Toward a Coherent Theory of Environmentally
Toward a Coherent Theory of
Environmentally
Significant Behavior
Paul C. Stern
National Research Council
Stern, P. C.
(2000). New environmental theories: toward a coherent theory of environmentally
significant behavior. Journal of social
issues, 56(3), 407-424.
Re-product From: web.stanford.edu/~kcarmel/CC_BehavChange_Course/readings/Stern_metareview_2000.pdf
http://web.stanford.edu/~kcarmel/CC_BehavChange_Course/readings/Stern_metareview_2000.pdf
ect.
This
article develops a conceptual framework for advancing theories of
environmentally significant individual behavior and reports on the attempts of
the author’s research group and others to develop such a theory. It discusses
definitions of environmentally significant behavior; classifies the behaviors
and their causes; assesses theories of environmentalism, focusing especially on
value-belief-norm theory; evaluates the relationship between environmental concern
and behavior; and summarizes evidence on the factors that determine environmentally
significant behaviors and that can effectively alter them. The article
concludes by presenting some major propositions supported by available research
and some principles for guiding future research and informing the design of
behavioral programs for environmental protection.
Recent
developments in theory and research give hope for building the understanding
needed to effectively alter human behaviors that contribute to environmental
problems. This article develops a conceptual framework for the theory of environmentally
significant individual behavior, reports on developments toward such a theory,
and addresses five issues critical to building a theory that can inform efforts
to promote proenvironmental behavior.
Defining Environmentally Significant Behavior
Environmentally
significant behavior can reasonably be defined by its impact: the extent to
which it changes the availability of materials or energy from the environment
or alters the structure and dynamics of ecosystems or the biosphere itself (see
Stern, 1997). Some behavior, such as clearing forest or disposing of household
waste, directly or proximally causes environmental change (Stern, Young, &
Druckman, 1992). Other behavior is environmentally significant indirectly, by
shaping the context in which choices are made that directly cause environmental
change (e.g., Rosa & Dietz, 1998; Vayda, 1988). For example, behaviors that
affect international development policies, commodity prices on world markets,
and national environmental and tax policies can have greater environmental
impact indirectly than behaviors that directly change the environment.
Through
human history, environmental impact has largely been a by-product of human
desires for physical comfort, mobility, relief from labor, enjoyment, power,
status, personal security, maintenance of tradition and family, and so forth, and
of the organizations and technologies humanity has created to meet these desires.
Only relatively recently has environmental protection become an important
consideration in human decision making. This development has given
environmentally significant behavior a second meaning. It can now be defined
from the actor’s standpoint as behavior that is undertaken with the intention
to change (normally, to benefit) the environment. This intent-oriented
definition is not the same as the impact-oriented one in two important ways: It
highlights environmental intent as an independent cause of behavior, and it
highlights the possibility that environmental intent may fail to result in
environmental impact. For example, many people in the United States believe
that avoiding the use of spray cans protects the ozone layer, even though
ozone-destroying substances have been banned from spray cans for two decades.
The possible discrepancy between environmental intent and environmental impact
raises important research questions about the nature and determinants of
people’s beliefs about the environmental significance of behaviors.
Both
definitions of environmentally significant behavior are important for research
but for different purposes. It is necessary to adopt an impact-oriented definition
to identify and target behaviors that can make a large difference to the environment
(Stern & Gardner, 1981a). This focus is critical for making research useful.
It is necessary to adopt an intent-oriented definition that focuses on people’s
beliefs, motives, and so forth in order to understand and change the target behaviors.
Types of Environmentally Significant Behavior
Much
early research on proenvironmental behavior presumed it to be a unitary,
undifferentiated class. More recently it has become clear that there are
several distinct types of environmentally significant behavior and that
different combination of causal factors determine the different types.
Environmental
Activism
Committed
environmental activism (e.g., active involvement in environmental organizations
and demonstrations) is a major focus of research on social movement
participation. This research provides detailed analysis of the “recruitment” process
through which individuals become activists (McAdam, McCarthy, & Zald, 1988).
Nonactivist Behaviors in the Public Sphere
Recently,
the social movement literature has pointed to nonactivists’ support of movement
objectives as another important class of behavior (Zald, 1992). Public opinion
researchers and political scientists sometimes examine such behavior, but
relatively little research has been done to classify the behaviors into
coherent subtypes. It seems reasonable as a first approximation to distinguish
between more active kinds of environmental citizenship (e.g., petitioning on
environmental issues, joining and contributing to environmental organizations)
and support or acceptance of public policies (e.g., stated approval of
environmental regulations, willingness to pay higher taxes for environmental
protection). My colleagues and I have found empirical support for
distinguishing these types from each other and from activism (Dietz, Stern, &
Guagnano, 1998; Stern, Dietz, Abel, Guagnano, & Kalof, 1999). Although
these behaviors affect the environment only indirectly, by influencing public
policies, the effects may be large, because public policies can change the
behaviors of many people and organizations at once. An important feature of
public-sphere behaviors, including activism, is that environmental concerns are
within awareness and may therefore be influential.
Private-Sphere Environmentalism
Consumer
researchers and psychologists have focused mainly on behaviors in the private
sphere: the purchase, use, and disposal of personal and household products that
have environmental impact. It is useful to subdivide these according to the type
of decision they involve: the purchase of major household goods and services that
are environmentally significant in their impact (e.g., automobiles, energy for the
home, recreational travel), the use and maintenance of environmentally important
goods (e.g., home heating and cooling systems), household waste dis-posal, and
“green” consumerism (purchasing
practices that consider the environmental impact of production processes, for
example, purchasing recycled products and organically grown foods). Making such
distinctions has revealed that some types of choice, such as infrequent
decisions to purchase automobiles and major household appliances, tend to have
much greater environmental impact than others, such as changes in the level of
use of the same equipment: the distinction between efficiency and curtailment
behaviors (Stern & Gardner, 1981a, 1981b).
Private-sphere
behaviors may also form coherent clusters empirically (e.g., Bratt, 1999a), and
different types of private-sphere behavior may have different determinants
(e.g., Black, Stern, & Elworth, 1985). Private-sphere behaviors are unlike public-sphere
environmentalism in that they have direct environmental consequences. The
environmental impact of any individual’s personal behavior, however, is small.
Such individual behaviors have environmentally significant impact only in the
aggregate, when many people independently do the same things.
Other
Environmentally Significant Behaviors
Individuals
may significantly affect the environment through other behaviors, such as
influencing the actions of organizations to which they belong. For example, engineers
may design manufactured products in more or less environmentally benign ways,
bankers and developers may use or ignore environmental criteria in their
decisions, and maintenance workers’ actions may reduce or increase the
pollution produced by manufacturing plants or commercial buildings. Such
behaviors can have great environmental impact because organizational actions
are the largest direct sources of many environmental problems (Stern &
Gardner, 1981a, 1981b; Stern, 2000). The determinants of individual behavior
within organizations are likely to be different from those of political or
household behaviors.
Evidence for Distinguishing Major Behavioral
Types
Research
my colleagues and I have conducted suggests that this distinction among
behavioral types is not only conceptually coherent but statistically reliable and
psychologically meaningful. For instance, a factor analysis of the behavioral items
in the environment module of the 1993 General Social Survey revealed a three-factor
solution (Dietz et al., 1998). One factor included four private-sector household
behaviors (e.g., buying organic produce, sorting household waste for recycling);
a second included two environmental citizenship behaviors (signing a petition
and belonging to an environmental group); and the third included three items
indicating willingness to make personal financial sacrifices for environmental
goals, which assess policy support. A different pattern of social-psychological
and socio-demographic predictors was associated with each of the behavioral types,
and even the two citizenship behaviors had quite different sets of predictors.
My
colleagues and I had similar results using data from a 1994 national
environmental survey (Stern et al., 1999). Factor analysis of 17 items
measuring self-reported behaviors and behavioral intentions again revealed
three factors: consumer behaviors (e.g., buying organic produce, avoiding
purchases from companies that harm the environment); environmental citizenship
(e.g., voting, writing to government officials); and policy support, expressed
as willingness to sacrifice economically to protect the environment (e.g., by
paying much higher taxes or prices). Self-reported participation in
environmental demonstrations and protests, presumably a measure of committed
activism, did not load on any of the above three factors. Each of these factors
was predicted by a different pattern of norms, beliefs, and values, and
activism had yet a different set of predictors.
The
Determinants of Environmentalism
Environmentalism
may be defined behaviorally as the propensity to take actions with
proenvironmental intent. Some theories treat environmentalism as a matter of
worldview. Perhaps the most prominent example in social psychology is the idea
that it flows from adopting a New Environmental (or Ecological) Paradigm,
within which human activity and a fragile biosphere are seen as inextricably interconnected
(Dunlap, Van Liere, Mertig, & Jones, this issue). Another worldview theory
explains environmentalism in terms of an egalitarian “cultural bias” or
“orienting disposition” (Dake, 1991; Douglas & Wildavsky, 1982; Steg
&Sievers, 2000). Recently, some researchers have begun to explore affective
influences on environmental concern and behavior, including sympathy for others
(Allen & Ferrand, 1999), “emotional affinity” toward nature (Kals,
Schumacher, & Montada, 1999), and empathy with wild animals (Schultz, this
issue).
Some
theories look to values as the basis of environmentalism. Inglehart (1990)
suggests that it is an expression of postmaterialist values of quality of life and
self-expression that emerge as a result of increasing affluence and security in
the developed countries. Some accounts emphasize religious values, arguing either
that certain Judaeo-Christian beliefs predispose adherents to devalue the
environment (Schultz, Zelezny, & Dalrymple, 2000; White, 1967) or that
beliefs that the environment is sacred enhance environmental concern (e.g.,
Dietz et al., 1998; Greeley, 1993; Kempton, Boster, & Hartley, 1995).
Others have linked environmental concern and behavior to general theories of
values (e.g., Schwartz, 1994) and have found that values those that focus
concern beyond a person’s immediate social circle (values called
self-transcendent or altruistic) are stronger among people who engage in
proenvironmental activities (e.g., Dietz et al., 1998; Karp, 1996; Stern &
Dietz, 1994; Stern, Dietz, Kalof, & Guagnano, 1995). A related line of research
finds greater evidence of environmental concern among individuals with “prosocial”
rather than individualistic or competitive social value orientations (e.g.,
Joireman, Lasane, Bennett, Richards, & Solaimani, in press; Van Vugt & Samuelson,
1998).
Theories
of altruistic behavior have also been used to explain environmentalism. This
approach, first articulated by Heberlein (1972), presumes that because environmental
quality is a public good, altruistic motives are a necessary for an individual
to contribute to it in a significant way. The best developed example of this
approach builds on Schwartz’s (1973, 1977) moral norm-activation theory of altruism.
The theory holds that altruistic (including proenvironmental) behavior occurs
in response to personal moral norms that are activated in individuals who believe
that particular conditions pose threats to others (awareness of adverse consequences,
or AC) and that actions they could initiate could avert those consequences
(ascription of responsibility to self, or AR). Substantial evidence supporting
the theory’s applicability to a range of environmental issues has accumulated over
two decades (e.g., Black, 1978; Black et al., 1985; Guagnano, Stern, &
Dietz, 1995; Schultz & Zelezny, 1999; Widegren, 1998).
My
colleagues and I have developed a value-belief-norm (VBN) theory of environmentalism
that builds on some of the above theoretical accounts and offers what we
believe to be the best explanatory account to date of a variety of behavioral
indicators of nonactivist environmentalism (Stern et al., 1999). The theory
links value theory, norm-activation theory, and the New Environmental Paradigm
(NEP) perspective through a causal chain of five variables leading to behavior:
personal values (especially altruistic values), NEP, AC and AR beliefs about
general conditions in the biophysical environment, and personal norms for proenvironmental
action (see Figure 1). The rationale and empirical support for this causal
ordering is drawn from previous work (Black et al., 1985; Gardner & Stern,
1996; Stern, Dietz, & Guagnano, 1995; Stern, Dietz, Kalof, & Guagnano, 1995;
Stern & Oskamp, 1987). The causal chain moves from relatively stable, central
elements of personality and belief structure to more focused beliefs about human-environment
relations (NEP), their consequences, and the individual’s responsibility for
taking corrective action. We postulate that each variable in the chain directly
affects the next and may also directly affect variables farther down the chain.
Personal norms to take proenvironmental action are activated by beliefs that
environmental conditions threaten things the individual values (AC) and that
the individual can act to reduce the threat (AR). Such norms create a general
predisposition that influences all kinds of behavior taken with proenvironmental
intent. In addition, behavior-specific personal norms and other social-psychological
factors (e.g., perceived personal costs and benefits of action, beliefs about
the efficacy of particular actions) may affect particular proenvironmental behaviors,
as discussed below.
The
VBN theory links value theory to norm-activation theory by generalizing the
latter. It postulates that the consequences that matter in activating personal norms
are adverse consequences to whatever the individual values (AC). Thus, people
who value other species highly will be concerned about environmentalconditions
that threaten those valued objects, just as altruists who care about other people
will be concerned about environmental conditions that threaten the other people’s
health or well-being. VBN theory links the NEP to norm-activation theory with
the argument that the NEP is a sort of “folk” ecological theory from which
beliefs about the adverse consequences of environmental changes can be deduced
(for empirical support, see Stern, Dietz, & Guagnano, 1995).
In
a recent study (Stern et al., 1999), my colleagues and I used the VBN theory, as
well as measures from three other theories (indicators of four cultural biases,
postmaterialist values, and belief in the sacredness of nature), to account for
three types of nonactivist environmentalism: environmental citizenship,
private-sphere behavior, and policy support (willingness to sacrifice). The VBN
cluster of variables was a far stronger predictor of each behavioral indicator
than the other theories, even when the other theories were taken in combination
(see Table 1). None of the theories, however, was very successful in predicting
the sole indicator of activism (participation in an environmental
demonstration), which appears to depend on other factors in addition to an
environmentalist predisposition.
Table
1. Explained variance in Three Indicators of Proenvironmental Behavior
|
|||
|
Dependent measures
|
||
Source
of explanatory variables
|
Private-sphere
behavior
|
Policy
support
|
Environmental
citizenship
|
VBN
theory
|
.194
|
.346
|
.302
|
Three
other theoriesa
|
.094
|
.199
|
.187
|
Added
variance from other theoriesb
|
.033
|
.033
|
.091
|
Note. From
“A Value-Belief-Norm Theory of Support for Social Movements: The Case of
Environmental Concern,” by P. C. Stern, T. Dietz, T. Abel, G. A. Guagnano,
and L. Kalof, 1999, Human Ecology Review, 6, p. 90. Copyright 1999 by Society
for Human Ecology. Reprinted with permission.
a Postmaterialist values, four cultural biases, and
beliefs about the sacredness of nature.
bDifference between R 2 value for model
combining VBN theory variables with the variables from the
other three
theories and value for model with VBN theory alone.
|
The
results provide strong initial support for the VBN theory’s contentions that
personal moral norms are the main basis for individuals’ general
predispositions to proenvironmental action (other studies supporting this
conclusion include Bratt, 1999b, and Widegren, 1998) and that these norms are
activated as the theory specifies. The personal norm variable was the only
psychological variable of the 14 in the study that is associated with all three
types of nonactivist environmentalism when the other variables are held
constant. Moreover, values, NEP, and AC beliefs accounted for 56% of the
variance in personal norms.
Data
from several studies indicate that the values most strongly implicated in activating
proenvironmental personal norms are, as norm-activation theory presumes,
altruistic or self-transcendent values (Karp, 1996; Stern, Dietz, Kalof, & Guagnano,
1995; Stern et al., 1999). However, other values are sometimes linked as well.
Self-enhancement or egoistic values and “traditional” values such as obedience,
self-discipline, and family security are negatively associated with proenvironmental
norms and action in some studies. The ways these values affect behavior are not
well understood, but they may be important bases for principled opposition by
some individuals to environmental movement goals. Another potentially important
issue, as yet unresolved empirically, is whether a set of biospheric values is
emerging, distinct from altruistic values about other people that might provide
a distinct basis for people’s support for preserving endangered species and habitats.
An
important element of the VBN theory is that the link from values to
environmentalism is mediated by particular beliefs, such as beliefs about which
kinds of people or things are affected by environmental conditions (AC) and
about whether there are individual actions that could alleviate threats to
valued persons or things (AR). Thus, environmentalist personal norms and the
predisposition to proenvironmental action can be influenced by information that
shapes thesebeliefs. This proposition suggests how environmentalism can be
affected by the findings of environmental science (about consequences),
publicity and commentary about those findings, and the actual and perceived
openness of the political system to public influence (which may affect
perceptions of personal responsibility). It also suggests an interpretation of
environmentalist and antienvironmentalistrhetoric as efforts to activate or
deactivate people’s environmental norms by highlighting certain kinds of values
or consequences (Stern, Dietz, Kalof, & Guagnano,1995). The VBN theory
offers an account of attitude formation that can deal with new or changing
attitude objects (Stern, Dietz, Kalof, & Guagnano, 1995) and, more
generally, with how environmental concern and environmental issues are socially
constructed (Dietz, Stern, & Rycroft, 1989). The VBN theory is thus
compatible with the constructed-preference tendency in cognitive psychology
(Dietz & Stern, 1995; Fischhoff, 1991; Payne, Bettman, & Johnson,
1992).
The Causes of Environmentally Significant
Behavior
Because
environmental intent and environmental impact are two different things,
theories explaining environmentalism are necessarily insufficient for understanding
how to change environmentally important behaviors. Environmentalist intent is
only one of the factors affecting behavior, and often, it is not one of the
most important. Many environmentally significant behaviors are matters of personal
habit or household routine (e.g., the setting of thermostats or the brand of paper
towels purchased) and are rarely considered at all. Others are highly
constrained by income or infrastructure (e.g., reinsulating homes, using public
transport). For others, environmental factors are only minor influences on
major actions (e.g., choosing an engine size option in a new automobile, deciding
whether to centrally air condition a home), or the environmental effects are
unknown to the consumer (e.g., choosing between products that have different
environmental impacts from their manufacturing processes). Sometimes, as with
spray cans, people may act in ways that are proenvironmental in intent but that
in fact have little or no positive environmental impact. Environmentally
beneficial actions may also follow from nonenvironmental concerns, such as a
desire to save money, confirm a sense of personal competence, or preserve time
for social relationships (De Young, this issue). And environmental concerns may
fail to lead to proenvironmental action for various reasons (Gardner &
Stern, 1996; Kempton, 1993). To understand any specific environmentally
significant behavior requires empirical analysis. The evidence suggests that
the role of environmentalist predispositions can vary greatly with the
behavior, the actor, and the context.
ABC Theory
A
first step toward understanding the complexities is to elaborate on the truism that
behavior is a function of the organism and its environment. In one formulation (Guagnano
et al., 1995), behavior (B) is an interactive product of personal-sphere attitudinal
variables (A) and contextual factors (C). The attitude-behavior association is
strongest when contextual factors are neutral and approaches zero when contextual
forces are strongly positive or negative, effectively compelling or prohibiting
the behavior in question (an inverted U-shaped function). We found supportive
evidence for this formulation in a study of curbside recycling (Guagnano et
al., 1995).
This
“ABC theory” formulation implies that for personal behaviors that are not
strongly favored by context (e.g., by being required or tangibly rewarded), the
more difficult, time-consuming, or expensive the behavior, the weaker its
dependence on attitudinal factors. Supporting evidence for this implication
exists in studies that have used the same attitudinal variables to account for
different proenvironmental behaviors. For example, in a study of household
energy conservation, the relative explanatory power of social-psychological
variables declined as effort or cost increased, from 59% of the explainable
variance in self-reported home thermostat settings to 50% for minor
curtailments such as shutting off heat in unused rooms, 44% for low-cost energy
efficiency improvements such as caulking and weather-stripping, and 25% for
major investments such as adding insulation or storm windows (Black et al.,
1985). There are similar findings for public-sphere behaviors. The
social-psychological variables of the VBN theory accounted for 35% of the
variance in expressed policy support for environmentalism and 30% of the
variance in environmental citizenship behaviors but only 4% of the variance in committed
activism (Stern et al., 1999). These findings suggest a provocative hypothesis
that is worthy of further exploration, namely that the more important a behavior
is in terms of its environmental impact, the less it depends on attitudinal variables,
including environmental concern.
Four Types of Causal Variables
It
is useful to refine the personal-contextual or organism-environment distinction
and to group the causal variables into four major types. Attitudinal factors, including
norms, beliefs, and values, are one. The VBN theory provides a good theoretical
account of one such factor, the general predisposition to act with proenvironmental
intent, which can influence all behaviors an individual considers to be
environmentally important. Other attitudinal variables affect only certain environmentally
relevant behaviors. These include behavior-specific predispositions (e.g.,
specific personal moral norms in the terms of norm-activation theory, attitudes
toward acts in the terms of the theory of planned behavior) and
behaviorspecific beliefs (e.g., about the difficulty of taking certain actions
or about their consequences for self, others, or the environment). Several
social-psychological theories, including cognitive dissonance theory,
norm-activation theory, and the theory of planned behavior, have been shown to
explain variance in specific proenvironmental behaviors. This research has
demonstrated that proenvironmental behaviors can be affected by personal
commitment and the perceived personal costs and benefits of particular actions
(e.g., Katzev & Johnson, 1987) as well as by behavior-specific beliefs and
personal norms (e.g., Black et al., 1985). As already noted, environmentally
significant behavior can also be affected by nonenvironmental attitudes, such
as those about attributes of consumer products that are correlated with
environmental impact (e.g., speed, power, and luggage capacity in motor
vehicles), or about frugality, luxury, waste, or the importance of spending time
with family.
A
second major type of causal variable is external orcontextual forces. These include
interpersonal influences (e.g., persuasion, modeling); community expectations;
advertising; government regulations; other legal and institutional factors (e.g.,
contract restrictions on occupants of rental housing); monetary incentives and
costs; the physical difficulty of specific actions; capabilities and
constraints provided by technology and the built environment (e.g., building
design, availability of bicycle paths, solar energy technology); the
availability of public policies to support behavior (e.g., curbside recycling
programs); and various features of the broad social, economic, and political
context (e.g., the price of oil, the sensitivity of government to public and
interest group pressures, interest rates in financial markets). It is worth
nothing that a contextual factor may have different meanings to people with
different attitudes or beliefs. For example, the higher price of “organic”
produce may be an economic barrier to purchase for some people, whereas for
others it is a marker of a superior product.
Personal
capabilitiesare a third type of causal variable. These include the knowledge
and skills required for particular actions (e.g., the skills of a movement organizer
for activism, mechanical knowledge for energy-conserving home repairs), the
availability of time to act, and general capabilities and resources such as
literacy, money, and social status and power. Sociodemographic variables such as
age, educational attainment, race, and income may be indicators or proxies for personal
capabilities. Although these variables have very limited explanatory power for
many environmentally significant behaviors (e.g., Dietz et al., 1998), they may
be important for behaviors that depend strongly on particular capabilities. For
instance, in a recent study (Stern et al., 1999), sociodemographic variables were
found to be unrelated to consumer behavior and policy support when social-psychological
variables were held constant, but environmental citizenship was found to be
positively associated with income and with White race. The findings reflect the
fact that the efficacy of environmental citizenship depends on an individual’s
social and economic resources. Also, environmental activism, for which
attitudinal variables had very little explanatory power, was significantly associated
(negatively) with age and income.
Finally,
habit or routineis a distinct type of causal variable. Behavior change often
requires breaking old habits and becomes established by creating new ones (Dahlstrand
& Biel, 1997). Habit, in the form of standard operating procedure, is also
a key factor in environmentally significant organizational behavior.
The
evidence suggests that different types of causal variables are important, depending
on the particular behavior (Gardner & Stern, 1996; Stern, 2000). Expensive
behaviors such as reinsulating homes are likely to be strongly influenced by monetary
factors; difficult behaviors such as reducing automobile use in the suburbs are
likely to be strongly influenced by public policy supports (e.g., for alternative
transport modes); behaviors that require specialized skills are likely to be
strongly influenced by whether or not one possesses those capabilities; and so forth.
Such hypotheses, though fairly obvious, do not go without saying. They offer a
good starting point for efforts to understand particular environmentally
significant behaviors.
Different
causal variables also appear to work different ways in influencing behavior.
For example, certain attitudinal factors create a general predisposition to act,
which may be shaped into specific action largely by personal capabilities and contextual
forces. A new context may make old habits untenable and lead someone to
consider his or her attitudes and values explicitly in developing new ones (Dahlstrand
& Biel, 1997). Or financial incentives may favor behaviors that
nevertheless do not occur unless information makes individuals aware that the
incentive is available (Stern, 1999).
The
insight of the ABC formulation, that the different types of causal factors may
interact, implies that interpretations based only on main effects can be
seriously misleading. Studies that examine only attitudinal factors are likely
to find effects only inconsistently, because the effects are contingent on
capabilities and context. Similarly, studies that examine only contextual
variables, such as material incentives, social norms, or the introduction of
new technology, may find effects but fail to reveal their dependence on
individuals’ attitudes or beliefs. Singlevariable studies may demonstrate that
a particular theoretical framework has explanatory power but may not contribute
much to the comprehensive understanding of particular environmentally
significant behaviors that is needed to change them. I return to this point
later.
Toward a Synthesis
The
field now needs synthetic theories or models that incorporate variables from
more than one of the above broad classes, postulate relationships among them,
and use them to explain one or more types of environmentally significant behavior.
Researchers are beginning to propose such models (e.g., Dahlstrand & Biel,
1997; Fransson & Gärling, 1999; Gardner & Stern, 1996; Hines, Hungerford,
& Tomera, 1987; Ölander & Thøgerson, 1995; Stern & Oskamp, 1987;
Vlek, 2000). Some of the models expand on familiar theories of altruistic
behavior (e.g., Schwartz, 1977) or planned behavior (e.g., Ajzen, 1991), which
emphasize attitudinal factors almost exclusively. Because the new models also
take into account personal capabilities, context, and habits, they are more
suitable for explaining behaviors that have significant environmental impacts,
which are often strongly influenced by such nonattitudinal factors.
A
dialogue among such models is needed to move the field toward synthesis. It is
also likely to build links to other psychological theories. For example, the distinction
between attitudes and habits as causes of behavior closely parallels the distinction
in a variety of “dual-process” models (Smith & DeCoster, 2000) between
conscious and effortful behaviors and automatic or associative ones.
Dual-process models may therefore have
something to say about proenvironmental behavior.
Changing Environmentally Significant Behavior
Many
approaches toward changing individuals’ environmentally significant behavior
have been tried. Gardner and Stern (1996) reviewed the evidence on four major
types of intervention: religious and moral approaches that appeal to values and
aim to change broad worldviews and beliefs; education to change attitudes and provide
information; efforts to change the material incentive structure of behavior by
providing monetary and other types of rewards or penalties; and community management,
involving the establishment of shared rules and expectations. They found that
each of these intervention types, if carefully executed, can change behavior.
However, moral and educational approaches have generally disappointing track
records, and even incentive- and community-based approaches rarely produce much
change on their own. By far, the most effective behavior change programs
involve combinations of intervention types.
These
findings underline the limits of single-variable explanations for informing
efforts at behavior change. The behavior is determined by multiple variables, sometimes
in interaction. There is strong evidence, for example, that incentives and
information interact, with the combination sometimes being much more effective
than the sum of the two interventions (Stern, 1999). In one evaluation study, increased
financial incentives for major investments in home energy conservation were
necessary but far from sufficient for programs to be successful. Even when electric
utility companies offered to subsidize 93% of the cost of home insulation, consumer
response varied from 1% to almost 20% adoption per year, apparently depending
on how the subsidy was made known to householders (Stern et al., 1986).
Often
the nature of the interaction can be well described in terms of barriers or limiting
conditions to behavior change (Gardner & Stern, 1996). Interventions do little
or nothing until one of them removes an important barrier to change. To promote
investments in home insulation, for example, it is necessary to reduce the financial
barriers, provide accurate information on which actions would be effective, and
reduce the difficulty of getting the information and finding a reliable contractor.
Programs that did all these things were vastly more successful than programs
that did only one or two (Stern et al., 1986). Since different individuals face
different impediments to behavior change and the impediments are often
multiple, little happens until the right combination of intervention types is
found. The concept of limiting conditions also implies that particular kinds of
interventions have diminishing returns after they have fulfilled their major
function. For example, once financial incentives are large enough to
demonstrate a clear personal benefit, increasing the incentive may be far less
effective in producing behavior change than providing information through
marketing (see Stern, 1999).
Theory
has progressed to the point at which it is possible to identify useful and practical
principles for intervention (see Table 2; for a guide to the application of these
principles, see McKenzie-Mohr & Smith, 1999). Space does not permit
elaboration of all the principles here. The admonitions to combine multiple
intervention types, to understand the situation from the actor’s perspective,
to continually monitor and adjust programs, and to use participatory methods
all suggest ways to make practical progress with incomplete theory.
For
researchers who would like to advance the understanding necessary to make
behavioral approaches to environmental protection more successful, a related set
of principles applies (see Gardner & Stern, 1996, chap. 10). First,
identify target behaviors that are environmentally significant in terms of
impact. Then analyze the behaviors to identify the responsible actors and
actions. Then consider the full range of causal variables and explore their
possible relevance to the target behavior from the actor’s standpoint. By
exploring the possibilities directly with representatives of the population
whose behavior is to be changed, it is possible to find promising strategies
for intervention without trying them all out experimentally.
This
research strategy offers the best approach to developing useful theory about
specific behavioral types that have important environmental impacts. In addition
to its practical value, such small-scale theory provides the essential building
blocks for broader, inductively developed theory about environmentally
significant behavior.
Conclusions
Environmentally
significant behavior is dauntingly complex, both in its variety and in the
causal influences on it. Although a general theory lies far in the distance,
enough is known to present a framework that can increase theoretical coherence.
This framework includes typologies of environmentally significant behaviors and
of their causes (see Table 3) and a growing set of empirical propositions about
these variables. For example:
•
The VBN approach offers a good account of the causes of the general predisposition
toward proenvironmental behavior.
•
Environmentally significant behavior depends on a broad range of causal
factors, both general and behavior-specific. A general theory of environmentalism
may therefore not be very useful for changing specific behaviors.
•
Different kinds of environmentally significant behavior have different causes.
Because the important causal factors may vary greatly across behaviors and
individuals, each target behavior should be theorized separately.
Table
3.Major Types of Environmentally Significant Behaviors and Causal Variables
Influencing
These Behaviors
|
|
Causal
variables
|
Environmentally significant behaviors
|
Attitudinal
|
Environmental activism
|
General
environmentalist predisposition a
|
Nonactivist
public-sphere behaviors
|
Behavior-specific
norms and beliefs b
|
Environmental
citizenship
(e.g.,
petitioning, joining groups)
|
Nonenvironmental
attitudes
(e.g.,
about product attributes)
|
Policy
support
|
Perceived
costs and benefits of action
|
|
|
|
Personal capabilities
|
Private-sphere environmentalism
|
Literacy
Social
status
Financial
resources
Behavior-specific
knowledge and skills
|
Consumer
purchase behaviors
Maintenance
of household equipment
Changes
in equipment use, lifestyle (curtailment)
Waste
disposal behaviors
“Green
consumerism”
|
Contextual factors
|
Other
|
Material
costs and rewards
Laws
and regulations
Available
technology
Social
norms and expectations
Supportive
policies
Advertising
|
Behaviors
affecting organizational decisions
|
|
|
Habit and routine
|
|
a
The VBN
theory incorporates various attitudinal variables believed to create this
predisposition.
b
These norms
and beliefs figure prominently in applications of norm-activation theory and
the theory of planned behavior to specific proenvironmental behaviors.
|
•
The causal factors may interact. Attitudinal causes have the greatest predictive
value for behaviors that are not strongly constrained by context or personal
capabilities. For behaviors that are expensive or difficult, contextual factors
and personal capabilities are likely to account for more of the variance.
In
addition to such empirical principles, past research has yielded important insights
for research and action on environmental protection, as described above and in
Table 2. One cannot overemphasize to behavioral scientists the importance of
identifying target behaviors from an environmental perspective (in terms of
their impact), even though understanding them requires an actor-oriented
approach that focuses on their causes. It is also critical to underscore the
need to draw on insights from across the behavioral and social sciences,
because the important causal variables lie in the domains of various
disciplines and because the variables interact.
Thus,
interdisciplinary research is necessary for full understanding.
By
following these insights and elaborating on the above principles, behavioral
researchers can further advance understanding of environmentally significant individual
behavior and can provide useful input to practical programs for environmental protection.
They are also likely to make contributions to the broader project of behavioral
science.
References
Ajzen, I. (1991).
The theory of planned behavior.Organizational Decision and Human Decision
Process,50, 179–211.
Allen, J. B., &
Ferrand, J. L. (1999). Environmental locus of control, sympathy, and
proenvironmental behavior: A test of Geller’s actively caring
hypothesis.Environment and Behavior,31, 338–353.
Black, J. S.
(1978). Attitudinal, normative, and economic factors in early response to an
energy-use field experiment (Doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin,
1978).Dissertation Abstracts International,39, 436B.
Black, J. S.,
Stern, P. C., & Elworth, J. T. (1985). Personal and contextual influences
on household energy adaptations.Journal of Applied Psychology,70, 3–21.
Bratt, C. (1999a).
Consumers’ environmental behavior: Generalized, sector-based, or compensatory? Environment
and Behavior,31, 28–44.
Bratt, C. (1999b).
The impact of norms and assumed consequences on recycling behavior.Environment and
Behavior,31, 630–656.
Dahlstrand, U.,
& Biel, A. (1997). Pro-environmental habits: Propensity levels in
behavioral change. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,27, 588–601.
Dake, K. (1991).
Orienting dispositions in the perception of risk: An analysis of contemporary worldviews
and cultural biases.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,22, 61–82.
Dietz, T., &
Stern, P. C. (1995). Toward a theory of choice: Socially embedded preference
construction. Journal of Socio-Economics,24, 261–279.
Dietz, T., Stern,
P. C., & Guagnano, G. A. (1998). Social structural and social psychological
bases of environmental concern.Environment and Behavior,30, 450–471.
Dietz, T., Stern,
P. C., & Rycroft, R. W. (1989). Definitions of conflict and the
legitimation of resources: The case of environmental risk.Sociological Forum,4,
47–70.
Douglas, M., &
Wildavsky, A. (1982).Risk and culture: An essay on the selection of
technological and environmental dangers. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University
of California Press.
Fischhoff, B.
(1991). Preference elicitation: Is there anything in there?American
Psychologist, 46, 835–847.
Fransson, N., &
Gärling, T. (1999). Environmental concern: Conceptual definitions, measurement methods,
and research findings.Journal of Environmental Psychology,19, 369–382.
Gardner, G. T.,
& Stern, P. C. (1996).Environmental problems and human behavior. Boston:
Allyn and Bacon.
Greeley, A. (1993).
Religion and attitudes toward the environment.Journal for the Scientific Study
of Religion,32, 19–28.
Guagnano, G. A.,
Stern, P. C., & Dietz, T. (1995). Influences on attitude-behavior
relationships: A natural experiment with curbside recycling.Environment and
Behavior,27, 699–718.
Heberlein, T. A.
(1972). The land ethic realized: Some social psychological explanations for
changing environmental attitudes.Journal of Social Issues,28(4), 79–87.
Hines, J. M.,
Hungerford, H. R., & Tomera, A. N. (1987). Analysis and synthesis of
research on responsible environmental behavior: A meta-analysis.Journal of
Environmental Education,18, 1–18.
Inglehart, R.
(1990).Culture shift in advanced industrial society. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Joireman, J. A.,
Lasane, T. P., Bennett, J., Richards, D., & Solaimani, S. (in press).
Integrating social value orientation and the consideration of future
consequences within the extended norm activation model of proenvironmental
behavior.British Journal of Social Psychology.
Kals, E.,
Schumacher, D., & Montada, L. (1999). Emotional affinity toward nature as a
motivational basis to protect nature.Environment and Behavior,31, 178–202.
Karp, D. G. (1996).
Values and their effects on pro-environmental behavior.Environment and
Behavior,28, 111–133.
Katzev, R. D.,
& Johnson, T. R. (1987).Promoting energy conservation: An analysis of
behavioral techniques. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Kempton, W. (1993).
Will public environmental concern lead to action on global warming?Annual Review
of Energy and Environment,18, 217–245.
Kempton, W.,
Boster, J. S., & Hartley, J. A. (1995).Environmental values in American
culture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
McAdam, D.,
McCarthy, J. D., & Zald, M. N. (1988). Social movements. In N. J. Smelser
(Ed.),Handbook of sociology(pp. 695–738). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
McKenzie-Mohr, D.,
& Smith, W. (1999).Fostering sustainable behavior: An introduction to
community-based social marketing. Gabriola Island, British Columbia, Canada:
New Society Publishers.
Ölander, F., &
Thøgerson, J. (1995). Understanding consumer behavior as a prerequisite for
environmental protection.Journal of Consumer Policy,18, 345–385.
Payne, J. W.,
Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1992). Behavioral decision research: A
constructive processing perspective. Annual Review of Psychology,43, 87–131.
Rosa, E. A., &
Dietz, T. (1998). Climate change and society: Speculation, construction and
scientific investigation.International Sociology,13, 421–425.
Schultz, P. W.,
& Zelezny, L. C. (1999). Values as predictors of environmental attitudes:
Evidence for consistency across cultures.Journal of Environmental
Psychology,19, 255–265.
Schultz, P. W.,
Zelezny, L. C., & Dalrymple, N. J. (2000). A multinational perspective on
the relation between Judeo-Christian religious beliefs and attitudes of
environmental concern.Environment and Behavior,32, 576–591.
Schwartz, S. H.
(1973). Normative explanations of helping behavior: A critique, proposal, and
empirical test.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,9, 349–364.
Schwartz, S. H.
(1977). Normative influences on altruism. In L. Berkowitz (ed.),Advances in
experimental social psychology(Vol. 10, pp. 221–279). New York: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S. H.
(1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human
values?Journal of Social Issues,50(4), 19–46.
Smith, E. R., &
DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology:
Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and
Social Psychology Review,4, 108–131.
Steg, L., &
Sievers, I. (2000). Cultural theory and individual perceptions of environmental
risks.Environment and Behavior,332, 250–269.
Stern, P. C.
(1997). Toward a working definition of consumption for environmental research
and policy.
In P. C. Stern, T.
Dietz, V. R. Ruttan, R. H. Socolow, & J. L. Sweeney (Eds.),Environmentally significant
consumption: Research directions(pp. 12–35). Washington, DC: National Academy Press,
1997.
Stern, P. C.
(1999). Information, incentives, and proenvironmental consumer behavior.Journal
of Consumer Policy,22, 461–478.
Stern, P. C.
(2000). Psychology, sustainability, and the science of human-environment
interactions. American Psychologist,55, 523–530.
Stern, P. C.,
Aronson, E., Darley, J. M., Hill, D. H., Hirst, E., Kempton, W., &
Wilbanks, T. J. (1986). The effectiveness of incentives for residential energy
conservation.Evaluation Review,10(2), 147–176.
Stern, P. C., &
Dietz, T. (1994). The value basis of environmental concern.Journal of Social
Issues, 50(3), 65–84.
Stern, P. C.,
Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G. A., & Kalof, L. (1999). A
value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of
environmental concern.Human Ecology Review,6, 81–97.
Stern, P. C.,
Dietz, T., & Guagnano, G. A. (1995). The new environmental paradigm in
social psychological perspective.Environment and Behavior, 27, 723–745.
Stern, P. C.,
Dietz, T., Kalof, L., & Guagnano, G. A. (1995). Values, beliefs and
proenvironmental action: Attitude formation toward emergent attitude
objects.Journal of Applied Social Psychology,25, 1611–1636.
Stern, P. C., &
Gardner, G. T. (1981a). Psychological research and energy policy.American
Psychologist 36, 329–342.
Stern, P. C., &
Gardner, G. T. (1981b). The place of behavior change in managing environmental
problems.Zeitschrift für Umweltpolitik,2, 213–239.
Stern, P. C., &
Oskamp, S. (1987). Managing scarce environmental resources. In D. Stokols &
I. Altman (Eds.),Handbook of environmental psychology(pp. 1043–1088). New York:
Wiley.
Stern, P. C.,
Young, O. R., & Druckman, D. (Eds.). (1992).Global environmental change:
Understanding the human dimensions. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Van Vugt, M., &
Samuelson, C. D. (1998). The impact of personal metering in the management of a
natural resource
crisis: A social dilemma analysis.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25,
731–745.
Vayda, A. P.
(1988). Actions and consequences as objects of explanation in human ecology. In
R. J.
Borden, J. Jacobs,
& G. L. Young (Eds.),Human ecology: Research and applications(pp. 9–18).
College Park, MD: Society for Human Ecology.
Vlek, C. (2000).
Essential psychology for environmental policy making.International Journal of Psychology,35,
153–167.
White, L., Jr.
(1967). The historical roots of our ecological crisis.Science,155, 1203–1207.
Widegren, Ö.
(1998). The new environmental paradigm and personal norms.Environment and
Behavior,30, 75–100.
Zald, M. (1992).
Looking backward to look forward: Reflections on the past and future of the
resource mobilization research program. In A. D. Morris & C. M. Mueller
(Eds.),Frontiers in social movement theory(pp. 326–348). New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
PAUL
C. STERN is Study Director of the Committee on the Human Dimensions of Global
Change at the U.S. National Research Council. He is also a Research Professor
of Sociology at George Mason University and President of the Social and Environmental
Research Institute. His current research interests include the study of
environmental values, beliefs, and behavior and the development of deliberative
approaches to environmental decision making. Recent publications include the coedited
volumes Understanding Risk: Informing Decisions in a Democratic Society(1996),Environmentally
Significant Consumption: Research Directions (1997),Making Climate Forecasts
Matter(1999), andInternational Conflict Resolution After the Cold War(2000),
all published by National Academy Press.
Komentar
Posting Komentar