Understanding Social Behavior
Social Psychology
Third Edition
Kenneth S. Bordens Indiana University—Purdue University Fort Wayne
Irwin A. Horowitz - Oregon State University
When we set out to write the first edition of Social
Psychology, our goal was to provide teachers and students with a book that
covered the important research and theoretical areas in social psychology in a
concise fashion. In the second edition, we strayed a bit from that original
goal but succeeded in writing a solid, research-based text for the introductory
social psychology course. In this third edition, we have returned to our
original goal and have streamlined the book, while maintaining its scientific
integrity.
Social psychology has become a diverse field, and any
attempt to present a totally comprehensive overview of all of its content area
would be difficult to execute in a single volume or course. Instead, we take the
approach of presenting students with information concerning three questions:
1. What is social psychology?
2. What do we know about social psychological phenomena?
3. How do we know what we know about social psychological
phenomena?
This third edition of Social Psychologymaintains the basic
structure of the second edition: Eleven chapters cover the core topics in
social psychology. By staying with the core organization and length, we believe
that the entire book can be covered in one semester or quarter. Each chapter
has been updated to include citations to new research and, where appropriate,
new topics have been added.
The most obvious change in the third edition is the new
publication format.
The first and second editions were both “traditional” textbooks
published the old-fashioned way. This third edition, however, is being
published by an online publisher and is free to students. Yet, it retains the
scientific, academic, and pedagogical integrity of the second edition.
Social psychology is important, interesting, relevant to
the current world, and exciting. This is truly the golden age of social
psychology, with many bright, energetic people doing so much interesting work.
We hope to communicate to this generation of social psychology students the
excitement that we felt as budding social psychologists when we first learned
about Milgram’s obedience research
or Darley and Latané’s bystander
intervention research. Intrigued by the results of such studies, we
began to wonder how they could be applied to real-life situations that confront
each of us every day. In this edition, we communicate the excitement of the
field so that students new to the area will be as intrigued with social
psychological research and theory as we are.
Most social psychology texts approach the field from the
perspective of research and theory, using examples from everyday life as
illustrations of social psychological phenomena. This approach often leaves
students without a full appreciation of the applications of social psychology.
By applications, we mean not only the usual pplied social psychology topics
that are interesting in their own right, but also the theory and research of
social psychology that can be used to understand the complexities of cultural,
historical, and current events. Social psychology can help us understand how
we, as individuals, fit in with the wider social nvironment. Students will come
away from this text with a sense that they are truly social creatures, subject
to the influence of the social and physical environment.
Changes to the Third
Edition
As noted earlier, the most drastic third-edition change is
the method of publication and delivery. The chapters are now in PDF format, and
as was the case in the second edition, are in simple black-and-white. However,
we have retained the second edition’s
chapter organization, order, and structure. So instructors moving to the
third edition from the second edition should find the transition seamless.
Some second-edition elements have been eliminated. For example,
there are no photographs in the third edition. While photos may add to the
appeal of the book, they have little educational value and significantly raise
the book’s cost. Also, the
lists of suggested readings that ended each chapter in the second edition have
been dropped. We felt that these were of little value to most students and that
any students wishing to do follow-up reading would be guided by the
citations/references in the chapters themselves. The Internet activities that
were at the end of each chapter in the second edition have been removed from
the book as well. These are now found in the student study guide that
accompanies the third edition. Key pedagogical elements from the second
edition, such as the chapter-opening vignettes, opening questions, running
glossary, and focused chapter summaries, have been retained.
Some major changes to the existing chapters include the
following:
Chapter 3, “Social Perception: Understanding Other People”:
The information from the second edition on optimism and dealing with life
events has been updated and reorganized in a new section on positive
psychology.
Chapter 4, “Prejudice and Discrimination”: The core content
of this chapter on prejudice has been retained. However, new material discusses
how we must be careful about defining prejudice because popular and media
concepts of the term differ from a scientific concept. Material has been added
to the implicit stereotypes section on the “shooter paradigm,” which is a way
to measure the impact of subtle stereotypes on overt behavior. We have also
added material on how “thinking different” can attenuate the impact of negative
stereotypes. The section on personality correlates of prejudice has been
updated to include information on right-wing authoritarianism, social dominance
orientation, and the Big Five model. The section on stereotype threat has been
updated with new research on this topic. We have added new sections on
collective threat and on reducing prejudice, which addresses the impact of
training on prejudice reduction.
Chapter 5, “Attitudes”: New sections have been added on
naïve realism and agenda setting. We have also included information on how
exposure to violent video games relates to attitudes toward violence, and how
groups and social networks relate to attitudes.
Chapter 6, “Persuasion and Attitude Change”: A new section
on the gender of the communicator has been added to the communicator section of
the Yale communication model. The material on cognitive dissonance theory has
been updated to include new research on topics such as postdecisional
dissonance. The section on alternatives to cognitive dissonance theory has been
expanded to include a subsection on the action-based model. The persuading the
masses discussion now includes a more-focused discussion of propaganda (its
historical context, definition, characteristics, aims, and techniques).
Chapter 7, “Conformity, Compliance, and Obedience”: A new
section on the social psychology of evil will help students understand this
concept from a social psychological perspective (versus a religious or
philosophical perspective). The section on the banality of evil has also been
updated with new research.
Chapter 9, “Interpersonal Attraction and Close
Relationships”: The section on loneliness has been updated to include research
on the cultural aspects and health implications of loneliness. The material on
Internet relationships has been expanded to include new topics and research.
New information on forgiveness has been added to the section on responses to
conflict in a relationship.
Chapter 10, “Interpersonal Aggression”: A new
chapter-opening vignette focuses on the “Beltway Snipers.” The section on
defining aggression has been expanded to include definitions of indirect
aggression, direct aggression, and relational aggression. The discussions of
gender and aggression, culture and aggression, and the effects of televised
aggression have been updated with new research. New sections look at the
relationship between genetics and aggression, the heat effect (including a
discussion of the general affective aggression model), and the impact of
violent video games.
Chapter 11, “Prosocial Behavior and Altruism”: A new
chapter-opening vignette tells the story of Irene Gut Odyke, a young woman who
helped rescue Jews from the Nazis. The section on assuming responsibility has
been updated to include information on social category relationships and new
research on the limits of the bystander effect. New discussions look at the
role of gratitude in helping behavior, the courageous resistance and heroism
that is linked to the research on those who rescued Jews from the Nazis, and
the relationship between gender and rescue.
Ancillaries
An extensive, computerized test bank of examination
questions is available. The questions in the test bank have been written by the
authors and not by someone paid a small amount of money per question. We hope
that these author-prepared questions will be an asset to the instructor.
As was the case with the second edition, the hard-copy student
study guide has been replaced with a free online study guide. Students can
download materials for each chapter, print them out, and use them as they wish.
The online study guide features chapter outlines, key questions, practice
questions, and Internet activities.
Acknowledgments
A project of this scope requires much hard work and the
support of many people. First and foremost, we would like to thank our wives
Ricky Karen Bordens and Kay F. Schaffer, who provided much-needed love and
support while we toiled on this book.
We would also like to thank the editor at Freeload Press,
Ed Laube; Victoria Putman of Putman Productions, LLC; and Daphne Loecke of
Laurel Arts Design Studio.
Understanding
Social Behavior
Few people are capable of expressing with equanimity
opinions which differ from the prejudices of their social environment. Most
people are even incapable of forming such opinions.
—Albert Einstein—
The events of September 11, 2001, conjure up many memories
and images of what occurred on that fateful day. Most of us can vividly
remember where we were and what we were doing when we fi rst heard of the
attacks on the World Trade Center and the Pentagon. We can also recall the
images of jet airliners slamming into buildings in great orange fireballs,
bringing with them destruction and death. We can see in our mind’s eye the poor
souls who chose to leap to their deaths rather than burn alive in the World
Trade Center towers. We can still experience the horror as those two majestic
towers collapsed and crumbled into cinders, taking around 2,700 people to their
deaths.
On September 11, 2001, we witnessed the worst that human
behavior can offer us: 19 young men deliberately fl ying fuel-laden jetliners
into buildings where unsuspecting people were going about their daily lives.
However, on that day we also witnessed some of the best that human behavior can
offer. Many people—police, firefighters, and civilians—put their lives on the
line to save others. One such person was Rick Rescorla, who is credited with
saving around 3,000 lives that day. Who was Rick Rescorla, and what did he do
that saved so many lives?
Rick Rescorla was the Vice President for Corporate Security
for Morgan Stanley Dean Witter and Company. On September 11, he began his day
as usual: rising at 4:30 A.M., kissing his wife good-bye, and catching the
train to work. He was at his desk on the 44th fl oor of the south tower of the
World Trade Center by 7:30 A.M.He was there when the first jetliner slammed
into the north tower. He was instructed to stay put and not leave the south
tower. He called his friend, Dan Hill, and told Hill that the “dumb sons of
bitches told me not to evacuate.” In typical Rescorla style, he ignored those
directions, telling Hill, “I’m getting my people … out of here.” And get his
people out he did! Using a megaphone to give instructions, he guided over 2,600
of his employees out of the south tower, following an evacuation plan he had
developed.
Once Rescorla had his employees out of the building and
made sure they were safe, he went back into the south tower, which by this time
had been hit by the second plane, to go after stragglers. Nobody knows how many
times he went back in or how many stragglers he saved. Rick Rescorla perished
when the south tower collapsed. What we do know is that because of Rick
Rescorla’s actions, only six Morgan Stanley employees lost their lives that
day. Due to his assistance in both the evacuation of the south tower and a
building across the street, Rescorla is credited with saving nearly 3,000
people.
Social Psychology
and the
Understanding of Social Behavior
The events that occurred on September 11 in general and
Rick Rescorla’s actions in particular,
raise many questions about why things happened the way they did. In the
aftermath of 9/11, many questioned the motives of the hijackers (officially and
unofficially). It puzzles us when we try to figure out why 19 young men would
sacrifice themselves to murder 3,000 total strangers. What internal and social
forces can possibly explain such behavior? We also marvel at the behavior of
people like Rick Rescorla. Why did he run back into the burning south tower to
save people in need? It causes us to question whether we ourselves would have
the courage to do such a thing. Most of us are content with coming up with
so-called commonsense explanations for events such as 9/11. For example, we
label the hijackers as “evil,” or “disturbed,” or just plain “nuts.” We
conclude that Rick Rescorla was a special person imbued with qualities that
allowed him to do what he did in the face of death. However, as is often the
case, such simple, commonsense explanations do not give us the final answers to
our questions. Behavior is simply much too complex to be explained in overly
simplistic terms. This is why we turn to science to help us better understand
and explain events such as 9/11.
One science that can help us make sense out of the things
that happen to us and around us is psychology, which is the study of behavior
and the motives and cognitions that underlie that behavior. By studying
“abnormal psychology,” “personality psychology,” and other areas of psychology,
we can begin to piece together rational explanations for events such as 9/11.
One branch of psychology can give us a unique perspective on behavior and
perhaps help us best understand events that occur to us and around us: social
psychology. Social psychologyis the scientific study of how individuals think
and feel about, interact with, and influence one another, individually and in
groups. It is the branch of psychology that studies social behavior—the
thinking and behavior of individuals as they relate to other human beings.
Social psychology provides tools to help you understand
things that happen in your personal life. It can help you make sense of your
day-to-day interactions—your friendships, love relationships, interactions at
work, and performance at school. It can give you insight, for example, into why
your most recent romantic relationship did not succeed, and why you find
yourself attracted to one person in your afternoon math class but not to
another. It can also help you understand why you may behave aggressively when
someone cuts ahead of you in a cafeteria line, or why you get annoyed when
someonesits right next to you in a theater when there are plenty of other empty
seats. Social psychology can also help you understand why otherpeople act the
way they do. For example, social psychology can help us understand the forces
that led to the attacks on 9/11 and Rick Rescorla’s heroism.
Your life also is touched by events beyond your immediate,
day-to-day affairs—events that occur in the community and the nation. Although
these events are more distant, you may still feel strongly about them and find
a link between them and your personal life. If your friend’s father were very sick, for example, you
might want to share with him knowledge about a man whose determination
kept him alive for six years. Perhaps the story would encourage him to keep on
with his life. If a terrorist attack happened in your hometown, you would
experience directly the consequences of young men driven to acts of murder by a
radical ideology. You probably would hear many people decrying terrorism and
talking about ways to deal with such acts.
In one form or another, all the events of 9/11 represent
recurring themes in human history. Terrorism dates back hundreds, perhaps
thousands of years. As soon as humans began to claim ownership of territory,
they began to fight with each other. Humans have always been both aggressive and
altruistic toward one another. Human beings have always had to find ways to
live with each other. We have always functioned together in groups; had love
relationships; tried to persuade others of our point of view; followed or
rebelled against authority; and sought ways to resolve conflicts, whether
through negotiation or through coercion. We help each other, and we hurt each
other. We display prejudice and discrimination; we even have tried to kill
entire populations. History is a tapestry of the best and the worst that human
beings can do. Social psychology can help us understand these human social
events in their infinite variety.
It’s important
to note, however, that social psychologists do not simply wonder and speculate
about social behavior. Instead, they use scientific methods involving carefully
designed and executed research studies to help explain complex, uncertain
social issues. Social psychology is first and foremost a science. Through
theory, research, and thoughtful application of concepts and principles to
real-life situations, social psychologists provide insights into everyday
events, both past and present, as well as those monumental events that are the
stuff of history.
More than any other branch of psychology, social psychology
offers a broad perspective on human behavior. Rather than focusing on the
personal histories of individuals (as would a personality psychologist), or on
how individuals respond to their environment (as would a strict behaviorist),
it looks at how people interact with and relate to each other in social
contexts. It is within these social contexts that a wide range of behaviors and
events fall.
A Model for
Understanding Social Behavior
Social psychologists are interested in the forces that
operate on individuals and cause them to engage in specific examples of social
behavior. But social behavior is typically complex and has many contributing
causes. Consequently, explaining social behavior is a difficult task. To
simplify this task, we can assign the multiple causes of social behavior to one
of two broad categories: the situation and the individual. According to a
formula first proposed by Kurt Lewin (1936), one of the important early figures
in social psychology, social behavior is a function of the interaction of the
situation and the individual’s
characteristics, or
Behavior = f(social situation × individual characteristics)
Lewin’s model of
social behavior was inspired by his observation that the individual’s
perception of a situation is influenced by the tasks he or she has to
accomplish. Lewin was a soldier in the German army during World War I.
He noticed that as he came nearer to the battlefield, his view of the world
changed. Where he once might have seen beautiful flowers and beckoning forests,
he now saw boulders to hide behind and gullies from which he could ambush the
enemy. Lewin came to believe that a person’s perception of the world is influenced by what he or she has
to do in that situation. He termed the combination of individual needs and
situational factors the psychological fieldin which the individual lives
(Pratkanis & Aronson, 1992).
According to this view, individuals with different needs
and tasks would come to see the same event in dissimilar ways (Pratkanis &
Aronson, 1992). Although Lewin looked at the individual’s needs and tasks, he emphasized the importance of social context
in producing the forces that control the individual’s actions. Lewin was aware that we often fail to take
situational factors into account when we try to explain why people behave as
they do (Ross & Nisbett, 1991). For example, there were undoubtedly other
young men with similar backgrounds to the 19 hijackers. However, their
differing needs and interpretations of the social situation did not manifest
itself in an overt act of mass killing. There were probably many bystanders on
9/11 who heard people in the burning towers calling from help. Yet, those cries
did not resonate in them the same way they resonated in Rick Rescorla. Thus far
we have seen that the situation and individual characteristics are central to
the understanding of social behavior in a general way. How do social
psychologists define situation and individual characteristics? Let’s take a closer look.
The Social Situation
The social situationcomprises all influences on behavior those
are external to the individual. A situational factor might be any aspect of the
physical and/or social environment (the presence of other people, real or
imagined) that influences behavior. Different individuals will react differently
to the social situation.
Sometimes the situation works on us in subtle ways. We may
modify our behavior even if there is no pressure on us to do so. We may imagine
or believe that we are expected to act a certain way in a certain situation,
and those beliefs can be as powerful as the situation itself. For example, let’s say that you are in a restaurant with a
group of friends. You are trying to decide what to order. You are
leaning toward the sautéed buffalo, but the stewed rabbit sounds good too. When
the waiter comes to the table, you order last, intending to try the buffalo.
However, each of your friends orders the rabbit. When your turn comes, you also
order the rabbit. You modified your behavior based on your friends’actions, because you didn’t want to
appear different. You felt and responded to social pressure of your own
making!
Situational or social determinants of behavior exist on
several levels simultaneously. Sometimes the social environment leads to
temporary changes in behavior, as was the case in the restaurant. Ordering the
rabbit may be specific to that one situation; you may never order rabbit again.
In other cases, the social environment is a more pervasive influence and may
lead to relatively permanent, enduring patterns of behaviors. The culture
within which a person lives exerts a long-lasting influence over a wide range
of behaviors. Culture influences the foods we like, how we relate to members of
the other sex, the amount of personal space we require (the area immediately
surrounding us that we claim and defend), what we plan and expect to accomplish
in life, and a host of other behaviors. It may also influence one’s decision concerning flying airliners
into inhabited buildings
Individual Characteristics
Individual
characteristics include sex, age, race or ethnicity, personality
characteristics, attitudes, self-concept, ways of thinking, and so on. In
short, individual characteristics consist of anything internal to the person
that might influence behavior. Physical traits are individual characteristics
that are relatively enduring and for the most part known to others. Personality
characteristics also tend to be enduring, but they are not necessarily obvious
to others. Personality is an area of growing interest in social psychology
today (Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991). Other internal characteristics, such as
attitudes, opinions, self-concept, and so on, can change over time. People
often have some choice about how much of these areas of themselves they reveal
to others.
Let’s
consider Rick Rescorla again. What of the other people on the scene who did not respond to others’cries
for help? These individuals were subjected to the same situational pressures as
was Rick Rescorla.
However, they did not act in an altruistic way.
Did some
combination of personal traits (e.g., desire for self-preservation) and
attitudes (e.g., it is the job of police and firefighters to save victims) mix
with the situation (e.g., flames roaring inside the building) to produce this
different behavior? Since the situation was similar for others on 9/11, we look
to individual characteristics such as personality traits to understand why some
acted in violent ways and others did not.
Another
important individual characteristic that is somewhat different from personality
characteristics is the particular way each individual perceives and thinks
about his or her social world. Social cognitionrefers to a general process we
use to make sense out of social events, which may or may not include other
people. For example, seeing the events on 9/11 on the news, you probably began
to interpret those events, attempting to determine a reason for the hijackers’behavior.
Eventually, you probably began to make
inferences about the motives of the individuals involved and to form impressions
of them. Social psychologists call this process social perception. For example,
thinking about Rick Rescorla, who gave his life to save others, may lead you to
an inference that he was a highly empathic, caring person and was not simply
doing his job as a Vice President for Security. Once you infer these
characteristics and form an impression that he was a caring, compassionate
person, you then settle on these internal characteristics as the primary
motivation for his behavior.
Social
cognition and social perception are central to our interpretation of
situations. When we are exposed to a particular situation, how we respond
depends on how we interpret that situation. Social cognition gives direction to
our interpretation. The decisions we make based on our perception and cognition
will influence our response. Every individual has a slightly different view of
the world, because everyone has unique personal traits and a unique history of
life experiences. This is because each of us actively constructs our own view
of our social world, based on interpretations of social information.
Expanding Lewin’s Model
Lewin’s
model tells us that both the social situation (physical setting, the presence of other
people, real or imagined) and individual characteristics (physical traits,
personality traits, attitudes and habitual ways of thinking, perceptual and
cognitive processes, needs and tasks) influence social behavior. Lewin’s
model, however, does not specify how
situational factors and individual characteristics fit together into a broad,
general model of social behavior. We need to expand on Lewin’s
original model to gain a better understanding
of the forces that shape social behavior. An expansion of Lewin’s
original model is shown in Figure 1.1.
As shown
in this model, input from the social situation and individual characteristics
do not directly influence social behavior. Instead, they both contribute to how
we process information via mechanisms of social cognition and social
perception. How that information is processed yields a particular evaluation of
the situation. For example, in the wake of 9/11, controversy swirls around how
the site of the World Trade Center should be used. Some want to redevelop the
area, building a new office tower to replace the fallen towers. Others see the
site as hallowed ground and maintain that the site should be used mainly for a
memorial to those who were killed or injured. Even those who want a memorial
constructed cannot agree on what form that memorial should take. A person
(individual characteristics) who opposes redeveloping the World Trade Center
site commercially may interpret the situation (social cognition) in a way that
suggests that it is sacrilegious to the dead and injured to build a new office
tower. Another person might focus on the economy of the area when supporting
the construction of a new office tower.
According
to Figure 1.1, our evaluation of the social situation does not translate
immediately into overt social behavior. Instead, based on our evaluation of the
situation, we form a behavioral intention. For example, one family of a 9/11
victim may decide to sue the owners of the World Trade Center, blaming
inadequate safety measures in the buildings for the loss of their loved one.
Another family might form an intention to direct their energies into raising
money to help the children who lost parents on 9/11. In these cases, the same
event yields different intentions. Thus, a behavioral intention is the
immediate, proximate cause for social behavior.
It is important
to realize that just because we form a behavioral intention does not mean we
will act on that intention. For example, a person can form the intention of
filing a lawsuit but never follow through, thinking that perhaps more harm than
good would be done.
This view
of social behavior implies that it is a dynamic process. Our monitoring of the
social situation does not end with an evaluation of the situation, or the
formation of an intention, or social behavior. Instead, we are constantly
monitoring the social situation (our own behavior and that of others) and may
modify our assessment of it on a moment-to-moment basis. Thus, we fine-tune our
behavioral intentions up to the point that we engage in social behavior. So,
even though the various processes underlying social behavior are presented in
Figure 1.1 in a sequence of discrete boxes, they are really quite fluid and
involve constant updating of our evaluation of the situation.
One final
aspect of this model needs to be addressed. Notice that in Figure 1.1 there is
a dotted arrow going from social behavior to the social situation. In any
social situation in which we are directly involved, our own behavior influences
the social environment and probably will cause changes in the behavior of
others. For example, imagine that you are talking to someone you have just met.
Based on the first thing she says, you determine that she is not very friendly.
Consequently, you become defensive (you fold your arms, lean away from her) and
respond to her in a cold way. She picks up on your behavior and becomes colder
herself. This cycle continues until one of you breaks off the conversation.
How might
this situation have played out if you had interpreted her initial behaviors as
nervousness and responded to her in a positive way? You may have made a new
friend.
Thus, your
own interpretations and behaviors had a profound effect on the situation.
Social Psychology and Related Fields
We have
seen that social psychology is a field of study that seeks to understand and
explain social behavior—how individuals think and act in relation to other
people. Yet many other disciplines are also concerned with the thoughts and
actions of human beings, both individually and in groups. In what ways does
social psychology differ from its two parent disciplines, sociology and
psychology? And how is it similar to and different from other fields of study,
such as biology, anthropology, and history?
To see how
these fields differ in their approaches, let’s consider a
single question:
Why do groups
of people, including nations, display hostility toward one another? Although
social psychologists are interested in this social problem, they have no unique
claim to it (nor to others). Biologists, psychologists, anthropologists,
sociologists, historians, and others all have explanations for the never-ending
cycle of human violence. Let’s consider first those fields that look for the
causes of violent behavior within the individual and then move on to fields
that focus increasingly on factors in the environment.
Many
biologists say the answer to the puzzle of human violence resides not in our
social situations, organizations, or personalities but rather in our genetic
structure. For example, scientists have identified a tiny genetic defect that
appears to predispose some men toward violence. Scientists studied a large
Dutch family with a history of violent and erratic behavior among many,
although not all, of the males. They found that those males who were prone to
violence had an enzyme deficiency due to a mutation of a gene carried by the X
chromosome (Brunner, Nelon, Breakefield, Ropers, & van Oost, 1993). Because
men have only one X chromosome, they were the only ones who manifested the
defect. Women may be carriers of the deficiency, but they are protected from
expressing it by their second X chromosome with its backup copy of the gene.
Geneticists
do not argue that genetic defects are the sole cause of violence, but they do
say that these factors play a definite role in determining who is violent.
Another
biologically oriented view of this question comes from developmental
psychologists (who study the development of human beings across the lifespan).
They suggest that human beings may have an innate fear of strangers. They point
out that at about 4 or 5 months, infants begin to react with fear to novel or
unusual stimuli, such as the faces of strangers (Hebb & Thompson, 1968).
Between 6 and 18 months, infants may experience intense stranger anxiety. These
psychologists, as well as some biologists, argue that fear of strangers may be
part of our genetic heritage. Early humans who possessed this trait may have
been more likely to survive than those who didn’t, and they passed
the trait down to us.
On a group or societal level, this innate mistrust of strangers might be
elaborated into hostility, aggression, or even warfare. Other psychologists,
however, are not convinced that fear of the novel is inborn (Hebb &
Thompson, 1968).
Along
similar lines, anthropologists (who study the physical and cultural development
of the human species) have documented that some tribal societies view strangers
with suspicion and may even attempt to kill them. Some anthropologists argue
that hostility to strangers may have benefited early human groups by helping
them unite against threats from the outside.
Other
scientists emphasize the psychological makeup of individuals as a way of
explaining behavior. Personality psychologists suggest that aggressiveness (or
any other behavioral trait) is a characteristic of the individual. The person
carries the trait from situation to situation, expressing it in any number of
different circumstances (Derlega, Winstead, & Jones, 1991). Personality
psychologists would argue that some internal characteristic drove Rick Rescorla
to behave altruistically on September 11, just as some other personality traits
affected the behavior of the hijackers.
One
researcher studied the aggressive behavior of adolescent boys in Sweden over
3 years
(Olweus, 1984). He found that boys who were aggressive (started fights, were
bullies) in the sixth grade were also physically aggressive in the ninth grade.
Personality researchers take this as evidence that individual factors are an
important determinant of aggression. Over the course of the 3 years, the boys had
different teachers, were in different buildings, and had a variety of
classmates. Yet their behavior remained consistently aggressive, despite the
change in their social situation (Derlega et al., 1991).
Social
psychologists study the individual in the social situation. They are concerned
with determining what characteristics of a situation increase or decrease the
potential for violence. In looking at the question of hostility between groups,
social psychologists focus on the forces both in individuals and in situations
that lead to this outcome.
Whereas
psychology (including social psychology) focuses on the role of the individual,
other fields look for causes of behavior in more impersonal and general causes
outside the individual. For example, sociologists are concerned primarily,
although not exclusively, with larger groups and systems in society. A
sociologist interested in violence might study the development of gangs.
Interviews with gang members, observation of gang activity, or even
participation in a gang as a participant, if possible, would be potential
methods of study.
Although
sociology and social psychology are related, there are important differences
between them. The sociologist asks what it is about the structure of society
that promotes violence; the social psychologist, in contrast, looks at the
individual’s particular
social situation as the potential cause of violence. The social psychologist is
interested primarily in the behavior of individuals or of small groups, such as
a jury.
Sociology
may be empirical in the sense that it attempts to gather quantitative
information. A sociologist might compare rates of violent behavior in two
societies and then try to determine how those societies differ. Social
psychology is much more an experimental, laboratory-based science.
Historians
take an even broader view of intergroup hostility than sociologists. They are
primarily concerned with the interplay of large forces such as economic,
political, and technological trends. Historians have shown, for example, that
one nation can express power against other nations only if it has sufficient
economic resources to sustain armed forces and if it has developed an adequate
technological base to support them (Kennedy, 1987; O’Connell,
1989). One historian documented the importance of a single technological advance—the
invention of stirrups—in accelerating violence between groups in the early
Middle Ages (McNeill, 1982). Before stirrups were invented, knights on
horseback were not very effective fighters. But once they were able to steady
themselves in the saddle, they became capable of delivering a powerful blow
with a lance at full gallop. The use of stirrups quickly spread throughout
Europe and led to the rise of cavalry as an instrument of military power.
History
and sociology focus on how social forces and social organization influence
human behavior. These fields tend to take a top-down perspective; the major
unit of analysis is the group or the institution, whether a nation, a
corporation, or a neighborhood organization. Psychology, with its emphasis on
individual behavior and the individual’s point of view, offers a bottom-up
perspective. Social psychology offers a distinct
perspective on social behavior. Social psychologists look at how social forces
affect the individual’s thinking and behavior. Although the field takes a
bottom-up perspective, focusing on the individual as the unit of analysis,
behavior is always examined in social situations. Social psychology, therefore,
tries to take into account individual factors, such as personality, as well as
social and historical forces that have shaped human behavior.
As
indicated earlier, social psychology is a science. The use of scientific
methods is the primary contribution of social psychology to the understanding
of complex, uncertain social behaviors such as intergroup hostility.
Research in Social Psychology
In January
1992, a celebrity basketball game was held in New York City. There was open
seating at a college basketball arena that held slightly more than 4,000
people.
Therefore,
the first people in the arena would get the best seats. As the crowd outside
the arena grew into the thousands, anticipation built. People began pushing and
shoving to get closer to the doors. As the crowd pressed forward toward the
arena, the situation got out of control, and in the crush that followed, nine
people were killed.
Even if
you only read about this in the newspaper, you probably would wonder how it
could happen and try to come up with an explanation. You might ask yourself,
Could it be that there were thousands of highly aggressive, mean-spirited
individuals waiting to see the game? That would be hard to believe. Well, then,
could the fact that the event occurred in New York City explain it? This also
seems unlikely, because similar things have happened in smaller cities with
more benign reputations, such as Cincinnati, Ohio. Or could it be that the
presence of celebrities, the limited number of good seats, and the excitement
of the event somehow influenced the crowd’s behavior, causing them to act in ways
they wouldn’t act as individuals? This seems more likely, but is it true?
When we
devise explanations for events like these, based on our prior knowledge and
experiences, our attitudes and biases, and the limited information the
newspaper provides, we don’t know if they are accurate or not. Such
commonsense explanations—simplistic
explanations for social behavior that are based on what we believe to be true
of the world (Bordens & Abbott, 2005)—serve us well in our day-to-day
lives, providing easy ways to explain complex events. People would be
hopelessly bogged down in trying to understand events if they didn’t
devise these explanations
and move on to the next concern in their lives. Unfortunately, commonsense explanations
are usually inadequate; that is, there is no evidence or proof that they
pinpoint the real causes of events.
The aim of
social psychology is to provide valid, reliable explanations for events such as
the one in New York City. Rather than relying on conjecture, rumor, and
simplistic reasoning, social psychologists approach the problem of explaining
complex social behavior in a systematic, scientific way. They develop
explanations for phenomena by applying the scientific method, which typically involves
the four steps shown in Figure 1.2. First, you identify a phenomenon to study.
This can come from observation of everyday behavior, reading research
literature, or your own previous research. Next, a testable research
hypothesismust be formed. A hypothesis is a tentative statement about the
relationship between variables. The third step is to design a research study to
test your hypothesis. Finally, the study is actually carried out and the data
analyzed.
Only after
applying this method to a problem and conducting careful research will a social
psychologist be satisfied with an explanation.
Throughout
this book, we refer to and describe research that social psychologists have
conducted to test their ideas, to gain information about events, and to discover
the causes of social behavior. We turn now to some of the basic principles of
research, including the major research methods, the role of theory in research,
the settings for social psychological research, and the importance of ethical
conduct in research involving human participants.
The
principal aim of the science of social psychology is to uncover scientific
explanations for social behavior. A scientific explanation is an interpretation
of the causes of social behavior that is based on objective observation and
logic and is subject to empirical testing (Bordens & Abbott, 2005). To this
end, social psychologists use a wide variety of techniques to study social
behavior. Generally, they favor two research strategies in their quest for
scientific knowledge: experimental researchand correlational research. Let’s
consider the characteristics of each of these methods, along with their advantages and disadvantages.
Experimental Research
One goal
of research in social psychology is to understand the causes of social
behavior. The researcher usually has an idea he or she wants to test about how
a particular factor affects an event or a behavior—that is, whether a
particular factor causesa particular behavior. To establish a causal
relationship between factors, researchers have to use the research method known
as the experiment. Because experimental research is the only kind of study that
can establish causality, it is the method most social psychologists prefer. An
experiment has three essential features: manipulating a variable, ensuring that
groups comprising the experiment are equivalent at the beginning of the
experiment, and exercising control over extraneous variables.
Manipulating Variables
In an
experiment, a researcher manipulates, or changes the value or nature of, a
variable. For example, Sturmer, Snyder, and Omoto (2005) conducted an
experiment to determine if individuals would be more likely to help a member of
their own group (in-group) compared to a member of another group (out-group).
Heterosexual students were randomly assigned to one of two conditions. In the
first condition, participants were led to believe that they were communicating
with a male heterosexual student (in group condition) who indicated that he
just found out that his new female dating partner had contracted hepatitis. In
the second condition, participants were led to believe that they were
communicating with a male homosexual student (out-group condition) who
indicated that he just found out his new male dating partner had contracted
hepatitis.
The
results showed that empathy was a significant predictor of intentions to help
in the in-group condition, but not in the out-group condition.
In this
experiment, Sturmer et al. (2005) manipulated the type of information given to
participants (communicating with either an in-group or out-group member).
This
variable that the researcher manipulates is called the independent variable.
The researcher wants to determine whether changes in the value of the
independent variable cause changes in the participant’s
behavior. To this end, the researcher obtains some measure of behavior. For example,
Sturmer et al. measured the participants’willingness to help the other student. This
second variable is called the dependent variable: It is the measure the
researcher assesses to determine the influence of the independent variable on
the participant’s behavior. The essence of experimental
research is to manipulate an independent
variable (or two or even more independent variables) and look for related
changes in the value of the dependent variable.
The Equivalence of Groups
The second
essential characteristic of an experiment is that there are at least two groups
involved who are comparable at the outset of the experiment. In the simplest
type of experiment, one group of participants receives a treatment (for
example, they are told there is open seating). The participants who receive the
experimental treatment comprise the experimental group. To know for sure that
an experimental treatment (the independent variable) is causing a particular
effect, you have to compare the behavior of participants in the experimental
group with the behavior of participants who do not receive the treatment (they
are told nothing about seating arrangements). The participants who do
notreceive the experimental treatment comprise the control group. A simple
example of this strategy is an experiment testing the effects of a drug on
behavior. Participants in the experimental group would receive a dose of an
active drug (e.g., norepinephrine), whereas participants in the control group
would not receive the drug. The researcher then compares the behavior of the
participants in the experimental and control groups. In essence, the control
group provides a baseline of behavior in the absence of the treatment against
which the behavior of the treated participants is compared.
In the
real world of research, the distinction between the experimental and control
groups may not be this obvious. For example, in the Sturmer et al. (2005) experiment
on in-group versus out-group helping, there is no true control group in the
true sense of the concept. Instead, participants in both groups received a
“treatment” (i.e., in-group or out-group information). Most experiments you
will encounter will follow this model.
In order
to establish a clear cause-and-effect relationship between the independent and
dependent variables in an experiment, the participants in the groups must have
the same characteristics at the outset of the experiment. For example, in the
experiment on norepinephrine and aggression, you would not want to assign
individuals with bad tempers to the 15-mg group. If you did this and found that
15 mg produces the highest levels of aggression, one could argue that the
heightened aggression was due to the fact that all the participants in that
group were hotheads.
The best
way to ensure that two or more groups will be comparable at the outset of an
experiment is random assignmentof individuals to groups, which means that each
participant has an equal chance of being assigned to the experimental or
control group.
Researchers
can then be fairly certain that participants with similar characteristics or
backgrounds are distributed among the groups. If the two or more groups in an
experiment are comparable at the outset, the experiment is said to have
internal validity, and it can legitimately demonstrate a causal relationship.
Researchers
are also concerned about another kind of validity, known as external validity,
or generality. When researchers study how experimental treatments affect groups
of participants, they want to be able to generalize their results to larger
populations. To do so, they have to be reasonably sure that the participants in
their experiments are representative (typical) of the population to which they
wish to generalize their results. For example, if the participants of a study
were all male science majors at a small religious college, the researchers
could not legitimately generalize the results to females or mixed populations,
to younger or older people, or to music majors. If the researchers have gotten
a representative sample of their population of interest, then they can
legitimately generalize the results to that population, and the study is said
to have external validity.
Controlling Extraneous Variables
The goal
of any experiment is to show a clear, unambiguous causal relationship between
the independent and dependent variables. In order to show such a relationship,
the researcher must ensure that no other variables influence the value of the
dependent variable. The researcher must tightly control any extraneous
variablethat might influence the value of the dependent variable. An extraneous
variable is any variable not con trolled by the researcher that could affect the
results. For example, if the temperature in the room where an experiment is run
fluctuates widely, it could influence participants’behavior. When it is hot,
participants may get irritable and impatient. When it is cold, participants may
become sluggish and uninterested in the task at hand.
As just
described, extraneous variables affect the outcome of an experiment by adding a
random influence on behavior. In short, extraneous variables make it more
difficult to establish a causal connection between your independent and
dependent variable.
In some
cases, an extraneous variable can exert a systematic effect on the outcome of
an experiment. This happens when the extraneous variable varies systematically
with the independent variable. The result is that a confounding variableexists
in the experiment. For example, let’s say you are running an experiment on
the relationship between frustration
and aggression. Participants in the experimental group perform a puzzle for
which there is no solution (frustration group), whereas participants in the
control group do a puzzle that is solvable (no frustration group). As it
happens, on the days when you run the experimental group, the room you are
using is hot and humid, whereas on the days when you run the control group, the
temperature and humidity are normal. Let’s say
you find that participants in the experimental group show higher levels of
aggression than those in the control group. You want to attribute the
difference in aggression between your two groups to the frustration levels.
However, it may be that the higher levels of aggression recorded in the
experimental group are due to the high temperature and humidity and not the
frustrating task.
In the
real world of research, confounding is seldom as obvious and blatant as in our
example. More often, confounding results because a researcher is careless when
designing an experiment. Confounding variables often creep into experiments
because independent variables are not clearly defined or executed. The presence
of confounding variables in an experiment renders the results useless. The
confounding variable provides an alternative explanationfor any results that
emerge. Because of this, a clear causal connection between the independent and
dependent variables cannot be established. Consequently, it is essential that a
researcher identify potential sources of confounding and take steps to avoid
them. The time to do this is during the design phase of an experiment. Careful
attention to detail when designing an experiment can go a long way toward
achieving an experiment that is free from confounding variables.
Factorial Experiments
An
important aspect of real-world research is that experiments are usually more
complex than the simple experimental group/control group design we discussed
previously. In fact, a vast majority of research in social psychology has two
or more independent variables. These are called factorial experiments.
As an
example of a simple factorial experiment, consider one conducted by Patricia
Oswald (2002) that investigated the effects of two independent variables on
willingness to help. Oswald had participants watch a videotape of a person
presented as an older adult (Michelle), who was discussing some of her thoughts
and emotions about returning to college. The first independent variable was
whether participants were instructed to focus on Michelle’s
thoughts (cognitions) or emotions (affect) while watching her on the videotape. The second independent
variable was the type of affect (positive or negative) and cognitions (positive
or negative) Michelle displayed on the videotape. Participants filled out
several measures after watching the videotape, including how much time they
would be willing to devote to helping the student shown on the tape. Before we
get to Oswald’s results, let’s analyze the benefits of doing a factorial experiment.
The
principal benefit of doing a factorial experiment as compared to separate
one-factor (i.e., one independent variable each) experiments is that you obtain
more information from the factorial experiment. For example, we can determine
the independent effect of each independent variable on the dependent variable.
In Oswald’s experiment we determine the effect of participant focus (the focus on either
Michelle’s affect or cognition) on willingness to help. This is called a main
effect of one independent variable on the dependent variable.
We could
also determine, independently, the main effect of the second independent
variable (positive or negative cognition or affect) on the dependent variable.
The main
advantage of the factorial experiment lies in the third piece of information
you can determine: the interaction between independent variables. An
interactionexists if the effect of one independent variable (e.g., focus of
attention) changes over levels of a second (e.g., type of affect displayed).
The presence of an interaction indicates a complex relationship between
independent variables. In other words, an interaction shows that there is no
simple effect of either independent variable on the dependent variable. For
this reason, most social psychological experiments are designed to discover
interactions between independent variables.
Let’s
go back to Oswald’s experiment to see what she found. First, Oswald found a statistically significant main
effect of focus of attention on willingness to help.
Participants
who focused on Michelle’s affect volunteered more time than those
who focused on Michelle’s
cognitions. If this were all that Oswald found, we would be content with the conclusion that focus of
attention determines helping. However, Oswald also found a statistically
significant interaction between focus of attention and the type of affect
(positive or negative) Michelle displayed. This interaction is shown in Figure
1.3.
As you can
see, focus of attention had a significant effect when Michelle displayed
positive emotion, but not when she displayed negative emotion. In the light of
this interaction, would you still be confident in the broad conclusion that
focus of attention affects helping? Probably not, because whether focus of
attention affects helping dependsupon the type of emotion displayed.
Evaluating
Experiments
Most of
the research studies described in this book are experimental studies. When
evaluating
these experiments, ask yourself these questions:
• What was
the independent variable, and how was it manipulated?
• What
were the experimental and control groups?
• What was
the dependent variable?
• What
methods were employed to test the hypothesis, and were the methods sound?
• Were
there any confounding variables that could provide an alternative explanation
for the results?
• What was
found? That is, what changes in the dependent variable were observed as a
function of manipulation of the independent variable?
• What was
the nature of the sample used? Was the sample representative of the general
population, or was it limited with respect to demographics, such as age,
gender, culture, or some other set of characteristics?
Correlational
Research
Although
most research in social psychology is experimental, some research is
correlational. In correlational research, researchers do not manipulate an
independent variable. Instead, they measure two or more dependent variables and
look for a relationship between them. If changes in one variable are associated
with changes in another, the two variables are said to be correlated. When the
values of two variables change in the same direction, increasing or decreasing
in value, there is a positive correlation between them. For example, if you
find that crime increases along with increases in temperature, a positive
correlation exists. When the values change in opposite directions, one
increasing and the other decreasing, there is a negative correlation between
the variables. For example, if you find that less help is given as the number
of bystanders to an emergency increases, a negative correlation exists. When
one variable does not change systematically with the other, they are
uncorrelated.
Even if
correlations are found, however, a causal relationship cannot be inferred.
For
example, height and weight are correlated with each other—the greater one is,
the greater the other tends to be—but increases in one do not cause increases
in the other. Changes in both are caused by other factors, such as growth
hormone and diet.
Correlational
research indicates whether changes in one variable are related to changes in
another, but it does not indicate whythe changes are related. Cause and effect
can be demonstrated only by experiments.
In
correlational studies, researchers are interested in both the direction of the
relationship between the variables (whether it is positive or negative) and the
degree, or strength, of the relationship. They measure these two factors with a
special statistical test known as the correlation coefficient (symbolized as
r). The size of the correlation coefficient, which can range from –1 through 0
to +1, shows the degree of the relationship. A value of rthat approaches –1 to
+1 indicates a stronger relationship than a value closer to 0.
In Figure
1.4, the five graphs illustrate correlations of varying strengths and
directions. Figure 1.4A shows a 0 correlation: Points are scattered at random
within the graph. Figures 1.4B and 1.4C show positive correlations of different
strengths. As the correlation gets stronger, the points start to line up with
each other (Figure 1.4B). A positive correlationexists when the values of two
variables increase or decrease in the same direction. In a perfect positive
correlation (r= +1), all the points line up along a straight line (Figure
1.4C). Notice that in a positive correlation, the points line up along a line
that slopes in an upward direction, beginning at the lower left of the graph
and ending at the upper right.
In a negative
correlation (shown in Figures 1.4D and 1.4E), the same rules concerning
strength apply that held for the positive correlation. However, in a negative
correlation, as the value of one variable increasesthe value of a second
decreases. Figure
1.4E shows
a perfect negative correlation (–1).
An
excellent example of a correlational study is one conducted by Del Barrio,
Aluja, and Garcia (2004). Del Barrio et al. investigated the relationship
between personality characteristics and an individual’s
capacity to feel empathy for someone in need. Del Barrio et al. administered a measure
of empathy and personality inventory measuring the “Big Five” personality
dimensions (energy, friendliness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and
openness) to Spanish adolescents. Del Barrio et al. found that “friendliness”
correlated most strongly with empathy for both boys and girls. High scores on
the “friendliness” dimension related to higher empathy scores. They also found
that “energy,” “conscientiousness,” and “openness” all positively correlated
with empathy for girls and boys, although not as strongly as “friendliness.”
“Emotional stability” did not significantly correlate with empathy.
Based on
this brief summary, you can see that six variables were measured: five personality
dimensions and empathy. However, notice that Del Barrio and her colleagues did
not manipulate any of the variables. Therefore, there were no independent
variables.
Although
correlational research does not demonstrate causal relationships, it does play
an important role in science. Correlational research is used in situations
where it is not possible to manipulate variables. Any study of individual
characteristics (age, sex, race, and so on) is correlational. After all, you
cannot manipulate someone’s age or sex. Correlational research is also used when it
would be unethical to manipulate variables. For example, if you were interested
in how alcohol consumption affects the human fetus, it would not be ethical to
expose pregnant women to various dosages of alcohol and see what happens.
Instead, you could measure alcohol consumption and the rate of birth defects
and look for a correlation between those two variables. Finally, correlational
research is useful when you want to study variables as they occur naturally in
the real world.
Settings
for Social Psychological Research
Social
psychological research is done in one of two settings: the laboratory or the
field.
Laboratory
research is conducted in a controlled environment created by the researcher;
participants come into this artificial environment to participate in the
research. Field research is conducted in the participant’s
natural environment; the researcher goes
to the participant, in effect taking the study on the road. Observations are
made in the participant’s natural environment; sometimes,
independent variables are even manipulated in this environment.
Laboratory Research
Most
research in social psychology is conducted in the laboratory. This allows the
researcher to exercise tight control over extraneous (unwanted) variables that
might affect results. For example, the researcher can maintain constant
lighting, temperature, humidity, and noise level within a laboratory
environment. This tight control over the environment and over extraneous
variables allows the researcher to be reasonably confident that the experiment
has internal validity—that is, that any variation observed in the dependent
variable was caused by manipulation of the independent variable. However, that
tight control also has a cost: The researcher loses some ability to apply the
results beyond the tightly controlled laboratory setting (external validity).
Research conducted in highly controlled laboratories may not generalize very
well to real-life social behavior, or even to other laboratory studies.
Field Research
Field
research comes in three varieties: the field study, the field survey, and the
field experiment. In a field study, the researcher makes unobtrusive
observations of the participants without making direct contact or interfering
in any way. The researcher simply watches from afar. In its pure form, the
participants should be unaware that they are being observed, because the very
act of being observed tends to change the participants’behavior. The researcher avoids
contaminating the research situation by introducing any changes in the
participants’natural environment.
Jane
Goodall’s original research on chimpanzee behavior was a field study.
Goodall investigated
social behavior among chimpanzees by observing groups of chimps from a
distance, initially not interacting with them. However, as Goodall became more
accepted by the chimps, she began to interact with them, even to the point of
feeding them. Can we be sure that Goodall’s later observations are characteristic
of chimp behavior in
the wild? Probably not, because she altered the chimps’environment
by interacting with
them.
In the
field survey, the researcher directly approaches participants and asks them
questions. For example, he or she might stop people in a shopping mall and
collect information on which make of car they plan to buy next. The ubiquitous
political polls we see all the time, especially during election years, are
examples of field surveys.
Field
studies and surveys allow us to describe and catalogue behavior. Political
polls, for example, may help us discover which candidate is in the lead,
whether a proposition is likely to pass, or how voters feel about important
campaign issues. However, they cannot tell us what causes the differences observed
among voters, because we would need to conduct an experiment to study causes.
Fortunately, we can conduct experiments in the field.
The field
experiment is probably the most noteworthy and useful field technique for
social psychologists. In a field experiment, the researcher manipulates
independent variables and collects measure of the dependent variables (the
participant’s behavior).
In this
sense, a field experiment is like a laboratory experiment. The main difference
is that in the field experiment, the researcher manipulates independent
variables under naturally occurring conditions. The principal advantage of the
field experiment is that it has greater external validity—that is, the results
can be generalized beyond the study more legitimately than can the results of a
laboratory experiment.
As an
example, let’s say you are interested in seeing whether the race of a person needing help
influences potential helpers. You might consider a field experiment in which
you have someone, a confederate of yours (a confederateis someone working for
the experimenter), pretend to faint on a subway train. In the experiment, you
use two different confederates, one a black male, the other a white male. The
two are as alike as they can be (in age, dress, and so on) except, of course,
for skin color. You then observe how many people help each man and how quickly
they do so. Such an experiment would be very realistic and would have a high
degree of external validity.
Consequently, the results would have
broad generality.
A
disadvantage of the field experiment is that the researcher cannot control
extraneous variables as effectively as in the laboratory. Thus, internal
validity may be compromised. In the subway experiment, for example, you have no
control over who the participants are or which experimental condition (white or
black confederate) they will walk into. Consequently, the internal validity of
your experiment—the legitimacy of the causal relationship you discover—may
suffer. The experiment also poses some ethical problems, one of which is that
the people who purchased a ride on the subway did not voluntarily agree to
participate in an experiment. We discuss the ethics of research in a later
section of this chapter.
The Role of Theory in Social
Psychological Research
On many
occasions throughout this book, we refer to social psychological theories. A
theoryis a set of interrelated statements or propositions about the causes of a
particular phenomenon. Theories help social psychologists organize research
results, make predictions about how certain variables influence social
behavior, and give direction to future research. In these ways, social
psychological theories play an important role in helping us understand complex
social behaviors.
There are
a few important points to keep in mind as you read about these theories. First,
a theory is not the final word on the causes of a social behavior. Theories are
developed, revised, and sometimes abandoned according to how well they fit with
research results. Rather than tell us how things are in an absolute sense,
theories help us understand social behavior by providing a particular
perspective. Consider attribution theories—theories about how people decide
what caused others (and themselves) to act in certain ways in certain
situations. Attribution theories do not tell us exactly how people assign or
attribute causality. Instead, they suggest rules and make predictions about how
people make such inferences in a variety of circumstances. These predictions
are then tested with research.
The second
important point about social psychological theories is that often, more than
one theory can apply to a particular social behavior. For example, social
psychologists have devised several attribution theories to help us understand
how we make decisions about the causes for behaviors. Each theory helps provide
a piece of the puzzle of social behavior. However, no single theory may be able
to account for all aspects of a social behavior. One theory helps us understand
how we infer the internal motivations of another individual; a second theory
examines how we make sense of the social situation in hich that individual’s
behavior took place.
Theory and the Research Process
Theories
in social psychology are usually tested by research, and much research is
guided by theory. Research designed to test a particular theory or model is
referred to as basic research. In contrast, research designed to address a
real-world problem is called applied research. The distinction between these
two categories is not rigid, however. The results of basic research can often
be applied to real-world problems, and the results of applied research may
affect the validity of a theory.
For
example, research on how stress affects memory may be primarily basic research,
but the findings of this research apply to a real-world problem: the ability of
an eyewitness to recall a violent crime accurately. Similarly, research on how
jurors process evidence in complex trials (e.g., Horowitz & Bordens, 1990)
has implications for predictions made by various theories of how people think
and make decisions in a variety of situations. Both types of research have
their place in social psychology.
Theory and Application
Application
of basic theoretical ideas may take many forms. Consider, for example, the idea
that it is healthy for individuals to confront and deal directly with
psychological traumas from the past. Although various clinical theories have
made this assumption, evidence in support of it was sparse.
In one
study, social psychologist Jamie Pennebaker (1989) measured the effects of
disclosure on mind and body. The research showed that when the participants
confronted past traumas, either by writing or talking about them, their
immunological functioning improved and their skin conductance rates were
lowered. This latter measure reflects a reduction in autonomic nervous system
activity, indicating a lessening of psychological tension. In other words,
people were “letting go” as they fully revealed their feelings about these past
traumas. Those who had trouble revealing important thoughts about the event—who
could not let go of the trauma—showed heightened skin conductance rates.
Pennebaker’s work shows that the act of confiding in someone protects the
body from the internal
stress caused by repressing these unvoiced traumas. Thus, this is an example of
basic research that had clear applications for real-life situations.
What Do We
Learn from Research in Social Psychology?
Two
criticisms are commonly made of social psychological research. One is that
social psychologists study what we already know, the “intuitively obvious.” The
other is that because exceptions to research results can nearly always be
found, many results must be wrong. Let’s consider the merits of each of these
points.
Do Social
Psychologists Study the Obvious?
William
McGuire, a prominent social psychologist, once suggested that social
psychologists may appear to study “bubba psychology”—things we learned on our
grandmother’s knee. That is, social psychologists study what is already
obvious and predictable
based on common sense. Although it may seem this way, it is not the case. The
results of research seem obvious only when you already know what they are. This
is called hindsight bias, or the “I-knew-it-all-along” phenomenon (Slovic &
Fischoff, 1977; Wood, 1978). With the benefit of hindsight, everything looks
obvious. For example, after the attacks on 9/11, some commentators asked why
President
Bush or the CIA did not “connect the dots” and see the attacks coming.
Unfortunately,
those dots were not so clear in the months and years leading up to the attacks.
In hindsight, the signs seemed to point to an attack, but before the incident,
things were not so clear. In fact, the 9/11 Commission pointed out that hindsight
can bias our perceptions of events:
Commenting
on Pearl Harbor, Roberta Wohlstetter found it “much easier after the event to
sort the relevant from the irrelevant signals. After the event, of course, a
signal is always crystal clear; we can now see what disaster it was signaling
since the disaster has occurred. But before the event it is obscure and
pregnant with conflicting meanings.” As time passes, more documents become
available, and the bare facts of what happened become still clearer. Yet the
picture of how those things happened becomes harder to reimagine, as that past
world, with its preoccupations and uncertainty, recedes and the remaining
memories of it become colored by what happened and what was written about it
later. (9/11 Commission Report, 2004)
Although
the results of some research may seem obvious, studies show that when
individuals are given descriptions of research without results, they can
predict the outcome of the research no better than chance (Slovic &
Fischoff, 1977). In other words, the results were not so obvious when they were
not already known!
Do
Exceptions Mean Research Results Are Wrong?
When the
findings of social psychological research are described, someone often points
to a case that is an exception to the finding. Suppose a particular study shows
that a person is less likely to get help when there are several bystanders
present than when there is only one. You probably can think of a situation in
which you were helped with many bystanders around. Does this mean that the research
is wrong or that it doesn’t apply
to you?
To answer
this question, you must remember that in a social psychological experiment,
groups of participants are exposed to various levels of the independent
variable. In an experiment on the relationship between the number of bystanders
and the likelihood of receiving help, for example, one group of participants is
given an opportunity to help a person in need with no other bystanders present.
A second group of participants gets the same opportunity but with three
bystanders present. Let’s say that our results in this
hypothetical experiment look like those shown in Table 1.1.
Seven out of 10 participants in the no-bystander condition
helped (70%), whereas only 2 out of 10 helped in the 3-bystander condition
(20%). Thus, we would conclude that you are more likely to get help when there
are no other bystanders present than if there are three bystanders.
Notice, however, that we do not say that you will never receive
help when three bystanders are present. In fact, two participants helped in
that condition. Nor do we say that you always receive help when there are no
bystanders present. In fact, in three instances no help was rendered.
The moral to the story is that the results of experiments in
social psychology represent differences between groups of participants, not
differences between specific individuals.
Based on the results of social psychological research, we can
say that on the average, groups differ. Within those groups, there are nearly
always participants who do not behave as most of the participants behaved. We
can acknowledge that exceptions to research findings usually exist, but this
does not mean that the results reported are wrong.
Ethics and Social Psychological Research
Unlike research in chemistry and physics, which does not
involve living organisms, research in social psychology uses living organisms,
both animal and human. Because social psychology studies living organisms,
researchers must consider research ethics.
They have to concern themselves with the treatment of their
research participants and with the potential long-range effects of the research
on the participants’well-being. In every study conducted in social psychology,
researchers must place the welfare of the research participants among their top
priorities.
Questions about ethics have been raised about some of the most
famous research ever done in social psychology. For example, you may be
familiar with the experiments on obedience conducted by Stanley Milgram (1963;
described in detail in Chapter 7).
In these experiments, participants were asked to administer
painful electric shocks to an individual who was doing poorly on a learning
task. Although no shocks were actually delivered, participants believed they
were inflicting intense pain on an increasingly unwilling victim. Following the
experiment, participants reported experiencing guilt and lowered self-esteem as
well as anger toward the researchers. The question raised by this and other
experiments with human participants is how far researchers can and should go to
gain knowledge.
Research conducted by social psychologists is governed by an
ethical code of conduct developed by the American Psychological Association
(APA). The main principles of the APA (2002) code are summarized in Table 1.2.
Notice that the code mandates that participation in psychological research be
voluntary. This means that participants cannot be compelled to participate in
research. Researchers must also obtain informed consentfrom the participants,
which means that they must inform them of the nature of the study, the
requirements for participation, and any risks or benefits associated with
participating in the study. Subjects must also be told they have the right to
decline or withdraw from participation with no penalty.
Additionally, the APA code restricts the use of deception in
research. Deception occurs when researchers tell their participants they are
studying one thing but actually are studying another. Deception can be used
only if no other viable alternative exists.
When researchers use deception, they must tell participants
about the deception (and the reasons for it) as soon as possible after
participation.
Following ethical codes of conduct protects subjects from harm.
In this sense, ethical codes help the research process. However, sometimes
ethical research practice conflicts with the requirements of science. For
example, in a field experiment on helping, it may not be possible (or
desirable) to obtain consent from participants before they participate in the
study. When such conflicts occur, the researcher must weigh the potential risks
to the participants against the benefits to be gained.
Rick Rescorla and 9/11 Revisited
How can we
explain the behavior of Rick Rescorla on 9/11? Social psychologists would begin
by pointing to the two factors that contribute to social behavior: individual
characteristics and the social situation. Was there something about Rescorla’s
personality, attitudes,
or other characteristics that predisposed him to act altruistically? Or was it
the social environment that was more important? Social psychologists focus on
the latter. Rescorla’s experiences in Vietnam, where he lost
several men under his command, surely helped
shape his behavior on 9/11. Close associates indicate that he was determined
never to lose people for whom he had responsibility. Of course, there were
others who experienced the same kind of loss as Rescorla, but did not translate
it into altruism. His unique way of viewing the social situation led him to do
what he did.
Social
psychology is not the only discipline that would be interested in explaining
Rick Rescorla’s and the 9/11 hijackers’behavior. Biologists studying
ethology would look at
Rescorla’s behavior in the light of what altruism does to help a species survive. Sociologists might
point to poverty and lack of education contributing to terrorist acts. Each
discipline has its own way of collecting information about issues of interest.
Social psychology would face the daunting task of explaining Rescorla’s
behavior (and the
behavior of the hijackers) by conducting carefully designed research. Through
the scientific method, one could isolate the variables that contribute to
aggressive acts and altruistic acts such as those that occurred on September
11, 2001.
Table
1.2 Summary of the 2002 APA Ethical
Principles That Apply to Human Research Participants
1. Research proposals submitted to Institutional Review
Boards shall contain accurate information. Upon approval researchers shall
conduct their research within the approved protocol.
2. When informed consent is required, informed consent
shall include: (1) the purpose of the research, expected duration, and
procedures; (2) their right to decline to participate and to withdraw from the
research once participation has begun; (3) the foreseeable consequences of
declining or withdrawing; (4) reasonably foreseeable factors that may be
expected to infl uence their willingness to participate such as potential
risks, discomfort, or adverse effects; (5) any prospective research benefi ts;
(6) limits of confi dentiality; (7) incentives for participation; and (8) whom
to contact for questions about the research and research participants’ rights.
They provide opportunity for the prospective participants to ask questions and
receive answers.
3. When intervention research is conducted that
includes experimental treatments, participants shall be informed at the outset
of the research of (1) the experimental nature of the treatment; (2) the
services that will or will not be available to the control group(s) if
appropriate; (3) the means by which assignment to treatment and control groups
will be made; (4) available treatment alternatives if an individual does not
wish to participate in the research or wishes to withdraw once a study has
begun; and (5) compensation for or monetary costs of participating including,
if appropriate, whether reimbursement from the participant or a third-party
payer will be sought.
4. Informed consent shall be obtained when voices or images
are recorded as data unless (1) the research consists solely of naturalistic
observations in public places, and it is not anticipated that the recording
will be used in a manner that could cause personal identifi cation or harm, or
(2) the research design includes deception, and consent for the use of the
recording is obtained during debriefing.
5. When psychologists conduct research with
clients/patients, students, or subordinates as participants, psychologists take
steps to protect the prospective participants from adverse consequences of
declining or withdrawing from participation. When research participation is a
course requirement or an opportunity for extra credit, the prospective
participant is given the choice of equitable alternative activities.
6. Informed consent may be dispensed with only (1)
where research would not reasonably be assumed to create distress or harm and
involves (a) the study of normal educational practices, curricula, or classroom
management methods conducted in educational settings; (b) only anonymous
questionnaires, naturalistic observations, or archival research for which
disclosure of responses would not place participants at risk of criminal or
civil liability or damage their fi nancial standing, employability, or reputation,
and confi dentiality is protected; or (c) the study of factors related to job
or organization effectiveness conducted in organizational settings for which
there is no risk to participants’ employability, and confi dentiality is
protected or (2) where otherwise permitted by law or federal or institutional
regulations.
7. Psychologists make reasonable efforts to avoid
offering excessive or inappropriate fi nancial or other inducements for
research participation when such inducements are likely to coerce
participation. When offering professional services as an inducement for
research participation, psychologists clarify the nature of the services, as
well as the risks, obligations, and limitations.
8. Deception in research shall be used only if they
have determined that the use of deceptive techniques is justifi ed by the
study’s significant prospective scientifi c, educational, or applied value and
that effective nondeceptive alternative procedures are not feasible. Deception
is not used if the research is reasonably expected to cause physical pain or
severe emotional distress. Psychologists explain any deception that is an
integral feature of the design and conduct of an experiment to participants as
early as is feasible, preferably at the conclusion of their participation, but
no later than at the conclusion of the data collection, and permit participants
to withdraw their data.
9. Participants shall be offered a prompt opportunity
to obtain appropriate information about the nature, results, and conclusions of
the research, and they take reasonable steps to correct any misconceptions that
participants may have of which the psychologists are aware. If scientifi c or
humane values justify delaying or withholding this information, psychologists
take reasonable measures to reduce the risk of harm. When psychologists become
aware that research procedures have harmed a participant, they take reasonable
steps to minimize the harm.
Chapter Review
1. What is
social psychology?
Social psychology is the scientific study of
how we think and feel about, interact with, and influence each other. It is the
branch of psychology that focuses on social behavior—specifically, how we
relate to other people in our social world. Social psychology can help us
understand everyday things that happen to us, as well as past and present
cultural and historical events.
2. How do
social psychologists explain social behavior?
An early model of social behavior proposed by
Kurt Lewin suggested that social behavior is caused by two factors: individual
characteristics and the social situation. This simple model has since been
expanded to better explain the forces that shape social behavior. According to
modern views of social behavior, input from the social situation works in
conjunction with individual characteristics to influence social behavior
through the operation of social cognition (the general process of thinking
about social events) and social perception (how we perceive other people).
Based on our processing of social information, we evaluate the social situation
and form an intention to behave in a certain way. This behavioral intention may
or may not be translated into social behavior. We engage in social behavior
based on our constant changing evaluation of the situation. Once we behave in a
certain way, it may have an effect on the social situation, which in turn will
affect future social behavior.
3. How
does social psychology relate to other disciplines that study social behavior?
There are many scientific disciplines that
study social behavior. Biologists, developmental psychologists,
anthropologists, personality psychologists, historians, and sociologists all
have an interest in social behavior. Although social psychology has common
interests with these disciplines, unlike biology and personality psychology,
social psychology focuses on the social situation as the principal cause of
social behavior. Whereas sociology and history focus on the situation, social
psychology takes a narrower view, looking at the individual in the social
situation rather than the larger group or society. In other words, history and
sociology take a top-down approach to explaining social behavior, making a
group or institution the focus of analysis. Social psychology takes a bottom-up
approach, focusing on how individual behavior is influenced by the situation.
4. How do
social psychologists approach the problem of explaining social behavior?
Unlike the layperson who forms commonsense
explanations for social behavior based on limited information, social
psychologists rely on the scientific method to formulate scientific
explanations—tentative explanations based on observation and logic that are
open to empirical testing. The scientific method involves identifying a
phenomenon to study, developing a testable research hypothesis, designing a
research study, and carrying out the research study.
Only after
applying this method to a problem and conducting careful research will a social
psychologist be satisfied with an explanation.
5. What is
experimental research, and how is it used?
Experimental research is used to uncover
causal relationships between variables. Its main features are (1) the
manipulation of an independent variable and the observation of the effects of
this manipulation on a dependent variable, (2) the use of two or more initially
comparable groups, and (3) exercising control over extraneous and confounding
variables. Every experiment includes at least one independent variable with at
least two levels. In the simplest experiment, one group of participants (the
experimental group) is exposed to an experimental treatment, and a second group
(the control group) is not. Researchers then compare the behavior of
participants in the experimental group with the behavior of participants in the
control group. Independent variables can be manipulated by varying their
quantity or quality. Researchers use random assignment to ensure that the
groups in an experiment are comparable before applying any treatment to them.
The basic
experiment can be expanded by adding additional levels of an independent
variable or by adding a second or third independent variable.
Experiments
that include more than one independent variable are known as factorial
experiments.
6. What is
correlational research?
In correlational research, researchers
measure two or more variables and look for a relationship between them. When
two variables both change in the same direction, increasing or decreasing in
value, they are positively correlated. When they change in opposite directions,
one increasing and the other decreasing, they are negatively correlated. When
one variable does not change systematically with the other, they are
uncorrelated. Even if a correlation is found, a causal relationship cannot be
inferred.
7. What is
the correlation coefficient, and what does it tell you?
Researchers evaluate correlational
relationships between variables with a statistic called the correlation
coefficient (symbolized as r). The sign of r(positive or negative) indicates
the direction of the relationship between variables; the size of r(ranging from
–1 through 0 to +1) indicates the strength of the relationship between
variables.
8. Where
is social psychological research conducted?
Social psychologists conduct research either
in the laboratory or in the field. In laboratory research, researchers create
an artificial environment in which they can control extraneous variables. This
tight control allows the researchers to be reasonably confident that any
variation observed in the dependent variable was caused by manipulation of the
independent variable. However, results obtained this way are sometimes
legitimately generalized beyond the laboratory setting.
There are
several kinds of field research. In the field study, the researcher observes
participants but does not interact with them. In the field survey, the
researcher has direct contact with participants and interacts with them. Both
of these techniques allow the researcher to describe behavior, but causes
cannot be uncovered. In the field experiment, the researcher manipulates an
independent variable in the participant’s natural environment. The field
experiment increases the
generality of the research findings. However, extraneous variables may cloud
the causal relationship between the independent and dependent variables.
9. What is
the role of theory in social psychology?
A theory is a set of interrelated statements
or propositions about the causes of a phenomenon that helps organize research
results, makes predictions about how certain variables influence social
behavior, and gives direction to future research. A theory is not the final
word on the causes of a social behavior.
Theories
are developed, revised, and sometimes abandoned according to how well they fit
with research results. Theories do not tell us how things are in an absolute
sense. Instead, they help us understand social behavior by providing a
particular perspective. Often, more than one theory can apply to a particular
social behavior.
Sometimes,
one theory provides a better explanation of one aspect of a particular social
behavior, and another theory provides a better explanation of another aspect of
that same behavior. Some research, called basic research, is designed to test
predictions made by theories. Applied research is conducted to study a
real-world phenomenon (e.g., jury decisions). Basic and applied research are
not necessarily mutually exclusive. Some basic research has applied
implications, and some applied research has theoretical implications.
10. What can
we learn from social psychological research?
Two common criticisms of social psychological
research are that social psychologists study things that are intuitively
obvious and that because exceptions to research results can nearly always be
found, many results must be wrong. However, these two criticisms are not valid.
The findings of social psychological research may appearto be intuitively
obvious in hindsight (the hindsight bias), but individuals cannot predict how
an experiment will come out if they don’t already know the results. Furthermore,
exceptions to a
research finding do not invalidate that finding. Social psychologists study
groups of individuals. Within a group, variation in behavior will occur. Social
psychologists look at average differences between groups.
11. What
ethical standards must social psychologists follow when conducting research?
Social psychologists are concerned with the
ethics of research—how participants are treated within a study and how they are
affected in the long term by participating. Social psychologists adhere to the
code of research ethics established by the American Psychological Association.
Ethical treatment of participants involves several key aspects, including
informing participants about the nature of a study and requirements for
participation prior to participation (informed consent), protecting
participants from short-term and long-term harm, and ensuring anonymity.
*********************************************
Social Psychology
Third Edition
Kenneth S. Bordens Indiana University—Purdue University Fort Wayne
Irwin A. Horowitz - Oregon State University
Social Psychology, 3rd Edition
Copyright ©2008 by Freeload Press
Illustration used on cover © 2008 JupiterImages Corporation
ISBN 1-930789-04-1
No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or
by any means, electronic, mechanical, recording, photocopying, or otherwise, without the prior written
permission of the publisher.
Printed in the United States of America by Freeload Press.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Komentar
Posting Komentar