EDUCATIONAL INTERVENTIONS-Prologu
EDUCATIONAL
INTERVENTIONS:
CHANGING ATTITUDES
AND PROVIDING INFORMATION
CHAPTER PROLOGU
This chapter begins with brief passages from
two of the best-known and most widely read books ever written about
environmental problems. The books were -written to educate people about the problems
and, thereby,
change their behavior toward the environment. Such efforts to educate usually have
two main thrusts, which the passages below illustrate: changing people's attitudes
and providing them with information.
The first passage
comes from Rachel Carson's classic
book on the dangers of pesticide use, Silent
Spring
(1962):
[Insecticide and herbicide] sprays, dusts, and aerosols are now applied almost universally to farms, gardens, forests, and homes—nonselective chemicals that have the power to kill every insect, the "good" and
the "bad," to still the song of birds
and the leaping offish in streams, to coat the leaves with a deadly
film, and to linger on in the soil—all this
though the intended target may be only a few weeds or insects. Can anyone
believe it is possible to lay down
such a barrage of poisons on the surface of the earth without making it unfit
for all life? They should not be
called "insecticides," but "biocides."
.. Future historians may
well be amazed by our distorted sense of proportion. How could
intelligent beings seek to control a few unwanted species
by a method that contaminated the entire environment and brought the threat of disease and death even to their own
kind? (PP. 7-8)
[Carson goes on to
argue for the use of biological pest control, a system that controls pests
with predators, diseases, and other natural enemies.]
The second passage is from the popular book 50 Simple Things You Can Do to Save the Earth (Earth Works Group, 1989), published in connection with the U.S. observance of Earth Day 1990. The passage is part of a discussion of water-saving shower
heads from number 24 of the 50 things.
Shower
Facts:
Showers usually account for a whopping
32% of home water use.
•••
A standard shower head uses about 5-7 gallons of water per minute (gpm)—so even a 5-minute shower can use 35 gallons!
•.• "Low-flow" shower heads reduce water use by
50% or more. They typically cut the flow rate to just 3 gpm—or less. So installing one is the single most
effective water conservation step you can take inside your home.
. . . [In addition,
with] a low flow shower head, energy use
(and costs) for heating hot water for showers may dtop as much as 50%. (pp.
50-51)
[The section goes on to explain how to tell if the shower head in your
bathroom is a standard or a low-flow model, how low flow models work, and where
they can be purchased.]
The passage above from 50 Simple Things . . and the entire book—aims to change the way people treat the environment by providing information. The authors don't try to change attitudes; they
assume that the
reader already wants to save the earth. The authors believe, however, that the reader needs to know exactly what to do and how to do it in order to take
effective action.
INTRODUCTION
The books Silent
Spring and 50 Simple Things You Can
Do to Save the Earth were
written on the assumption
that educating people—changing their attitudes and beliefs and providing them with
information—would
change their actual behavior, The kinds of educational efforts we discuss in this chapter
are much more
focused than the kinds of moral and ethical-religious
appeals we discussed in Chapter 3. This is because people's beliefs about
particular environmental issues, such as
the effects of pesticides on bird populations,
and their related attitudes, such as about the widespread use of
pesticides, are much more specific and less
deeply rooted than their morals and basic
values (such as a religious reverence for nature) or their general ideas about how the environment responds to human intervention.
The assumption
about the efficacy of education that underlies Silent
Spring and 50 Simple Things is not one confined to environmentalists who
write books.
It is shared by many public officials, doctors, educators, and ordinary citizens who are
concerned about societal problems. deed, it is almost common sense that education is essential for solving
a wide range
of social problems, and many also believe that a good educational effort will be
sufficient to do the job., —,
Consider, for example, this brief quotation from the Saline, Michigan, hospital newsletter: "Today, marijuana use
is not uncommon in junior high schools, and is
creeping into elementary schools. How can it be stopped? As with any behavior, the most effective way. . .is through
education." Following the same logic, people propose sex education as the
way to prevent the spread of AIDS
and other sexually transmitted
diseases, education on smoking and diet as ways to prevent heart disease, and environmental education as the way to get people to be more
respectful of wilderness areas and other fragile environmental systems
(see Figure 4-1).
..Behavioral and social science research,
however, indicates that this
conventional wisdom—that education is
enough to solve social problems—is oversimplified and misleading. The research shows that education can
help but that education is rarely sufficient. \For example, decades of careful
study of health promotion campaigns
show that it is possible to get people to
stop smoking tobacco or to eat healthier foods, but not with education alone. In the 1960s and 1970s, a large number of programs were conducted
in schools to keep children from
developing the smoking habit. These programs, which operated mainly by providing
information on why smoking is bad for health, changed some of the students' beliefs and attitudes, but rarely reduced the onset of smoking behavior (Thompson, 1978). Other educational programs for health
promotion—to improve eating habits, cut alcohol
consumption and the like—have been plagued by problems of limited success and frequent relapses into old
behavior patterns.
FIGURE
4-1 Some
Written Educational Materials Intended to Promote
Healthy Behavior
Clearly, there have been major strides in
health promotion
in the United States over the past 30 years. Since the 1950s, when the link between
cigarette smoking and cancer first
came to light, the proportion of American
adults who smoke has decreased, and after decades of publicity about the health
effects of fiber and cholesterol in
the diet, sales figures have shown increased consumption of whole grains and fresh produce and decreased consumption of red meat. But these successes are based on more than
just education. We identify the other key elements of success later in
the chapter.
Chapter Overview. We devote this chapter to a careful examination oforefftstpenczspgntra prom
environmental behavior via education. We
focus on interventions that aim to change
people's behavior in the relatively
short run. (We do not address general environmental education programs, such as
those in some schools, targeted specifically at children, that attempt to
produce changes in the long run by changing children's basic environmental understanding so they will
believe differently as adults. The long-term effects of such education are very
difficult to measure, but we believe that
these effects can be significant, and
we return to this theme at the end of the chapter.)
We find that, as
with health promotion, education is helpful but not sufficient for promoting the desired behaviors. We look first at educational
efforts that try to change fairly specific
environmental attitudes and beliefs and then at efforts that offer information
about how to action pro-environmental
attitudes. We see that education can
change attitudes and beliefs, but that many
barriers, both within individuals and in their social and economic environments, can keep pro-environmental
attitudes from being expressed in action. Some
internal barriers can be overcome with informational programs, but only if the programs are carefully designed to take advantage of psychological principles
of communication. The chapter presents those principles
and some illustrative examples of successful and unsuccessful information
programs. But even the best
educational programs cannot overcome external barriers to action, such as financial expense or serious inconvenience. The chapter details what
environmental education can and cannot
accomplish, and tells what must be
done to take the educational strategy as far as it can go. In later
chapters, we show how even greater success
can be achieved by combining education with other approaches.
EDUCATION
TO CHANGE ENVIRONMENTAL ATTITUDES AND
BELIEFS
Education can change attitudes and specific
environmental beliefs, but it cannot quickly or easily change ethics or values. Furthermore, education
is not likely to work
if it promotes attitudes that clash with people's basic ethics or values. Educators like
Carson know this. If an educator
tells people that in order to have a clean
environment they must sacrifice financial security, fresh food, or time
with their families, people who value those things highly will reject the educator's message. But if the message is that
environmental quality does not require people to reorder their basic values, it will go down easier.
Carson's message can work partly
because she explains how giving up
pesticides does not mean giving up fresh food. It is not necessary to
choose between environmental values and
fresh produce, because one can have
both by rejecting pesticides in favor of biological controls. (Of course, major shifts in Western
values may also be needed to
permanently solve environmental problems,
as we discussed in Chapter 3.)
Changing environmental attitudes can make a
difference. It is no
coincidence that the increased awareness and concern about environmental problems in U.S. public opinion beginning in the 1960s
was followed
by a burst of new legislation in the 1970s. And many scholars and writers believe that this
shift in opinion was strongly
influenced by Carson's Silent Spring. Also, when the word first came out in the mid-1970s that the chlorofluorocarbon propellants used in aerosol cans might harm the earth's ozone layer, Americans quickly reduced their purchases
of the cans and the government instituted a ban (Morrisette, et al.,
1990). This could not have happened without
widespread public concern. People who strongly
favor environmental protection are more likely to join environmental movement organizations (Mitchell,
1979) and vote for environmental protection
in public referenda (Gill et al., 1986), so attitudes can lead to action. But environmental attitudes
are not always correlated with
behavior, and attitude change does
not always lead to behavioral change. These facts greatly limit what the
attitude-change strategy alone can accomplish.
Controlled studies show that educational
efforts to change
environmental attitudes and beliefs generally have little effect on behavior. The most
careful studies focus
on consumer behaviors—recycling, energy conservation,
and other things individuals can do on their own
to directly change how environmental resources are used. (Researchers have not conducted experiments on changing people's political attitudes and
beliefs—what Silent Spring tried to
do—probably because doing this as an
experiment poses serious ethical
questions.) The following examples are typical. They focus on energy conservation in the home, an important way of reducing environmental problems such
as air pollution and global warming and one on which there is considerable research.
In 1977, a year when natural gas
shortages caused some businesses and schools
to close down to preserve heating
fuel, state agencies in Virginia conducted
three-hour workshops in various communities to educate people about energy conservation in the home. The workshops,
which consisted of lectures, slide
shows, discussions, and demonstrations, were designed to convince people that they could save substantial amounts of energy in their homes and to
show them how. Scott Geller and his colleagues at the Virginia
Polytechnic Institute and State University evaluated the effects of the workshop approach with surveys and
follow-up visits to participants' homes to look
for behavioral change (Geller, 1981). The workshops were effective in
changing attitudes and beliefs, as measured
by before-and-after surveys. After the workshop,
participants expressed increased concern about the energy crisis, increased awareness that simple changes in the home can yield substantial
energy savings, and stronger
beliefs that they could do something
about the energy crisis and that they had not yet done enough to insulate their homes. The surveys also
revealed stronger expressed commitment to
change "residential lifestyle for energy conservation." But these attitudes, beliefs, and
commitments did not translate into behavioral change. Follow-up visits to participants' homes six weeks after the
workshop revealed that only one of
forty workshop participants had
lowered a water heater thermostat, as the workshop had recommended, and that the only two with insulated water
heaters (another workshop recommendation) had insulated them before the workshop. The only behavioral change was in installing
low-flow shower heads. Eight of
forty workshop participants had
installed them, compared with two of forty
nonparticipants in nearby homes. But this change was not produced by education alone. The workshop leaders
also gave participants water-flow restrictors
and explained how to use them. By doing this, they removed a barrier to
energy conservation—the effort of obtaining the flow restrictor people may have come to want as a result of the workshop.
Such barriers between attitudes and
behavior impede educational efforts, as we see throughout this chapter.
In sum, although the workshops changed
people's attitudes, beliefs, and
even their plans to act (at least for a while), education alone did not lead to any observable action.
A similar result was observed in a government
pilot program
conducted in 1977 in Denver, Colorado. The purpose of this program was to change people's attitudes about appliance purchases so that instead of trying to get the lowest price, they
would want the model with the lowest
"energy cost of ownership." This concept is that the true
cost of a household appliance such as a
refrigerator includes not only the purchase price but also the cost of
the energy used to operate it. The U.S.
Department of Energy believed that if
consumers developed energy-wise attitudes about appliance purchases,
they would begin to buy models that achieve
great energy savings, even if they cost
a bit more to buy. The program used paid radio, television, and
newspaper advertisements, as well as signs in appliance stores, using the
slogan, "Products That Save Energy Pay
for Themselves." In addition, displays were placed in shopping
malls and in bank lobbies to show how much
could be saved, carrying the message,
"Products That Save Energy Finance Themselves." The program, which ran for seven months, produced increased awareness and some
attitude change. At the end of the
project, people knew more about the
cost and expected savings from specific
conservation measures and were more likely to believe they could personally
help solve the energy problem. They also
expressed greater willingness to pay
10 to 15 percent more for energy-efficient appliances. But their actual behavior changed very little (Hutton,
1982).
FIGURE 4-2 Some Barriers to Making Major Energy-Conserving Home Improvements
Why did these efforts fail? One likely
explanation is the
ap between attitudes and behavior. There are many good reasons p actions that reflect their values and attitudes. Consider
the example of someone who wants
to cut his energy bills. He may not know
how much he can save in his particular
home by upgrading insulation or installing an energy-efficient furnace, may not
have the necessary money or credit, may not want to change a heating system that is functioning adequately, may want to
spend the money on something else, may not trust a local contractor to do the work, or may be unable
to act because, as a renter, he does
not have the right to alter the
building (see Figure 4-2). The more of these barriers that exist, the
less difference a strong attitude in favor
of saving energy will make in terms of behavior. Box 4-1 reports the results of a statewide survey of
Massachusetts households that demonstrates this point. It shows that attitudes predict simple, low-cost energy-conserving behaviors such as resetting
thermostats, but the more difficult or expensive the behavior, the weaker the relationship to energy
attitudes.
Barriers to action also prevent other kinds of environmentally responsible behavior. Raymond De
Young (1989) interviewed thirty-two participants and fifty-nine nonparticipants in a long-established community
recycling program in Ann Arbor, Michigan,
to try to understand their behavior. He found that the groups had about equally strong pro-recycling
attitudes. On a scale that
represented strong anti-recycling attitudes
as 1 and strong pro-recycling attitudes as 5, recyclers' responses averaged
4.13 on items such as "I like
it when stores carry recycled products" and "recycling is good because it helps reduce
imports." Non-recyclers'
attitudes were not significantly different at 4.02. What differentiated the groups was their beliefs about barriers to recycling, particularly
difficulty. The two groups were a full half-point apart on these items: "It's a big nuisance to keep
everything separated for
recycling," "A problem with recycling is finding a place to put the stuff," and
"I'm never exactly sure what
I'm supposed to do to recycle." Recyclers' opinions were about neutral at 3.14, but non-recyclers, who averaged 2.65, saw significant
difficulties with recycling.
In a similar vein, Georg Prester and his
colleagues (1987) examined the differences between participants and
nonparticipants in a local political debate about extending a high-speed railroad line in a
residential area of
Mannheim, Germany. People who became politically active in the controversy had
a slightly higher
level of general environmental awareness and were more likely to believe that the project
would decrease local environmental quality. However, two of the strongest determinants of political
involvement were knowledge about how to
participate and interest in politics. When
it came to action, political skills—interest and know-how—were more
important than environmental attitudes.
The studies of environmental attitudes and behavior indicate that although the right attitudes are
conduI cive to environmeniariction, th-ey-arcaily predictive of action-under certain conditions. Attitudes are more likely to lead to behavior when strong
barriers to action are removed. (A
recent study of recycling attitudes and
behavior suggests that attitudes have their strongest effects when external conditions—barriers or inducements—have moderate strength, and that both strong barriers and strong inducements limit
the effects of attitudes [Guagnano,
Stern, & Dietz, 1995].)
The conceptual framework of Table 4-2, which
is based on an analysis of numerous studies
of pro-environmental behavior, makes clearer
what the barriers are. The table shows a long causal chain of factors
influencing environmentally relevant behavior, which is at the bottom of the chain (level 1). Note that any variable at a higher level in the chain is able
to influence any variable at a lower level. For example, owning one's own home rather than renting (level 6
in the table) may affect one's attitudes toward energy efficiency (level 4). This is because, to a homeowner,
an energy-efficient furnace and
well-insulated attic mean more than just lowered utility expenses. Having an efficient, modern furnace and good insulation
may be an
important part of a homeowner's attitudes about taking good care of her home. For this reason
also, homeowners may
become more knowledgeable than renters
about how to install insulation (level 3), and more committed to making this kind of home improvement (level 2). Note that it is possible
for factors lower on
the chain in Table 4-2 to influence those can increase levels of attention and
commitment, as we
discuss in a later section.
higher up. For example, behavior (level 1) can change attitudes and knowledge (levels 4 and 3) through a process of learning from experience or a
psychological
process of justifying one's past efforts by adopting attitudes consistent with them—the phenomenon
of cognitive
dissonance reduction.
TAbel 4-1
The framework shown in Table 4-2 indicates
that there are two main types of barriers that can keep. people from acting on
pro-environmental attitudes.
First, the frameworll lips that any break
thechain between attitudes (level 4) and behavior (level 1), such as absence of appropriate knowledge (level 3) or of
attention or commitment (level 2), can keep *environmental
attitudes from generating action (see examples in the table). Such barriers
exist w.libin individuals, so they can be addressed with interventions aimed at
individuals. Information programs, —2 which we discuss in the next section, are
designed to remove knowledge barriers at level 3. Other programs
Second, the framework in Table 4-2 identifies
bar- riers that lie outside the individual. These external barriers, which
appear at levels 7 and 6—the individual's socioeconomic background, available
tech nology, social and political institutions, economic forces, and
inconvenience—precede attitudes in the causal chain and so can prevent
pro-environmental attitudes from forming. For example, opinion polls show a
weak but cioeconomic factors such as level of education (level consistent
relationship between so-7), and concern with the environment (level 4) (Hines,
Hungerford, and Tomera, 1987). External barriers can also inhibit the
expression of pro-environmental attitudes. Attitudes in favor of recycling
produce no action when recycling is too inconvenient, and attitudes favoring
energy conservation lead nowhere when action is costly, difficult, or blocked
by the rules of property ownership.
Tabel 4-2
As we mentioned in Chapter 3, external
barriers can also
impede the expression of values (level 5). The pro-environmental values of Indian Hindus and Chinese Taoists were not strong enough to
overcome the pressures
of poverty, tyranny, and competition for scarce resources (factors at level 7). As we noted in Chapter 3 and see again in this chapter, such
external factors
are very difficult to change at the individual level. In Chapters 5, 6, and 7,
we examine the effects on
individual behavior of interventions that alter some of the external economic and social forces
shaping people's
treatment of the environment.
To summarize: When can one expect attempts to
change attitudes and beliefs to induce pro-environmental behavior? The simple
answer is: When the barriers to action are low. In the case of consumer
behaviors, barriers are low for inexpensive actions that are ready at hand.
These include participating in well-designed, convenient recycling programs,
making simple and low-cost changes in household energy use, and the like. The
barriers are higher when...the actions are inconvenient, complex, or when they
have costs to the individual in terms of money, time, or opportunities
foregone. Note further that some political actions are relatively easy to take.
The most obvious one —voting— is the one where attitude-behavior relationships
are easiest to demonstrate. In contrast, joining organizations takes more time
and sometimes money, and becoming an environmental activist, which takes
considerable effort, requires much more than just a pro-environmental attitude.
What can educational efforts aimed at
attitude change
accomplish when the external barriers to action are high? In the short run, they can do very little by themselves. But interventions need not be restricted to attitude change. As a few of the
examples above have already shown, efforts to change attitudes and beliefs, combined with a lowering of the
external barriers
to action (for example, providing a flow-restricting shower head to install), have real
potential. We
discuss combination approaches to behavior change later in the book, especially in Chapter 7. In the short run, tile most promising role for education is to help overcome internal barriers to action,
particularly the barriers of
ignorance and misinformation. We turn now to this use of education.
Copas From:
Chapter 4
Gardner & Stern. (1996). Environmental Problems and Human Behavior. Boston: Allyn and
Bacon
Komentar
Posting Komentar