, as early as 12 months of
age,we are influenced by the emotional expressions of those around us (Moses,
Baldwin, Rosicky, & Tidball, 2001). Geoffrey Cohen (2003), in a series of
four clever and interrelated studies, demonstrated that a person’s
stated attitude toward a public issue was dependent solely on the stated position of the political
party with which the person was aligned. This was true no matter what the
objective of the policy or the person’s own position on that policy. Furthermore, the individuals did not
seem to be aware that the group’s position was counter to what they personally
believed. For example, in one study Cohen presented two versions of a welfare
policy to liberal and conservative college students. One version of the plan
had generous benefits, while the other version had very limited benefits. Some
students read the generous plan, others the stringent plan. In addition, they
were given information that the Republicans or the Democrats had taken a stand
either in favor of or against the plan. Therefore, some conservative students
may have read the generous plan and been told that the Republicans had endorsed
that plan. Similarly some liberals students read the stringent plan and were
told that the Democratic Party had endorsed that plan.
The
results were striking. Both conservatives and liberal participants in this
study simply followed the party line. If their party endorsed a policy, so did
the liberal and conservative students, no matter their originally expressed
beliefs on that issue. So, liberals supported a harsh welfare policy if their party
did, and conservatives supported a generous welfare policy if their party did
as well. In follow-up studies, it became clear that in the absence of any
information about how their party stood on the issues, conservatives preferred
the less generous plan while liberals the more generous one. Cohen also found
that the effect of group information influenced both attitudes and behavior.
As we will
see in the later chapter on persuasion, people may undertake “biased
processing” of information in order to evaluate that information in a manner
that favors their group.
In another
twist on the effect of group membership on our attitudes, Norton, Monin,
Cooper, and Hogg (2003) found that individuals will change their attitudes when
they observe other members of a group with which they identify agreeing with a
point of view that the group had originally disagreed with. In this study,
college students who disagreed strongly with the tuition increase overheard a
supposedly spontaneous interaction between another student and the
experimenter. In actuality, it was a prescripted interaction. This other
student, who was actually part of the experiment, was given the choice of
either expressing an opinion on the tuition increase or leaving the experiment.
If the
“overheard” student was given a choice and she strongly advocated a position
counter to the other students (that is, in favor of an increase in tuition),
some students actually changed their opinion and favored the tuition increase.
Which students?
It is
precisely those students who strongly identified with the student group. Why
was choice important? As we will see in a later chapter, when we observe
someone take an unusual position and do so by his or her own volition, we are
much more likely to believe that the individual has a strong belief in that
opinion. It appears that people may change their attitudes to adjust to the
fact that someone they identify with (a member of their group) has changed his
or her attitude on an important issue and has apparently done so freely (recall
that the student had a choice of whether to express her attitude or leave).
Social Networks
We have
seen the importance of groups on our evaluation of public issues. What we know,
obviously, is that we do not form nor do we keep attitudes in isolation from
important groups.
Visser and
Mirabile (2004) showed that when you are part of congruent social networks
(people with similar views), your attitude becomes more resistant to change
because you have strong social support for that attitude. However, if you are
embedded in a heterogeneous social network with lots of people who have
different views, you are less resistant to change. It appears that when you are
with people who think as you do, not surprisingly, you become more certain of your
attitudes, and any doubts you may have had are removed (Visser & Mirabile,
2004).
Crandall
(1988) studied the patterns of behavior of friendship groups in college
sororities. Residents of two sorority houses completed questionnaires that
dealt with binge eating and their social behavior. Crandall found that binge
eating was caused by “social contagion.” If a student was in a sorority where
there was binge eating, that behavior increased from the fall through the
spring terms. That is, the longer someone was in the group, the more the
individuals’behaviors converged. Crandall further argued that reduced social influence over the
summer would cause dissimilarity of binge eating in the fall, but he did not
directly test this hypothesis. Of course, it is possible that students with
tendencies toward binge eating may have pledged those groups that may have been
known for such behavior (Crandall, 1988). Social psychologists have observed
that individuals will adjust, or “tune,” their beliefs to the apparent beliefs
of other people when they desire to get along with this person. This type of
behavior is referred to as the affiliative social tuning hypothesis (Sinclair,
Lowery, Hardin, & Colangelo, 2005). Often, we will modify our expressed
attitudes so that social interaction in groups is smooth. Therefore, people
will modify their expressed, often automatic (recall the IAT described
earlier), racial attitudes within groups that contain people of different
racial or ethnic groups. Sinclair et al. (2005) have shown that automatic
attitudes serve a social regulatory function, That is, they regulate social
interactions so as to make them less confrontational and more congenial. Thus,
these automatic racial or ethnic attitudes are sensitive to the social demands
of interpersonal interactions. Therefore, automatic attitudes are influenced by
the desire to get along with others.
Attitudes and Behavior
Intuitively,
it makes sense that if we know something about a person’s
attitudes, we should be
able to predict his or her behavior. In Allport’s definition of
attitude given at the
beginning of this chapter, attitudes exert a directive influence on the
individual’s behavior.
There is a rationality bias in all of this—a belief that people will act in a
manner consistent with their innermost feelings and ideas. Do we, in fact,
behave in accordance with our attitudes? Early researchers assumed that a close
link did exist between ttitudes and behavior. However, a review of
attitude-behavior research revealed a quite different picture: Attitudes
appeared to be, at best, only weak predictors of behavior (Wicker, 1969).
We begin
this section by looking at one early study that appeared to show little
orrelation between attitudes and behavior. Social psychologists eventually
concluded that a relationship exists but is more complex than they suspected.
We look at their attempts to unravel the complexities and to thereby show that
attitudes can predict behavior. More recently, other social psychologists have
argued that our behavior often is nonrational and has nothing to do with our
attitudes. We conclude the section by seeing how the rational and nonrational
approaches can be reconciled.
An Early
Study of Attitudes and Behavior
In one
well-known study from the 1930s, a young sociologist traveled around the United
States with a young Chinese couple (LaPiere, 1934). They traveled 10,000 miles
and visited over 200 places (Oskamp, 1991). The 1930s were a time of relatively
overt expression of prejudice against many groups, including Asians. What did
LaPiere and the Chinese couple encounter? Interestingly, during their entire
trip, they were refused service by only one business. Several months after the
trip, LaPiere wrote to every establishment he and his friends had visited and
asked the owners if they would object to serving a Chinese couple. About half
the establishments answered; of these, only nine said they would offer service,
and only under certain conditions.
The visits
measured the behavior of the business owners. The follow-up question about
offering service was a measure of attitudes. Clearly, the expressed attitudes
(primarily negative) and the behavior (primarily positive) were not consistent.
This kind of finding led to a great deal of pessimism among attitude
researchers concerning the link between attitudes and behavior. But let’s
consider the inconsistency more closely. Our
behavior is determined by many attitudes, not just one. LaPiere measured the
owners’attitudes about Asians. He did not measure their attitudes
about losing money
or
creating difficulties for themselves by turning away customers. Furthermore, it
is easier to express a negative attitude when you are not face-to-face with the
object of that attitude. Think how easy it is to tell the aluminum-siding
salesperson over the phone that you never want to hear about aluminum siding
again as long as you live. Yet when the person shows up at your door, you are
probably less blunt and might even listen to the sales pitch. In the case of
LaPiere’s study, being prejudiced is easy by letter, harder in person.
To
summarize, LaPiere’s findings did not mean there is little relationship between attitudes and
behavior. They just indicated that the presence of the attitude object (in this
case, the Chinese couple) is not always enough to trigger the expression of the
attitude. Other factors can come into play.
There are
several reasons why attitudes aren’t good predictors of behavior. First, research showed that it was when
investigators tried to link general attitudes and specific behaviors that the
link appeared weak. When researchers looked at a specific attitude, they often
were able to find a good relationship between that attitude and behavior.
However, when researchers asked people about a general attitude, such as their
religious beliefs, and assessed a specific behavior related to that attitude,
such as praying before meals, they found only a weak correlation (Eagly, 1992).
Another
reason why attitudes and behaviors may not relate strongly is the fact that a
behavior may relate to more than one attitude. For example, whether you vote
for a particular candidate may depend on how she stands on a range of issues
(e.g., abortion, health care, defense spending, civil rights). Measuring any
single attitude may not predict very well how you vote. However, if the entire
range of attitudes is measured, the relationship between attitudes and behavior
improves. Similarly, if only one behavior is measured, your attitude may not
relate to that behavior very well. It is much better if a behavioral trend
(several behaviors measured over time) is measured. Attitudes tend to relate
better to behavioral trends than a single behavior.
Theory of
Planned Behavior
Ajzen and
Fishbein (1980) proposed the theory of planned behavior. This theory sensibly
assumes that the best predictor of how we will behave is the strength of our
intentions (Ajzen, 1987). The theory is essentially a three-step process to the
prediction of behavior. The likelihood that individuals will carry out a
behavior consistent with an attitude they hold depends on the strength of their
intention, which is in turn influenced by three factors. By measuring these
factors, we can determine the strength of intention, which enables us to
predict the likelihood of the behavior.
The first
factor that influences behavioral intention is attitude toward the behavior. Be
careful here: We are talking about the attitude toward the behavior, not toward
the object. For example, you might have a positive attitude about exercise,
because you believe that it reduces tension. Exercise is the object of the
attitude. But you might not like to sweat. In fact, you hate to sweat. Will you
exercise? The theory says that the attitude toward the behavior, which includes
sweating, is a better predictor of your actions than your attitude about
exercise, because it affects your intentions.
The second
factor, subjective norms, refers to how you think your friends and family will
evaluate your behavior. For example, you might think, “All my friends exercise,
and they will think that it is appropriate that I do the same.” In this case,
you may exercise despite your distaste for it. Your friends’behavior
defines exercise as normative, the standard.
Wellness programs that attempt to change dietary and exercise habits rely
heavily on normative forces. By getting people into groups, they encourage them
to perceive healthy lifestyles as normative (everyone else is involved).
Perceived
behavioral control, the third factor, refers to a person’s
belief that the behavior
he or she is considering is easy or hard to accomplish. For example, a person
will be more likely to engage in health-related preventive behaviors such as
dental hygiene or breast self-examination if he or she believes that they can
be easily done (Ronis & Kaiser, 1989).
In
summary, the theory of planned behavior emphasizes that behavior follows from
attitudes in a reasoned way. If a person thinks that a particular behavior
associated with an attitude will lead to positive outcomes, that other people
would approve, and that the behavior can be done readily, then the person will
engage in the behavior (Eagly, 1992). People essentially ask themselves if they
can reasonably expect that the behavior will achieve their individual and
social needs.
Let’s
use the theory of planned
behavior to analyze voting behavior. Assume you have a positive attitude about
voting (the object). Will you actually vote? Let’s say you think that it is the duty of every
citizen to vote. Furthermore, your friends are going to vote, and you believe
they will think badly of you if you don’t (subjective norms). Finally, you feel that you will be
able to easily rearrange your schedule on election day (perceived behavioral
control). If we know all this about you, we can conclude you have a strong intention
to vote and can make a pretty confident prediction that, in keeping with your
attitude, you are likely to vote.
The
accuracy of behavioral intentions in predicting behavior is evident in the
Gallup Poll. The Gallup organization has been conducting voting surveys since
1936, the year Franklin Delano Roosevelt ran against Alf Landon, governor of
Kansas. Figure 5.2 shows the record of the Gallup Poll in national elections
from 1968 to 2001. In general, the polls are quite accurate. Yes, there have been
a few exceptions over the past 57 years. They certainly got it wrong in 1948:
The data indicated that Harry Truman did not have much of a chance to win. But
rarely in history books do we hear mention of Dewey, the governor of New York
who ran against Truman and who was projected as the winner.
Figure 5.2
In this
case, the pollsters were wrong primarily because they stopped polling a little
too early. They had not yet learned that people have other things on their
minds than elections and may not start to pay serious attention to the campaign
until a week or so before the actual vote. Pollsters will not make that error
again.
Although
the question, “For whom will you vote, candidate X or candidate Y?” might
appear to be a measure of attitude, it is really a measure of behavioral
intention. Voting is a single act and can be measured by a single direct
question. These are the circumstances in which consistency between attitude and
behavior is likely to be the highest. Pollsters often try to determine the strength
of these intentions by asking such questions as: How strongly do you feel about
your preferred candidate? How intense are your feelings? Although refinements
like these may add to the accuracy of voting surveys in the future, what is
needed is a concrete way of measuring behavioral intentions.
Recent
research has reinforced the notion that emotions are crucially involved in
turning attitude into behavior. For example, Farley and Stasson (2003) examined
the relationship between attitudes and giving blood donations. They found that
both donors’ behavioral
intentions to give blood and their positive emotions about doing so were
predictive of actually donating blood.
The
Importance of Conviction
So what we
have seen in the previous section is that the importance of some of our
attitudes is a crucial determinant of how we act. Some of our attitudes are
important to us; others are much less important. One reason researchers
underestimated the attitude–behavior link is because they did not focus on
attitudes that are important to people (Abelson, 1988). Attitudes held with
conviction are central to the person holding
them.
Examples include attitudes of racial and gender equality, racism and sexism,
patriotism, religious fundamentalism, and occultism. Attitudes held with
conviction are like possessions (Abelson, 1988). Recall that one function of an
attitude is that it defines us; it tells people who we are. The person owns his
or her attitudes, proudly displaying them to those who would appreciate them
and defending them against those who would try to take them away. For example,
someone deeply committed to one side or the other of the abortion issue will
likely defend his view against the other side and show his solidarity with
those on the same side. Such attitudes will be hard to change, as a change
would mean a major alteration in the way the person
sees the
world.
Because
attitudes to which people are strongly committed are hard to manipulate in a
laboratory experiment, researchers tended to stay away from them. As a result,
social psychologists overestimated the ease with which attitudes might be
changed and underestimated the relationship between attitudes and behavior. If
an attitude is important to people, they expect that behavior in agreement with
that attitude will help them get what they want. Thus, important attitudes and
behavior tend to be closely linked.
An
attitude held with conviction is easily accessible. This means that if you
discuss with someone a subject about which they feel strongly, they respond
quickly and have a lot of ideas about it. Moreover, attitude accessibility—the
ease with which one can bring a particular attitude to mind—is increased by
constant use and application of that attitude (Doll & Ajzen, 1992). In a
study several years ago, researchers measured latencies (speed of response)
with respect to questions about women’s rights, abortion, and racial integration
(Krosnick, 1989). Whatever the issue, people who considered an attitude
important responded more quickly than those who considered it unimportant.
Important attitudes are more available in memory and are more likely to
correspond to behavior.
If your
stand on abortion, women’s rights, gun ownership, or the Dallas
Cowboys is important,
you are more likely to act in a manner consistent with that attitude.
You can
get a sense of how accessible an attitude is by noting how long it takes you to
recall it. For example, notice how long it takes you to recall your attitude
toward the following: living wills, parent-teacher associations, the death
penalty, aisle seats, snakes, water filters, political action committees, the
clergy, daylight-savings time, baseball. Some of these notions brought feelings
and thoughts to mind quickly; others may not have.
If
attitude accessibility indicates strength of conviction, we might expect
attitudes high in accessibility to be better predictors of behavior than
attitudes lower in accessibility. Fazio, who has extensively studied attitude
accessibility, investigated this issue in connection with the 1984 presidential
election (Fazio & Williams, 1986). The summer before the election,
potential voters were asked whether they agreed with each of the following two
statements: “A good president for the next 4 years would be Walter Mondale (the
then Democratic nominee),” and “A good president for the next 4 years would be
Ronald Reagan (the elected Republican).” The respondents had to indicate how
strongly they agreed or disagreed by pressing one of five buttons: strongly
agree, agree, don’t care, disagree, strongly disagree.
The
researchers measured the time that passed before respondents pressed the
button. The delay interval between the moment you are confronted with an object
and the moment you realize your attitude is called the latency(Rajecki, 1990).
The longer respondents took to hit the button, the less accessible the
attitude. Not only were the researchers able to get a reading of the attitude
toward the candidates, but also they were able to get a measure of
accessibility.
On the day
after the election, respondents were asked whether they had voted and, if so,
for whom they had voted. Was there a relationship between latency times and
voting behavior? That is, did attitude accessibility predict behavior? The
answer is, yes, it did. Attitude accessibility measured in June and July 1984
accurately predicted voting behavior in November. Those who had responded
quickly for Reagan were more likely to vote for him than those who had taken
longer to respond. The same relationship held, although not quite as strongly,
for Mondale supporters.
The
Nonrational Actor
The
theories and ideas about attitudes and behavior so far tend to assume a
rational, almost calculated approach to behavior. In the theory of planned
behavior, if you can get measures of people’s attitude toward a behavior, their
perception of how important others might approve or disapprove of what they do,
and their sense of control over that behavior, then you can predict their
intentions and, therefore, their likely behavior. If there is a significant
criticism of the theory of planned behavior, it is that when you ask people to
tell you about the components of their intentions, they know that their answers
should be logical. If you reported that you voted but you had no interest in
the candidates and you thought all candidates were crooks, this hardly makes
you look like a logical individual.
Some
theories have taken the opposite approach: They assume that human beings are
nonrational actors (Ronis & Kaiser, 1989), and our attitudes may often be
totally irrelevant to our behavior. Cigarette smoking, for example, is so
habitual as to be automatic, totally divorced from any attitude or behavioral
intention the smoker may have. Most of our behaviors are like that (Ronis &
Kaiser, 1989). We do them over and over without thought (Gilbert, 1991). You
floss your teeth, but your attitude and intentions about dental hygiene are
activated only when you run out of floss. Even though you believe flossing is
important, and even though you remember that sign in your dentist’s
office that reads, “No,
you don’t have to floss all you teeth—only the ones you want to keep,” you now have to act on your
attitude. Are you willing to get in the car at 11 P.M. and drive to the store
to buy more dental floss? Similarly, if your regular aerobics class becomes
inconvenient, is your attitude about the importance of exercise strong enough
that you will rearrange your whole schedule?
In sum,
people usually behave habitually, unthinkingly, even mindlessly. They make
active decisions only when they face new situations. Thus, there is a good
chance of inconsistencies between our attitudes and our behavior.
Mindless
Behavior in Everyday Life
Have you
ever arrived home after work or school and not been able to recall a single thing
about how you got there? In everyday life, we often run on a kind of automatic
pilot. Our behavior becomes so routine and automatic that we are hardly aware
of what we are doing. We are in a state of mind that Ellen Laner (1989) termed
mindlessness, one that involves reduced attention and loss of active control in
everyday activities. Mindlessness occurs when we’re engaging in
behaviors that have been overlearned and routinized. In this state, we carry out the
behaviors rigidly, according to a preconceived pattern and without thought or
appraisal. Mindlessness is fairly common in our everyday interactions. The
cashier at a restaurant asks you, “How was everything?” You say that your steak
was overcooked, your potato was cold, and the service was terrible. The cashier
replies, “Here’s your change, have a nice day.” In this
example, the cashier’s question and
response were automatic; she really didn’t care how you enjoyed your meal.
Langer was
interested in studying this state of mind (Langer, Blank, & Chanowitz, 1978). She had a researcher approach people
waiting to use a copy machine in the library and ask to use it first. The
request was phrased in one of several ways: “Excuse me, I have five
pages to
copy. May I use the machine because I am in a rush?” “Excuse me, I have five pages
to copy. May I use the machine?” and “Excuse me, I have five pages to copy. May
I use the machine because I have to make copies?” The researcher also asked to
make 20 copies in these three different ways. Request 2 offers no reason for
using the copier first, and request 3 offers a mindless reason (“because I have
to make copies”); only request 1 provides a minimally acceptable reason
(“because I am in a rush”). If the participants in this situation were dealing
with the request in a mindless fashion, they would fail to distinguish between
legitimate and illegitimate (or ridiculous) reasons. As it turns out, any kind
of excuse works as long as the request is small. When the request was to make
five copies, people apparently did not appraise the quality of the excuse as
long as one was offered: Having to make copies was just as good as being in a
rush. People snapped out of their mindless state, however, when the request was
to make 20 copies. It is clear that when the behavior (the request) had a
significant impact, people paid more attention to the difference between bad
and good excuses. Although we usually pay close attention to good and bad
reasons for people’s behavior, it may be that the request to copy five
pages isn’t worth the effort. When the ante is raised to 20 pages, then we are more mindful.
The fact
that we hold a number of attitudes without really thinking about them means
there can be some interesting consequences once we are forced to think about
them. Thinking about our attitudes and the reasons we hold them can sometimes
be disruptive and confusing (Wilson, Dunn, Kraft, & Lisle, 1989). More
generally, the process of introspecting—of looking into our own mind, rather
than just behaving—can have this effect.
Timothy
Wilson’s work showed that thinking about the reasons for our
attitudes can often
lead us to behave in ways that seem inconsistent with those attitudes (Wilson
et al., 1989). For example, if you are forced to think about why you like your
romantic partner, you might wind up ending the relationship in the near future.
Much depends on the strength of the relationship. If the relationship is not
strong, thinking about reasons might weaken it. If it is pretty strong, then
reasoning might further strengthen it. The stronger our attitude or belief, the
more likely that thinking about it will increase the consistency between it and
our behavior (Fazio, 1986).
Why should
thinking about reasons for our attitudes sometimes lead to inconsistency
between our attitudes and behavior? The basic answer is that if we have never
really thought about an attitude before, then thinking about it may cause us to
change it (Wilson et al., 1989). If you are forced to count the ways you love
your current partner, and it takes you a lot of time to use all the fingers on
one hand, you have gotten some insight into how you really think about the
relationship.
This
explanation was supported by a study in which people were asked their attitudes
about social issues, such as the death penalty, abortion, and national health
insurance, in two separate telephone surveys conducted a month apart (Wilson
& Kraft, 1988). In the first survey, some people were asked to give their
reasons for their opinions, whereas others were just asked their opinions. A
month later, those people who had been asked to give reasons proved more likely
to have changed their opinion. So thinking about reasons seems to lead to
change. Why? The full explanation might lie in the biased sample hypothesis,
proposed by Wilson and colleagues (1989). It goes like this: If you ask people
why they believe something, they are not likely to say, “I don’t
know.” Instead, they will conjure up
reasons that seem plausible but may be wrong or incomplete. That is, because
people often do not know their true reasons, they sample only part of those
reasons. Thus, they present a biased sample of their reasons. People then
assume the reasons in the biased sample are their true reasons for holding the
belief. If these reasons don’t seem compelling, thinking about them
may persuade people to
change their belief.
Hal. 179
The
Rational and Nonrational Actors: A Resolution
Sometimes
we are rational actors; sometimes we are nonrational actors. Sometimes our
behavior is “coupled” to our attitudes; sometimes it is “uncoupled” from them.
Isn’t this where
we began? Let’s see if we can now resolve the apparent conflict. It makes
sense to see attitudes
and behavior as ordinarily linked, with uncoupling occurring primarily under
two kinds of circumstances.
The first
circumstance is when an attitude is not particularly important to you. You may
not have thought about the attitude object much or have expressed the attitude
very often. So in this case, you don’t really know what you think. True,
capital punishment and
national health care are important issues. But many of us may not have thought
them through. When you are forced to consider these issues, you may be
surprised by what you say. This may make you reconsider your attitude.
The second
circumstance is slightly more complicated. Essentially, it is when you don’t
have a clear sense of your goals and needs. Let’s go back to the theory of
planned action for a
moment. The theory says if you expect that a behavior can help you achieve your
goals and social needs, you will do it. But people are often not clear about
their goals and needs (Hixon & Swann, 1993). When you are not clear about
what you want to accomplish, then your behavior will be relatively
unpredictable and might well be uncoupled from your attitudes.
For
example, we exercise, but only sporadically, because we are mainly concerned
about looking good in front of our health-obsessed friends. Our reasons are
weak, not clear to us, and therefore our exercising behavior is infrequent and
unpredictable. But if we or a friend the same age has a heart attack, we
develop a much stronger attitude toward exercise. We now know that our reasons
for exercising are to improve ardiovascular function, to enhance our sense of
well-being, and, in short, to save our lives.
Now we
change our schedule around to exercise every day, subscribe to Runner’s
World magazine, invest
in better exercise shoes, and so on.
In sum,
then, our behavior is more likely to be consistent with our attitudes when the
attitudes concern an area that is important to us and when the behavior helps
us achieve clear and strong social needs. Attitudes we hold with conviction are
not ulnerable to uncoupling because we have expressed those attitudes in a
variety of situations and have thought deeply about them.
Why We Don’t
Like Those Who Think Differently
Than We
Do: Naïve Realism and Attitudes
There is a
confirmed tendency to question the motives of those who disagree with us,
particularly when the topic is of high importance (Reeder & Tramifow, 2005,
in Malle & Hodges, 2006). One big reason for this observation has to do
with the power of what the great Swiss developmentalist Jean Piaget called
naïve realism. For Piaget, naïve realism was the last stage of the child’s
cognitive development before
adulthood. It was the last remnant of egocentrism, when our thought processes
are concerned first and foremost with ourselves and our own views of the world.
Naïve
realisminvolves three intertwined processes. First is the belief that we are
seeing the world objectively, and second, that other people who are rational
will also see the world as we do. And finally, if those others don’t
see the world as we do, then either
they do not have the right information or they are not rational and harbor
ulterior and bad motives (Reeder, Pryor, & Wohl, & Griswell, 2005). In
essence, we are motivated to see ourselves as free of bias and objective, and
we have what might fairly be called a “bias blind spot” (Cohen, 2003).
Therefore,
if we examine any hotly contested controversial issue in the American political
scene, we will see evidence of thinking that has elements of naïve realism.
From the perspectives of the opponents of the Iraq War, the Bush administration
is accused of cooking the intelligence books to get what they wanted (a reason
to invade) and of lying repeatedly and maliciously about the situation on the
ground. From the point of view of the partisans of the war, anyone with his or
her eyes open could see that Saddam was a terrible man, a threat to the United
States, and that bringing democracy to the Arab Middle East was a worthy goal.
Anyone who disagrees with that has motive and thought processes that are not
objective. Recall that from the view of the naïve realist, if your opposition
had got the right information, they would see the righteousness of your view.
In the event of Iraq, anyone who has not been exposed to information about the
war is likely brain-dead and not worthy of a response. Thus, the only
explanation left to the naïve realist is to question the rationality and the
motive of one’s opponents.
Reeder et
al. (2005) explored the attitudes of Americans and Canadians (who have almost
uniformly been against the Iraq War from the start) toward Iraq. Please note
this study was conducted in 2004. The experimenters were interested in studying
the tendency (the bias, really) for people to attribute negative motive to
those who disagree with them. In fact, they found that those against the Bush
administration policies (primarily, but not only, Canadians) considered their
opponents as having selfish and biased motives. The same general finding was
true of issues such as abortion and gay marriage. Individuals on each side
consider their opponents to be biased and not rational. However, as you might
expect, the bias held only for those individuals highly involved in the issues.
One reason we know this is that the respondents in the Reeder et al. study seem
to have formed their opinions themselves first and then passed judgment on
their fellow citizens who agreed or disagreed with them (p. 1505).
Our
tendency to ascribe bad motives to our staunch opponents on big issues does not
mean that we ignore or dismiss their views. It just means that we think they
are wrong for the wrong reasons (irrationality and multiple biases). Eagly and
colleagues have challenged the notion that we attend to and select information
that we agree with and reject and indeed ignore information that we find
uncongenial to our most strongly held beliefs (Eagly, Kuleas, Chen, &
Chaiken, 2001). Eagly et al. examined a total of 70 experiments that tested the
“congeniality hypothesis” (to wit, that we only examine carefully congenial
information and ignore the rest). They found that the assumption was untrue.
People do attend to information that disagrees with their strong view. But they
examine it in a specific way. What they do is a kind of “skeptical and active
scrutiny” as compared to information they agree with, which is approached with
a view to confirm the congeniality of that information. Our view of arguments
that offend or challenge us is to figure out what the “devil’is
saying and devise counterarguments to that
view. We know what they are saying, but we will not be convinced by them
because that is not the purpose of our examination. We want to know how to beat
the heck out of those who would hold such views. At least, some of us see it
that way.
IDA
Tarbell Revisited
Today, Ida
Tarbell is not a well-known historical figure, but she held her attitudes with
conviction
and expressed them courageously. Although she didn’t
like being called a muckraker
at first, she realized that there was a lot of “muck” in American life that
needed to be raked. President Roosevelt and the American public came to agree.
Tarbell
followed her beliefs with a powerful sense of purpose. Her early experiences,
her family’s support, and her own strong education and temperament combined to produce a woman whose
attitudes and behavior were consistently in accord. No doubt this is an unusual
situation. Ida was a rational actor; the coupling of her attitudes and
her life’s
work was fierce and unshakeable.
Chapter Review
1. What is
an attitude?
An
attitude is a mental and neural state of readiness, organized through
experience, exerting a directive or dynamic influence upon the individual’s
response to all objects
and situations with which it is related.
2. What is
the relationship of attitudes to values?
A value is
a conception of what is desirable; it is a guideline for a person’s
actions, a standard for
behavior. Our attitudes flow from and express our values. Freedom, equity, and
similar concepts are values, and attitudes toward free speech, voting rights,
and so on flow from those values.
3. What
are implicit and explicit attitudes?
Explicit attitudes operate on a conscious
level, so we are aware of them—aware of the cognitive underpinnings of them—and
are conscious of how they relate to behavior. They operate via controlled
processing and take some cognitive effort to activate. For example, you may
know how you feel toward a given political candidate and match your behavior
(e.g., voting for him or her) to that attitude. It is these explicit attitudes
that we often find having a directive effect on behavior.
Implicit
attitudes affect behaviors automatically, without conscious thought, and below
the level of awareness. For example, an individual may have a quick negative
reaction toward a member of a minority group, even thought the individual
professes positive and tolerant attitudes toward that group. The “gut-level”
reaction occurs without thought and is often distasteful to the individual.
4. How are
attitude surveys conducted?
The most
commonly used techniques for measuring attitudes are attitude surveys. In an
attitude survey, the researcher mails a questionnaire to a potential
respondent, conducts a face-to-face interview, or asks a series of questions on
the telephone. Because respondents report on their own attitudes, an attitude
survey is a self-report measure. A respondent indicates his or her attitude by
answering a series of questions.
5. What
are the potential sources of bias in a survey?
Among the
greatest biases in attitude surveys are badly worded questions as well as the
lack of a random sample of sufficient size.
6. What
are behavioral measures of attitudes?
Behavioral
measures are used to overcome some of the problems inherent in attitude
(paper-and-pencil) measures. The idea is that an individual’s
actions are the truest
reflection of how he or she feels. For example, rather than asking people how
they feel about a new ethnic group moving into their neighborhood, a researcher
might use the “lost letter technique,” in which stamped envelopes are
apparently accidentally lost near mailboxes. The letters have a
foreign-sounding name on them, and one compares the proportion of those mailed
with other letters having more conventional names on the envelopes.
7. What is the Implicit Attitude Test (IAT)?
The IAT is
an online test of implicit attitudes. The IAT measures the relationship of
associative strength between positive or negative attitudes and various racial
and ethnic groups.
8. What
does the IAT tell us about our prejudices?
The
results of the millions of tests on IAT Web sites show that a large proportion
of the test-takers display unconscious biases against other social, racial, and
ethnic groups.
9. How are
attitudes formed?
The basic
mechanisms of attitude formation are the same as those for the acquisition of
other behavior: classical and operant conditioning and observational learning.
In addition, the mass media have had a profound effect on our attitudes and
behavior. Since its entry into American homes 50 years ago, television has
altered our conception of everything from our notions of “the good life” to
sexual behavior. Research has also shown that changes in music genres and the
advent of video games and cellular telephones have had significant influences
on what people consider to be acceptable behavior.
10. Can
attitudes be inherited?
Yes,
indirectly. Genetic differences in sensory structures, such as hearing and
taste, could affect our preferences for certain kinds of music and foods. Also,
aggressiveness, which has a genetic component, can affect a whole range of
attitudes and behaviors, from watching violent TV shows and movies, to
hostility toward women or members of other groups, to attitudes toward capital
punishment
11. What
is agenda setting?
Many
researchers suggest that the topics foremost in the mass media tend to set the
public agenda. This agenda settingoccurs because the topics most prominent in
the news shape the public’s cognitions, increasing the focus on certain issues as opposed to others.
12. What
impact do social networks have on attitude formation and change?
When you
are part of congruent social networks (people with similar views), your
attitude becomes more resistant to change because you have strong social
support for that attitude. However, if you are embedded in a heterogeneous
social network with lots of people who have different views, individuals are
less resistant to change. It appears that when you are with people who think as
you do, not surprisingly, you become more certain of your attitudes, and any
doubts you may have had are removed.
13. What
is the relationship between attitudes and behavior?
Researchers
have found only a modest relationship between attitudes and behavior. One
reason is that more than one attitude may be involved in deciding whether to do
something or not to do it. Second, while you might like to express a particular
attitude in some circumstance, other factors may stop you from doing so. For
example, you may think that your best friend made a grave mistake in marrying
Jane, but you would have to be an oaf to express that opinion in your wedding
toast.
14. What
is the notion of the nonrational actor?
Some
attitude theorists have criticized the theory of planned behavior because it
assumes that individuals are always rational when attitudes are concerned.
Other theorists maintain that humans are nonrational actors and that sometimes
attitudes are totally irrelevant to our behavior. In many cases, according to
this view, people behave habitually, unthinkingly, and even mindlessly in
everyday life.
15. How
has the controversy over the rational and nonrational actor been resolved?
The short
answer is that sometimes we are rational actors, and our attitudes are coupled
with our behavior. Other times we are nonrational actors, and our behaviors and
attitudes are uncoupled. Uncoupling is likely to occur when an attitude is not
particularly important to us or if we don’t have a clear sense of our goals and needs.
16. What
is naïve realism, and how does it influence our political attitudes?
Naïve
realism involves three intertwined processes. First is the belief that we are
seeing the world objectively, and second, that other people who are rational
will also see the world as we do. And finally, if those others don’t
see the world as we do,
then either they do not have the right information or they are not rational and
harbor ulterior and bad motives.
*********************************************
Social Psychology
Third Edition
Kenneth S. Bordens Indiana University—Purdue University Fort Wayne
Irwin A. Horowitz - Oregon State University
Social Psychology, 3rd Edition
Copyright ©2008 by Freeload Press
Illustration used on cover © 2008 JupiterImages Corporation
ISBN 1-930789-04-1
No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or
by any means, electronic, mechanical, recording, photocopying, or otherwise, without the prior written
permission of the publisher.
Printed in the United States of America by Freeload Press.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Komentar
Posting Komentar