In-group variability and motivation to reduce subjective uncertainty
In-group
variability and motivation to reduce subjective uncertainty.
Jetten, Jolanda; Hogg,
Michael A.; Mullin, Barbara-Ann. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and
Practice4.2 (Jun 2000): 184-198.
Abstrak
Building on
the subjective uncertainty reduction model of social identity processes (M. A.
Hogg, in press-b; M. A. Hogg & B.-A. Mullin, 1999), it was hypothesized
that homogeneous groups would be better than heterogeneous groups at reducing
uncertainty and therefore that people would show more intergroup
differentiation and turn more to a homogeneous in-group under conditions of
uncertainty rather than certainty. Two minimal-group studies were conducted to
investigate this idea. As predicted, when groups were homogeneous, more intergroup
differentiation was observed under low certainty than under high certainty
(Study 1). Study 2 provided evidence that group members turn actively to their
group to reduce low certainty when the group is homogeneous. Low certainty led
group members to request more feedback about other group members' responses,
and they were more likely to change their initial responses when the in-group
was perceived as homogeneous. These findings confirm predictions derived from
the uncertainty model proposing that uncertainty reduction is a motivation for
group behavior. These findings also highlight the importance of social
structural factors such as perceived group variability in the uncertainty
reduction process.
The importance of motivations to
reduce uncertainty when interacting or communicating with, for instance,
strangers (Berger, 1987), in intercultural encounters (Gudykunst & Hammer,
1988), or when establishing a romantic relationship (Parks & Adelman, 1983)
is generally acknowledged in interpersonal relations research. It is assumed
that processes relating to understanding the other person and understanding the
self are directed at reducing uncertainty and that uncertainty reduction
increases perceptions of attractiveness of the other person (Berger, 1987;
Berger & Calabrese, 1975). More generally, there is a cluster of human
motivations to reduce subjective uncertainty, and to meaningfully structure the
social field, that social psychologists (stretching at least as far back as
Bartlett, 1932) consider to be critical and fundamental to human existence (see
Banaji & Prentice, 1994; Baumgardner, 1990; J. D. Campbell, 1990;
Kruglanski, 1989; Sedikides & Strube, 1995; Sorrentino, Holmes, Hanna,
& Sharp, 1995; Sorrentino & Short, 1986; Stevens & Fiske, 1995; Swann
& Schroeder, 1995).
In line with recent theorizing based
on group process theories such as social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner,
1986), the present research extends to the intergroup domain the idea that
people are motived by uncertainty reduction. Hogg and his colleagues (Hogg, in
press-a, in press-b; Hogg & Abrams, 1993; Hogg & Mullin, 1999) recently
proposed that uncertainty reduction is a fundamental motivation for
self-categorization as a group member and an important drive for individuals to
join groups or turn to groups. A number of studies provide support for this
uncertainty reduction model of social identity processes (e.g., Grieve &
Hogg, 1999; Hogg & Grieve, 1999; Hogg & Mullin, 1998; Mullin &
Hogg, 1998, 1999). The aim of the present research was to develop this analysis
by focusing on social structural properties of intergroup contexts that may
make some groups better than others at satisfying the uncertainty reduction
motive. Specifically, when people are uncertain they are more likely to turn to
groups that they believe are better equipped to reduce uncertainty. We propose
that perceived group variability is critical to this process because group
variability consists of information about the extent to which groups can
provide clearly focused and consensual prototypes that are best suited to
uncertainty reduction through self-categorization and social identification.
Social
Identity and Self-Categorization
Social categorization is a fundamental
social–cognitive process that structures the world around us in meaningful ways
(e.g., Hogg & Abrams, 1988; Medin, 1989; Rosch, 1978; Tajfel, 1981; Tajfel
& Wilkes, 1963; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987), and
social categorization is the foundation stone of intergroup behavior (e.g.,
group identification, intergroup discrimination, stereotyping). From social
cognition theories, intergroup behavior is explained with reference to the
limited cognitive capacity of human beings that causes biased information
processing and reliance on categories instead of individual information (Fiske
& Neuberg, 1990; Fiske & Taylor, 1984; Macrae, Milne, &
Bodenhausen, 1994). While fully acknowledging the critical role of social
categorization in structuring the social world and one's place within it,
social identity researchers have traditionally placed a greater emphasis on how
the process is guided by self-evaluative motives (Tajfel & Turner, 1986).
Because evaluative self-definition is contingent on category membership,
individuals and groups strive for evaluatively positive social identity and
intergroup distinctiveness and thus, ultimately, positive self-esteem. Much
traditional social identity research on intergroup relations has mainly focused
on the social comparison and enhancement aspects of the theory, with relatively
little attention given to the categorization process itself (see Hogg &
Mullin, 1999).
This emphasis is rebalanced by
self-categorization theory (Turner, 1985; Turner et al., 1987), which can be
viewed as an extension of social identity theory. Self-categorization theory
elaborates the operation of the categorization process as the generative
mechanism for social identification and group behavior. On the basis of
considerations such as metacontrast, normative fit, and comparative fit,
particular social categorizations become contextually salient (see Oakes, 1987;
Oakes, Haslam, & Turner, 1994), so that cognition, perception, affect, and
behavior are framed by well-differentiated in-group and out-group prototypes.
Self-categorization transforms the self so that it is governed by an in-group
prototype that describes and prescribes behavior and thus produces typical
group and intergroup behaviors (e.g., stereotyping, cohesion, ethnocentrism,
in-group bias, normative behavior). This more cognitive emphasis in
self-categorization theory implicitly refocuses attention on motivations
associated with the categorization process to processes such as perceptual
simplification, structural clarification, maximization of meaning, and uncertainty
reduction.
Uncertainty
Reduction Model
The uncertainty reduction model (Hogg,
in press-a, in press-b; Hogg & Abrams, 1993; Hogg & Mullin, 1999)
suggests that the underlying motive for self-categorization is the reduction of
subjective uncertainty. This argument grew out of the general observation that
social identity research did not lend much support to the role of self-esteem
as the fundamental drive for intergroup comparisons (Hogg & Abrams, 1990;
Long & Spears, 1997; Rubin & Hewstone, 1998). Hogg (in press-a, in
press-b; see also Hogg & Abrams, 1993; Hogg & Mullin, 1999) has argued
that one reason for these disappointing results with respect to the self-esteem
hypothesis might be that other motivations may operate that are fundamental to social
identity processes. According to the uncertainty reduction model, subjective
uncertainty reduction is just such an alternative motivation, and it is a
motivation that is very well satisfied by the process of self-categorization
that generates social identity phenomena. Although group-derived self-esteem is
still seen as an important group motivation, it is argued that uncertainty
reduction is at least as important and in some contexts may be more important.
Because human survival rests
substantially on being able to construct a meaningfully predictable world,
uncertainty about one's attitudes, beliefs, feelings, and perceptions, as well
as about oneself and other people, is generally an aversive state associated
with feelings ranging from unease to fear (e.g., Lopes, 1987). It is predicted
from the uncertainty reduction model that people strive to minimize or reduce
uncertainty by means of self-categorization and assimilation of the self to the
in-group prototype. Category prototypes of self-inclusive categories prescribe
behavior, locate us in the social world, regulate our expectations and
perceptions of self and others, and furnish consensual validation for who we
are, what we believe, and what we do. Uncertainty is produced by contextual
factors that challenge people's certainty about their cognitions, attitudes,
perceptions, feelings, and behaviors. However, it is predicted that not all
uncertainty, but only uncertainty about subjectively important issues that have
implications for the self-concept, will motivate group identification.
Furthermore, in a given context some groups are more relevant than others for
uncertainty reduction. This is largely dependent on the extent to which the
prototypical properties of the group are relevant to the dimension of uncertainty
(Hogg & Mullin, 1998).
Previous research has already provided
evidence that people identify with groups and define themselves as group
members to reduce subjective uncertainty. Hogg and Grieve (1999) conducted a
traditional minimal-group experiment in which participants were randomly
categorized into X group, Y group, or were not categorized. It was assumed that
categorization under these normal minimal-group conditions embodied relatively
high uncertainty. Hogg and Grieve created low uncertainty by giving practice
trials on point allocation matrixes (matrixes were also used to measure
intergroup bias). They found that intergroup bias was significant only among
participants categorized under uncertainty. Grieve and Hogg (1999, Experiment
1) replicated this finding in a more extensive experiment also containing
self-report measures of group identification. In this study, group
identification was higher, and intergroup bias occurred only among participants
who were categorized under uncertainty. A similar result was obtained in a
second experiment (Grieve & Hogg, 1999, Experiment 2). Again, it was found
that only participants who were categorized under uncertainty identified with
the category and exhibited group behavior. Mullin and Hogg (1998), in another
minimal-group experiment, disentangled situational uncertainty from task
uncertainty and examined their independent effects on intergroup
discrimination. Situational uncertainty was defined as uncertainty arising from
the relationship between the self and other people in the social setting. It
was manipulated by selecting participants who had no experience as a
participant in psychological research (high situational uncertainty) versus
participants who had participated at least five times in previous research (low
situational uncertainty). As in Hogg and Grieve's research, task uncertainty
was manipulated by procedural practice on the allocation task. In the low task
certainty condition, participants were given six practice trials and were told
to complete as many trials as they needed to make sure they felt completely
certain about the task. For both dimensions of uncertainty, significant group
identification and in-group bias were found only when participants were
categorized under uncertainty. Finally, Hogg and Mullin (1998) reported a set
of three minimal-group experiments in which they provided some evidence that
category relevance is important. Participants who were categorized under
uncertainty about something important identified significantly more strongly
with the category if the category was self-conceptually relevant to the
dimension of uncertainty.
Together, these studies provide
support for the basic tenets of the uncertainty reduction model (see Hogg &
Mullin, 1999, for an overview). The last set of studies (Hogg & Mullin,
1998) also suggests that properties of the social category, in this case its
relevance to the dimension of uncertainty, may influence whether people
self-categorize. It is this idea that we took up in the present research. What
types ofgroups are contextually appropriate to reduce uncertainty? Why do we
seek out groups, join groups, and self-define ourselves as members of
particular groups and not others when we are uncertain?
Group
Variability and Uncertainty Reduction
According to social comparison theory,
people generally seek out similar other individuals to validate their beliefs
(e.g., Festinger, 1954a, 1954b; Kruglanski & Mayseless, 1990; Schachter,
1959; Suls & Wills, 1991). This theory rests on the assumption that people
are driven to know that their opinions are correct and to know what they can
and cannot do. Only when physical reality checks are not possible do people
make social comparisons with others, and then with others who are similar to
them on relevant dimensions. Festinger was quite explicit that such comparisons
were interpersonal. In his historical commentary, Wheeler (1991) stated that
“comparison theory emphasized individuals using others to fulfill their own
need to know. ... The shift ... was clearly toward an individualistic focus”
(p. 3), and the idea that people construct a group norm against which to
compare themselves was not elaborated—the group and intergroup aspect of social
comparison was not explored (see Hogg, in press-a). Although self-evaluation,
possibly uncertainty reduction, is a motive for interpersonal social
comparison, subsequent research on social comparison focused much more on the
self-enhancement motive (see Wills & Suls, 1991). Contemporary social
comparison research is thus largely about people making interpersonal
comparisons for self-enhancement reasons.
The uncertainty reduction model
explains how people find certainty and validate their perceptions, attitudes,
feelings, and behaviors through identification with an in-group (see also
McGarty, Turner, Oakes, & Haslam, 1993; Tajfel, 1978). However, as Hogg and
Mullin's (1998) studies indicated, not all in-groups are functionally
equivalent in reducing subjective uncertainty, and the ability of a group to
reduce uncertainty may be affected by its social structural characteristics.
Some attention has been paid to social structural characteristics of the
in-group in research conducted by McGarty et al. (1993; Experiments 1 and 2),
who investigated uncertainty reduction when the reference group was similar or
dissimilar to the self. However, in this research no evidence was found that confidence
about responses on a perceptual task was more influenced by feedback from a
similar reference group (students) than by feedback from a dissimilar reference
group (a group of visually impaired people). The focus of the present research
is slightly different in that the effect of group variability information on
the effectiveness of turning to a group to reduce uncertainty is examined.
Groups are homogeneous when there is
marked similarity among members and heterogeneous when there are substantial
individual differences within the group (Simon, 1992; Simon & Brown, 1987).
The perception of group variability comprises information about similarity
among group members and the “groupness,” or entitativity, of a group (D. T.
Campbell, 1958). For instance, research has demonstrated that in-group
homogeneity increases the social distinctiveness and social identity of a group
and that in-group homogeneity is positively related to in-group favoritism
(Hogg, 1992; Simon, 1992; Simon, Kulla, & Zobel, 1995; Vanbeselaere, 1991).
In line with previous research, it is predicted that perceived agreement and
similarity among in-group members (Wilder, 1981, 1986), degree of in-group
normative consensuality (Grieve & Hogg, 1999), perceived group cohesion
(Hogg, 1992), perceived groupness or entitativity (D. T. Campbell, 1958;
Sherman, Hamilton, & Lewis, 1999; Yzerbyt, Rogier, & Fiske, 1998), and
perceived intergroup distinctiveness (Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, 1997,
1998) would be higher in homogeneous groups than in heterogeneous groups.
Homogeneous groups thus provide more
distinctive, consensual, and clearly defined prototypes than heterogeneous
groups. We predict that homogeneous groups are therefore particularly well
suited to uncertainty reduction through self-categorization. Group
heterogeneity represents diversity and disagreement among members, there is
considerable variation within the group, the prototype of the group is less
clearly defined, and it is therefore less clear what the group as a whole
stands for (McGarty et al., 1993). Self-categorization–based assimilation of
self to such a prototype would do little to resolve uncertainty. It is even
possible that extreme heterogeneity undermines feelings of groupness altogether
and that the group is better described as an aggregate of individuals. We
predicted that people would be more likelyto self-define with and turn to
homogeneous groups under conditions of low certainty than under high certainty.
Certainty versus uncertainty should not affect heterogeneous groups
differently, because group heterogeneity limits the effective and functional
reduction of uncertainty.
Study
1
We examined the interactive effect of
uncertainty reduction and perceived group variability in a minimal-group
setting. We manipulated group variability by providing participants with a
homogeneous or heterogeneous distribution of the in-group. Uncertainty was
manipulated to be low certainty about the correctness of social attitudes. We
examined our prediction—that attempts to reduce low certainty affect the extent
to which group members categorize and identify with their group—by using degree
of intergroup differentiation as the main dependent measure. Intergroup
differentiation in the present research was used as a measure of group identification
and group processes and was measured in terms of the difference between
in-group and out-group evaluations on traits stereotypical for these groups.
Other important dependent measures were social identification and the degree to
which group members turn to their own group to validate their attitudes in
order to reduce subjective uncertainty. We predicted that homogeneous groups
are better suited to, and more effective at, reducing high versus low
subjective uncertainty and that uncertainty would have no effect when the
in-group is heterogeneous.
Method
Design and participants
This experiment had a 2 (certainty:
low vs. high) × 2 (group variability: homogeneous vs. heterogeneous) factorial
design, with random allocation of participants to conditions. Students (36
women and 20 men, distributed evenly across conditions) at a large Australian
university participated in partial fulfillment of course requirements.
Procedure
Participants arrived at the laboratory
in groups varying in size from 10 to 20 and sat at individual tables on which
answer booklets had been placed. The female experimenter introduced the study
as an investigation into “attitudes and perception” and informed participants
that the study involved three tasks.
The first task, taken from Mullin and
Hogg (1999), had participants indicate how personally important they felt each
of 10 “serious and important issues” was to them (e.g., having a group of good
friends, being physically fit and healthy). After completing this task,
participants in the high-certainty condition were informed that
The next two tasks are related to
these important attitudes and beliefs. Please note that there are no
objectively right or wrong answers to these questions. There may be some
variation in people's attitudes and beliefs regarding such important
issues—that is OK. Your own attitudes and beliefs are just as valid as anyone
else's.
Participants
in the low-certainty condition were told:
The next two tasks are related to
these important attitudes and beliefs. Previous research has shown, however,
that some attitudes and beliefs are actually more valid than others because
they are grounded in factual information. So, some people actually have more
correct views on these issues than other people. [ 1 ]
Next, participants completed
manipulation checks for the certainty manipulation (see Dependent variables
section, below) and continued with the second task, for which they were categorized
into two groups on the basis of a visual perception task. Participants were led
to believe that previous research had shown that attitudes and perception are
related and that it is useful to distinguish between two kinds of perceivers:
detailed and global (Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, 1996). To measure their
own perceptual style participants were given a “dot estimation task” in which
they were presented for 5 s with seven separate overhead sheets filled with
dots. They were asked to estimate the number of dots (which ranged from 40 to
100), after which the experimenter asked them to calculate the total scores for
these seven estimations. The experimenter inspected each participant's score
and selected one of two booklets to hand out. The booklet contained information
about the participant's perceptual style. All participants were categorized as
detailed perceivers. [ 2 ]
Hereafter participants were informed
that previous studies had demonstrated that there are variations in the scores
within the group of detailed perceivers. It was explained that it is possible
to locate the score of detailed perceivers on a continuum that ranges from
moderate in detailed perceiving (0) to very high in detailed perceiving (100).
The booklet contained a frequency distribution of scores of detailed perceivers
who had participated in a previous study. In all conditions, the in-group mean
was 50. The intragroup range (i.e., the difference between the scores of the
least and most detailed perceivers) was 30 in the homogeneous in-group
condition and 70 in the heterogeneous in-group condition.
Dependent
variables
All ratings were made on 9-point
scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much). Certainty was checked with
three items taken from Mullin and Hogg (1999) that measured how confident and
certain participants felt about their responses on the attitude–belief scale
and how confident they felt as a participant in this experiment (α = .86). Two
items checked whether the in-group was seen as more variable in the
heterogeneous condition compared to the homogeneous condition (“According to
the histogram detailed perceivers are very different from each other in the way
they perceive” and “According to the histogram detailed perceivers are very
similar in the way they perceive”). We averaged these two items after we
recoded the first item (α = .91). Higher means indicate a higher perception of
in-group homogeneity.
To assess the extent to which group
members turn to their own group for attitude validation, participants were told
that they would receive samples of other detailed perceivers' responses to the
attitude–belief scale after the final task. Two questions followed: “How much
would you like to find out the attitudes/beliefs of the other detailed
perceivers in your group?” and “How much would you like to discuss these
attitudes/beliefs with the other detailed perceivers in your group?” (α = .93).
Social identification was then measured with three items adapted from Jetten et
al. (1996): “I identify with detailed perceivers,” “I see myself as part of the
group of detailed perceivers,” and “I feel strong ties with the group detailed
perceivers” (α = .87).
In-group and out-group evaluations
were assessed with semantic differentials. On the basis of a pilot study, traits
were selected that were stereotypical for detailed and global perceivers.
Participants had to indicate on a 9-point scale where they judged the average
in-group member and the average out-group member to be. One endpoint always
represented stereotypical traits for detailed perceivers (accurate, profound,
rigid, very organized), and the other endpoint represented stereotypical traits
for global perceivers (slovenly, superficial, flexible, not at all organized).
Analysis of the difference scores (in-group evaluation minus out-group
evaluation) revealed that the reliability of the scale was acceptable (α = .78)
after deleting the in-group negative and the out-group positive trait “rigid
vs. flexible.” The difference scores between in-group and out-group evaluations
were averaged, with higher scores indicating higher positive intergroup
differentiation.
Results
Manipulation
checks
We conducted 2 (certainty) × 2 (group
variability) analyses of variance (ANOVAs) on manipulation checks, which
revealed only the expected main effect of certainty on the certainty check,
F(1, 52) = 110.70, p < .001. Participants were less certain in the low-certainty
condition (M = 5.84) than in the high-certainty condition (M = 7.83). The
expected main effect of group variability (and no other effects) was found on
the variability check, F(1, 52) = 130.40, p < .001. Participants in the
heterogeneous condition perceived the group as less homogeneous (M = 2.64) than
participants in the homogeneous condition (M = 7.73). No other effects were
significant on these measures.
Attitude
validation and group identification
A two-way ANOVA on the attitude
validation measure revealed a main effect of certainty, F(1, 52) = 141.47, p
< .001. Participants in the low-certainty conditions turned more to their
group for attitude validation (M=7.66) than did participants in the
high-certainty condition (M = 3.86). The interaction between group variability
and certainty was not significant (F < 1). On the identification scale there
was only a significant main effect of certainty, F(1, 52) = 15.77, p < .001.
Identification was higher for participants in the low-certainty condition (M =
5.71) than in the high-certainty condition (M = 4.27). Moreover, although
overall group identification was not significantly different from the midpoint
of the scale (M = 4.99, t < 1), identification was significantly higher than
the midpoint of the scale for participants in the low-certainty condition,
t(27) = 3.91, p < .001, and significantly lower than the midpoint of the
scale for those in the high-certainty condition, t(27) = 2.33, p < .05.
Intergroup
differentiation
A two-way ANOVA on the scale measuring
positive intergroup differentiation revealed an interaction between certainty
and group variability, F(1, 52) = 6.93, p < .01. As predicted, low-certainty
participants whose in-group was homogeneous engaged in greater positive differentiation
of the in-group from the out-group than did other participants (see Table 1).
Simple main effects analysis revealed, as predicted, that greater
differentiation occurred in the homogeneous–low-certainty condition than in the
homogeneous–high-certainty condition, F(1, 53) = 8.38, p < .01. There was
also a tendency for differentiation to be greater in the
homogeneous–low-certainty condition than in the heterogeneous–low-certainty
condition, F(1, 53) = 2.73, p = .11. Compared to the homogeneous–high-certainty
condition, differentiation was greater in the heterogeneous–high-certainty
condition, F(1, 53) = 4.14, p < .05.
Enlarge
this Image.
Study 1: Intergroup Differentiation
(In-Group–Out-Group Evaluative Difference) on In-Group–Favoring Dimensions as a
Function of Certainty and Group Variability
Discussion
The results of Study 1 replicate the
findings of earlier studies that subjective uncertainty increases
identification with an available social category and that participants who
experience low certainty turn to the in-group to reduce this uncertainty (e.g.,
Grieve & Hogg, 1999; Hogg & Grieve, 1999; see Hogg & Mullin, 1999).
However, on these measures, this effect was not influenced by the extent to
which participants perceived their own group as homogeneous or heterogeneous.
Support for the role of group
variability in uncertainty reduction was found on the intergroup
differentiation measure. When groups were homogeneous, differentiation between
the in-group and out-group on dimensions where the in-group compared favorably
to the out-group was greater for participants who were uncertain compared to
those who were certain. Differences between in-group and out-group evaluations
did not differ as a function of uncertainty for participants who had a heterogeneous
in-group. It is interesting that homogeneity not only led to higher
differentiation under uncertainty but also to lower differentiation in the
high-certainty condition (compared to the heterogeneous conditions). This
suggests that for participants who already were high in certainty, not only
might the group be less important, but also group homogeneity might be
perceived as threatening. That is, imposed homogeneity might threaten
distinctiveness needs for group members who do not need to rely on the group
for uncertainty reduction (Brewer, 1991).
Study
2
One possible reason why in Study 1 no
effects were observed for group variability on the extent to which group
members turn to other group members to validate the correctness of their
attitudes to resolve uncertainty is that it may have been unclear how the group
could actually provide guidance in reducing low certainty. Although the group
was more clearly defined when it was homogeneous than when it was
heterogeneous, perhaps it was still not clear to participants what the group
stood for and how the group might effectively reduce or resolve low certainty.
Specifically, it is possible that there were differences in intention to reduce
low certainty as a function of group variability and certainty but that no
opportunity was available to actually receive information from a homogeneous
group under low versus high certainty. We would expect that group variability
would become more important if the group actually provided information that
could be attended to and could be influential. We felt it important to demonstrate
that uncertainty reduction occurs through a process of actively using
group-based information and that effects might be more powerful if such an
opportunity is given compared to the more passive setting of Study 1. In Study
2 we tested whether people turn to groups to reduce uncertainty, by examining
to what extent group members are interested in other group members' responses
and to what extent the group exerts social influence on the decision-making
process. We also made changes to the nature of the uncertainty manipulation.
Decision certainty was manipulated, and participants were given the opportunity
to ask for information about other in-group members' decisions.
As in Study 1, we predicted that
in-group homogeneity in particular would allow group members to effectively
reduce low certainty. As in Study 1, the experimental setting involved minimal
groups. The main dependent variables were the extent to which participants were
interested in responses of other in-group members and the extent to which
feedback about other group members' responses would motivate participants to
change their own initial decisions.
Method
Design
and participants
The independent variables of certainty
(low vs. high) and group variability (homogeneous vs. heterogeneous) were
manipulated in a 2 × 2 factorial design. University of Amsterdam students (39
women and 37 men, distributed evenly over conditions) received 10 Dutch
guilders (approximately $5 U.S.) for their participation in the study.
Procedure
Participants were seated in separate
cubicles and completed the experimental tasks on personal computers. They
received brief instructions on how to work with the computer and then began the
experiment, which was introduced as an investigation of the way people make
decisions. The experiment consisted of three phases: the certainty
manipulation, followed by a categorization phase, and then the group
variability manipulation. The critical dependent variable was the extent to
which group members were interested in receiving information about decisions
made by other in-group members and the extent to which they would like to
change their initial decision after having had the chance to view other group
members' decisions.
We manipulated certainty by providing
participants with five personnel decision cases (adapted from Doosje, Ellemers,
& Spears, 1995; Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje, 1997). For each case
participants had to decide which of two options was the best solution to the
personnel problem. For instance, participants were told:
There is a vacancy at the
administration department for a new supervisor. Two candidates have been
short-listed for this position. Candidate 1 started 5 years ago as a secretary
at the administration department. A few years ago she was promoted and now she
is the assistant of the supervisor that is leaving. She knows exactly what is
going on in the department, but she has no management experience. Candidate 2
is currently supervisor of a production line. She would prefer a job in the
office but does not have experience with the administration department. Which
candidate is in your view best qualified for the job, 1 or 2?
Participants in the high-certainty
condition were told that they “had to keep in mind that there are no
objectively right or wrong decisions and that your decision is just as valid as
anyone else's.” Participants in the low-certainty condition were told that they
had to keep in mind that one of the
two decisions was clearly more valid than the other one. In other words, one
option is more correct than the other option. The decisions differ in their
effectiveness to solve the problem.
After participants had given their
answers to each of the five cases, the certainty manipulation checks were
administered (see Dependent measures section).
As in Study 1, the second part of the
experiment was designed to categorize participants. As before, they were told
that previous research had shown that there are two kinds of perceivers:
detailed and global. They completed the dot estimation task on the computer to
measure their own perceptual style. Again, all participants received feedback
that they were detailed perceivers. In the third part of the experiment
participants were provided with information (by means of histograms) about the distribution
of scores of detailed perceivers. Homogeneity and heterogeneity were
manipulated in the same way as before. After participants had studied the
in-group distribution histogram for 20 s, they were provided with a written
interpretation of the results (projected onto the computer screen together with
the histogram).
Dependent
measures
The manipulation checks for certainty
and group variability were similar to Study 1. We averaged the three items
checking for certainty and the two items checking for group variability (αs =
.80 and .88, respectively). Group identification was measured with the same
three items as in Study 1 (α = .85).
The main dependent measure consisted
of a task in which participants were given the opportunity to access other
group members' decisions on each of the five personnel decision cases. First,
participants were re-presented with the case and were asked if they were
interested in the decision of another group member. If they pressed the “yes”
key, the decision of an ostensibly randomly chosen other in-group member was
displayed on the screen. This feedback was randomized so that only in half of
the requests for other decisions did the other in-group member seem to have
made the same decision as the participant. For each case, participants were
given the option to ask for more decisions (pressing the “yes” key), up to a
maximum of five, or continuing with the next personnel case (pressing the “no”
key). The feedback was constructed so that on average 2 or 3 out of 5 other
in-group members would respond similarly to the participant. We computed a
6-point scale by counting the number of times participants had requested the
decision of other in-group members for each case. This scale ranged from 0 (no
decision of other group members required) to 5 (decisions of all group members
required). Scores were averaged across the five cases.
After being given the opportunity to
request the decisions of other in-group members, participants were asked if
they would like to change their initial decisions. If they pressed the “yes”
key, they were given the opportunity to type in their new decision. We computed
a scale by counting the number of times each participant changed his or her
initial decision. This scale ranged from 0 (no initial decisions changed) to 5
(all initial decisions changed). To assess the extent to which participants
would turn to an expert to reduce uncertainty, participants were asked to rate
the extent to which they would like to know the decision of organizational
psychologists for each of the cases (the scale ranged from 1 [not at all] to 9
[very much]). This question was asked twice: right after the manipulations of
group variability and certainty as part of the cover story (Time 1), and then
after the task in which participants could request the decisions of other
in-group members (Time 2).
Results
Manipulation
checks
A two-way ANOVA with the factors of
certainty and group variability on the certainty check revealed only a main
effect of certainty, F(1, 72) = 6.12, p < .05. Participants in the
high-certainty condition were more certain of their decisions (M = 6.92) than
participants in the low-certainty condition (M = 6.26). Analysis of the group
variability check revealed a main effect of group variability, F(1, 72) =
389.73, p < .001, and no other effects. The group was perceived as more
variable in the heterogeneous condition (M = 7.51) than in the homogeneous
condition (M = 2.23).
Group
identification
In contrast to Study 1, no significant
main or interaction effects were found on group identification. Overall, mean
group identification was 3.89, which is significantly lower than the midpoint
of the scale, t(75) = 4.96, p < .001.
Request
for in-group members' decisions
An ANOVA revealed a marginally
significant interaction between certainty and group variability on
participants' requests for in-group members' decisions, F(1, 72) = 3.68, p =
.06. As predicted, participants requested more in-group members' decisions in
the homogeneous–low-certainty condition than in other conditions (see Table 2).
Simple main effects analysis revealed that the request for information was
higher in the homogeneous–low-certainty condition compared to the
homogeneous–high-certainty condition, F(1, 72) = 3.81, p < .05.
Image.
Study 2: Requests for In-Group
Members' Decisions as a Function of Certainty and Group Variability
Change
of initial decisions
Few participants changed their initial
decisions at all, let alone more than once. Therefore, we created a dichotomous
variable differentiating between those who “did not change any initial
decisions” (0) and those who “changed initial decisions” (1). Analysis revealed
also a marginally significant interaction between certainty and group
variability, F(1, 72) = 2.95, p = .09. There was a tendency for participants to
change their initial decisions more in the homogeneous–low-certainty condition
and in the heterogeneous–high-certainty condition (see Table 3). Simple main
effects analysis revealed no significant differences.
Image.
Study 2: Desire to Change One's
Initial Decision on the Personnel Decision Task as a Function of Certainty and
Group Variability
Finally, we analyzed the extent to
which group members wished to know the decisions of organizational psychologists
for each of these cases with a three-way ANOVA with time as a repeated measure.
Means for Time 1, Time 2, and change over time are displayed in Table 4. This
analysis revealed a three-way interaction among group variability, certainty,
and time, F(1, 72) = 4.44, p < .05. Simple main effects analysis revealed
two significant effects. Participants in the homogeneous–low-certainty
condition became more interested in the decisions made by organizational
psychologists over time (M = .37) than participants in the
homogeneous–high-certainty condition (M = −.50), F(1, 72) = 4.56, p < .05.
Furthermore, the interest in these decisions was also higher over time in the
homogeneous–low-certainty condition than in the heterogeneous–low-certainty
condition (M = .05), F(1, 72) = 4.56, p < .05.
Image
Study 2: Interest in the Decisions
Made by Organizational Psychologists as a Function of Group Variability,
Certainty, and Time
Discussion
The focus of Study 2 was the extent to
which group members turn to other group members to reduce uncertainty, as a
function of subjective uncertainty and group variability. Against a background
of successfully manipulated independent variables, the results were generally
in line with the intergroup differentiation finding of Study 1. Although there
were no significant effects of group identification (see General Discussion
section), effects on other measures supported predictions about when
participants who were given the possibility to actively reduce their uncertainty
would do so. Results revealed that when the group was homogeneous, not only did
group members low in certainty request access to more decisions made by other
group members than those who were high in certainty, but there was also a
tendency for them to be more likely to change their own initial decision and to
increase their interest in decisions made by experts (organizational
psychologists).
As in Study 1, it should be noted that
information search, changing of initial decision, and interest over time in
decisions of organizational psychologists also were consistently higher in the
heterogeneous–high-certainty condition. As discussed more fully below, it is
possible that group members who were already certain found heterogeneity to
satisfy a distinctiveness need and thus were curious about decisions made by
fellow group members (Brewer, 1991, 1993a, 1993b).
General
Discussion
The results of two studies suggest
that perception of in-group variability by group members has implications for
the reduction of subjective uncertainty. Homogeneous groups were more relevant
and effective than heterogeneous groups as foci of social identity-contingent
uncertainty reduction. Participants differentiated between the in-group and
out-group on stereotypical dimensions more strongly, and thus showed more
positive intergroup differentiation, if they were subjectively low in certainty
and the in-group was relatively homogeneous (Study 1). In Study 2 we examined
the extent to which group members actively turn to other members to reduce low
certainty. Consistent with Study 1, participants requested more information
from fellow in-group members when they were subjectively low in certainty and
the group was homogeneous. In addition, there was evidence that in this
condition participants were more influenced by group members to change their
initial decisions than in other conditions. Manipulation checks confirmed that
the manipulation of uncertainty was successful in both studies. There is of
course the possibility that the manipulation may have had other effects. For
example, high-uncertainty participants might have also felt insecure and under
threat. Indeed, uncertainty is aversive, and this quite probably involves
feelings of insecurity and threat. The precise relationship among feeling
uncertain, feeling insecure, feeling threatened, and so forth needs to be
disentangled in future research.
It is reasonable to infer that people
turn to homogeneous groups to reduce uncertainty because their members are
prototypically similar, and such groups are more likely to furnish clearly
focused, consistent, and consensual information (Hogg & Mullin, 1999; see
also McGarty et al., 1993). Heterogeneous groups are less helpful for
uncertainty reduction because when coherence and unity within the group are
low, there is an attitudinal and behavioral dissensus, and the prototype is
less clearly defined, making it difficult to predict how the group might
actually resolve one's uncertainty.
In sum, our findings support the
notion that uncertainty motivates group behavior and that social structural
characteristics of the group such as perceived group variability (homogeneity
vs. heterogeneity) are important in this process. However, although our results
indicate that these group processes are stronger when groups are homogeneous
than when they are heterogeneous, there may be occasions when in-group
heterogeneity has an advantage in the uncertainty-reduction process. One such
occasion may be when group members do not identify very strongly with the
group. Under these conditions, the diversity within heterogeneous groups that
is usually considered a disadvantage may become an advantage. With greater
diversity comes an increased probability that people will always be able to
find at least some members who validate their own behavior. However, decreasing
identification is associated with a gradual shift from prototype-based social
attraction to interpersonal-relationship–based personal attraction (e.g., Hogg,
1992), and thus interpersonal comparison processes (e.g., Festinger, 1954a;
Schachter, 1959) may also be more prevalent when identification is low.
Heterogeneous groups may also be appropriate for reducing relatively low levels
of uncertainty or uncertainty about less self-conceptually critical matters.
Under these circumstances, homogeneous groups might be viewed negatively as
being overly controlling.
Another occasion when heterogeneity
may be better suited than homogeneity to uncertainty reduction is when
uncertainty is related to factual knowledge rather than to matters of opinion
or perspective. The diversity of heterogeneous groups increases the probability
that there is someone in the group with the correct information or relevant
expertise, whereas the uniformity of homogeneous groups increases the
consensuality of social support. It might be argued that Study 1 was mainly
concerned with judgmental (attitudinal, opinion, and value) uncertainty and
Study 2 with intellective uncertainty (i.e., a demonstrably correct solution
was possible). When uncertainty is experienced on judgmental dimensions in
which expert knowledge is not fundamental to reduce uncertainty, it can be
predicted that heterogeneous groups do not provide the best group structure to
reduce uncertainty. However, availability of expert knowledge would probably be
important and beneficial for participants who were asked to make personnel
decisions. The effect found in Study 2 that uncertainty led to more interest in
decisions made by organizational psychologists might point to that.
[ 3 ]
The present research can also be
related to Brewer's (1991, 1993a, 1993b) optimal distinctiveness theory. Brewer
has argued that people are motivated by opposing needs for assimilation and for
distinctiveness or uniqueness (Snyder & Fromkin, 1980) and that they strive
for an optimal balance between satisfaction of these needs. If assimilation
needs are overly satisfied people seek distinctiveness, and if distinctiveness
needs are overly satisfied they seek assimilation. Homogeneous groups satisfy
assimilation needs relatively more than distinctiveness needs, and
heterogeneous groups satisfy distinctiveness needs relatively more than
assimilation needs (Brewer & Miller, 1996). From an uncertainty
perspective, we argue that under conditions of high uncertainty people are
drawn to homogeneous groups because prototype clarity and consensuality satisfy
uncertainty. However, when uncertainty is low, people have little need to
identify with groups at all, or they may identify relatively weakly with
heterogeneous groups. One prediction is that people appear to strive for
relative distinctiveness or uniqueness when groups are heterogeneous. The
possibility exists that whether people strive for assimilation or
distinctiveness may be determined by their level of uncertainty motivation.
With increasing uncertainty people become increasingly motivated to assimilate
and less motivated to be distinct and unique. There is some support for this
idea from the high levels of intergroup differentiation in Study 1 and the high
decision-request rates and subsequent changes of initial decisions among
low-uncertainty participants in heterogeneous groups in Study 2. It is possible
that these effects were not so much the result of uncertainty reduction
processes as the effect of greater adherence to the group when distinctiveness
needs are preserved. As discussed before, homogeneous groups might even represent
a threat under low uncertainty because people's need to turn to groups is lower
when certainty is high. Group homogeneity might in that case be more
threatening to the identity than heterogeneity, because the group can be
perceived as overly controlling. Further research is required to investigate in
greater detail the interplay between distinctiveness needs resulting from group
variability feedback and uncertainty reduction motivations, in particular when
group members are relatively certain.
It was unexpected that no effects for
group variability or uncertainty were found on the group identification measure
in Study 2. This is not only inconsistent with previous uncertainty research
(e.g., Grieve & Hogg, 1999; Mullin & Hogg, 1998, 1999), but it also
weakens the key argument that group members categorize themselves more as group
members and identify more strongly with the group when they are uncertain.
However, as described in the Results section of Study 2, overall identification
with the detailed-perceivers group was weak and even significantly below the
midpoint of the scale. It might be that mentioning the availability of experts
(organizational psychologists) in the context of an intellective task (see
discussion above) made the experts a contextually more relevant comparison
group than the in-group consisting of other students and that this thus
depressed in-group identification. It was probably less clear to group members
how other detailed perceivers could reduce their task-specific uncertainty than
experts in this field. This argument is quite consistent with the notion that
people who are uncertain are more likely to identify with groups that are more
relevant rather than less relevant to uncertainty reduction (Hogg & Mullin,
1998; McGarty et al., 1993).
Our studies confirm basic tenets of
the uncertainty reduction model of social identity processes and provide
evidence for the importance of structural characteristics of the group that
might influence the extent to which group members turn to their group when
uncertain. The results of both studies show that group members used information
about in-group variability when they searched for a group to help them reduce
uncertainty. Generally, when certainty was low rather than high, people turned
more to homogeneous groups, presumably because homogeneity ensures a clear and
consensual prototype that is effective at reducing uncertainty. Group
variability is only one structural feature of groups that may influence
uncertainty reduction through group identification. Other characteristics of
groups, such as their size, status, power, or distinctiveness from other
groups, might also be taken into account when group members judge the
suitability of groups to reduce uncertainty. We might predict that minority
groups, high-status groups, powerful groups, and groups that are clearly
distinctive from other groups would be considered most capable of reducing or
resolving uncertainty. These are promising directions for future research on
subjective uncertainty reduction as a motivation for social identity processes.
References
Banaji, M.
R., & Prentice, D. A. (1994). Annual
Review of Psychology.
Bartlett, F.
C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press.
Baumgardner, A.
H. (1990). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Berger, C.
R., Rolotte, M. G., & Miller, G.
R. (Eds.) (1987). Interpersonal processes: New directions
in communication research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Berger, C.
R., & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). Human
Communication Research.
Brewer, M.
B. (1991). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.
Brewer, M.
B., Hogg, M.
A., & Abrams, D. (Eds.) (1993a). Group
motivation: Social psychological perspectives. London: Harvester
Wheatsheaf.
Brewer, M.
B. (1993b). Social Cognition.
Brewer, M.
B., & Miller, N. (1996). Intergroup
relations. Buckingham, England: Open University Press.
Campbell, D.
T. (1958). Behavioral Sciences. ...
Hogg, M.
A. (1992). The social psychology of group cohesiveness: From
attraction to social identity. Hemel Hempstead, England: Harvester
Wheatsheaf.
Hogg, M.
A., Suls, J., & Wheeler, L. (Eds.) (). Handbook
of social comparison: Theory and research. New York: Plenum.
Hogg, M.
A. (). European Review of Social Psychology.
Hogg, M.
A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social
identifications: A social psychology of intergroup relations and group
processes. New York: Routledge.
Hogg, M.
A., Abrams, D., Abrams, D., & Hogg, M.
A. (Eds.) (1990). Social identity theory: Constructive and
critical advances. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Hogg, M.
A., Abrams, D., Hogg, M.
A., & Abrams, D. (Eds.) (1993). Group
motivation. Hemel Hempstead, England: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Hogg, M.
A., & Grieve, P. (1999). Asian Journal
of Social Psychology.
Hogg, M.
A., & Mullin, B.-A. (1998). Reducing
subjective uncertainty by group identification: The role of group
relevance.
Hogg, M.
A., Mullin, B.-A., Abrams, D., & Hogg, M.
A. (Eds.) (1999). Social identity and social
cognition. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
...
Tajfel, H., & Tajfel, H. (Ed.) (1978). Differentiation
between social groups. London: Academic Press.
Tajfel, H., Turner, J.
C., & Giles, H. (Eds.) (1981). Intergroup
behaviour. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Tajfel, H., Turner, J.
C., Worchel, S., & Austin, W.
G. (Eds.) (1986). Psychology of intergroup
relations. Chicago: Nelson Hall.
Tajfel, H., & Wilkes, A.
L. (1963). British Journal of Psychology.
Komentar
Posting Komentar