Education Intervention (5)- Summary
SUMMARY
AND CONCLUSION:
WHAT
CAN EDUCATION ACCOMPLISH?
We have discussed educational interventions aimed at promoting fairly specific pro-environmental attitudes and
beliefs among individuals and overcoming internal
barriers, such as lack of knowledge or commitment,
that keep them from acting on those attitudes. Education can make a difference
in people's behavior, but there are serious limits to what it can accomplish. The chapter supports the following general conclusions:
In the short term, educational approach • work only when the
main barriers to action ar interna
o the
individual. As we have seen, education live mainly with relatively simple, low-cost behaviors, such as depositing cans in curbside recycling bins or altering home thermostat settings. Such actions help, but they typically have smaller effects on the environmental problems they are meant to lessen than more
permanent actions such as purchasing an energy-efficient
vehicle or appliance (see Chapter 10). Information has also been effective in getting people to request home energy audits, an action that has the
potential to lead to larger and more
permanent energy savings and
environmental benefits by changing heating and cooling equipment.
Nevertheless, when protecting the
environment requires great effort or expense,
as it often does, there is no experimental evidence that education alone will do the job. Under such conditions,
behavior change requires interventions
to reduce the external barriers to action. We examine those interventions in the next chapters.
Education may have important indirect effects over the long term. Though
external barriers to individual action limit
the effectiveness of education in the
short run, education may have important positive, though indirect, effects in the long run. For
example, the block-leader approach
to recycling (discussed in tightening
the Links) had indirect beneficial effects by changing community norms. A longer-term and possibly more important indirect effect—one we
have not yet discussed in this
chapter—can occur when education
changes people's political behavior; this behavior, in turn, changes government policy so as to lower the external barriers to pro-environmental
behavior. The history of smoking
reduction illustrates this process.
Over the several decades since the health hazards of smoking became established and widely publicized, the
proportion of smokers in the United States has slowly decreased. During
that time, scientists, physicians, and other individuals who became convinced of the dangers became politically active
and brought pressure on governments
and other powerful actors to bring
down the barriers to behavior change and alter some of the incentives
that govern smoking. Since 1964, cigarette
advertising has been restricted,
tobacco taxes have been deliberately raised,
no-smoking rules have been applied in airplanes and many public buildings, life insurance companies have made smokers pay more than nonsmokers
for coverage, and employers have
implemented antismoking programs.
These changes are fair because governments,
employers, and insurance companies incur higher costs for smokers than for
nonsmokers. At the same time, these changes have made it easier for individuals to act on antismoking attitudes.
People who intend to stop smoking find
more justifications and social
support, and nonsmokers find it easier to speak their minds to smokers.
Some of these changes, of course, even
influence people whose attitudes are not antismoking.
By a similar process, changes in environmental attitudes may come to affect behavior over the long term.
A generation of voters and environmental activists, influenced by the writings of Rachel Carson (1962), Paul Ehrlich (1968), Barry Commoner (1970), and other scientist-educators, has pressed
government agencies, corporations,
and other important actors to
implement new policies on air and water pollution, energy development,
and land use, and thus change the way they treat environmental resources.
Some of these policies also remove barriers to individuals' acting on their own pro-environmental attitudes, and thus change individual behavior. For example, they have helped bring more energy-efficient automobiles and appliances to market, so
that environmentally conscious consumers can buy them. If education about environmental problems has been
indirectly responsible for these
advances in environmental policy
over the last few decades, this would be a highly significant
accomplishment. Although it is difficult to
conclusively demonstrate the causal role of education over such long
time periods, improved public awareness and
understanding are among the most plausible causes of the policy changes.
This sort of long-term effect of attitude
change provides a key rationale for
environmental education programs in the schools.
Education is only
likely to induce behavior that is compatible with people's deeper values. As
we note above, environmental values and ethical beliefs are broader and more deeply rooted than environmental
attitudes or the specific beliefs addressed in this chapter. They are also more difficult to change.
Therefore, educational efforts aimed
at attitude change are unlikely to
succeed if they go against people's ethics and values. An example may be the repeated efforts of a coalition of nuclear energy industries through the 1980s
and early 1990s to change public attitudes toward nuclear power with
multimillion dollar advertising campaigns
emphasizing the benefits of the technology.
After well over a decade, public opinion is even more strongly opposed
than before. The bases for public opposition
to nuclear power are discussed in detail in Chapter 9.
Educational programs are more effective when they are designed
according to psychological principles of communication and also directly address the
links between attitudes and behavior. As the
chapter repeatedly shows, making information
available is not the same as getting it used. Even when people
are being asked to act according to their attitudes, and so are predisposed to use information, it is
essential to make special efforts to get their attention, use infor mation sources the audience trusts, and involve the
recipients of the information in the
effort. It may also be necessary to remind people that their pro-environmental attitudes apply to the situation at hand, and to tell them what to do to enact the attitudes. These things need to be done in different ways for
different behaviors and audiences, and the chapter illustrates a number of useful tools for the purpose.
Education
works best when combined with other strategies of intervention. We have seen how external barriers
such as cost and difficulty keep educational programs
from reaching their goals. We have also seen that programs work best
when they do more than just educate. For
example, recall that when an energy conservation program provided
water-flow restrictors along with information
on how to use them and on what they could save, it achieved its only
behavioral success (Geller, 1981). These
observations support a more general
conclusion, that education and other strategies
can act in synergy: The effects of both together are greater than one would expect from their separate
effects.
The general point has been demonstrated by decades
of research on health promotion, showing that educational
campaigns are not enough to change individuals' smoking, drinking,
dietary, and exercise behavior without
supplementary efforts. However, education plus other changes can make an
important difference over time. In the words
of one review of the literature
(Green, Wilson, and Lovato, 1986):
... [I-Health
promotion has been occurring and health practices have been changing. . . . The
changes have been
more notable since the advent of official policies supporting nonsmoking with more than information alone. . . . Organizational changes, such as
smoking restrictions on airplanes,
restaurants, and other public places
have helped. Economic supports, such as excise taxes on tobacco and
alcohol, insurance incentives for driver
training, not smoking, and blood pressure control, have helped. Environmental supports for behavior conducive to health, such as regulations on
marketing food products as healthful and availability of fitness facilities in worksites and public parks, have
helped. The combination of these
supports with health education appears to have made a substantial dent
in social norms of health-related behavior
(p. 513).
Copas From:
Chapter 4
Gardner & Stern. (1996). Environmental Problems and Human Behavior. Boston: Allyn and Bacon
Komentar
Posting Komentar